Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 2 de 2
Filter
Add filters








Language
Year range
1.
Salud ment ; 33(4): 341-345, jul.-ago. 2010. ilus, graf, tab
Article in Spanish | LILACS-Express | LILACS | ID: lil-632788

ABSTRACT

In order to perform a neuropsychologycal evaluation, the clinician may use several instruments; nevertheless, most of them have been designed for use on populations with very different social and cultural backgrounds from that of Mexico. This makes the research on the standardization of methods of evaluation for Mexican population a very important task for both clinical and research settings. Normative data obtained from Mexican population is necessary because it provides the clinician that works with Mexican patients with a reference framework that allows him or her to correctly classify a particular behavior of an individual as normal or abnormal and thus make specific evaluations and cooperate in diagnostic. Researchers interested on cognitive functioning also require qualitative and quantitative equivalent instruments that may allow them to objectively evaluate the efficacy of short-time interventions as in a pre- and post-treatment experimental designs; and it is precisely for this reason that Taylor's figure was developed. Taylor's figure (TF) was originally designed as an alternative to Rey-Osterrieth's complex figure (ROCF), in order to use it in test-retest situations. Similar to ROCF, Taylor's figure has two modalities: copy and memory. The former evaluates constructional praxia, while the latter measures immediate recalling. Parallel tests, that is, different tests that evaluate the same variables, are useful because they reduce the measurement error involved in applying the task twice to the same person (i.e. learning), thus increasing the validity of follow up evaluations of cognitive functioning. <

Dentro del ámbito neuropsicológico existe una serie de instrumentos de evaluación que en su mayoría han sido diseñados en un contexto sociocultural diferente al nuestro, por lo que es importante contar con pruebas neuropsicológicas estandarizadas en sujetos mexicanos. Desde esta perspectiva, los datos de una población normativa nos permiten tener un marco de referencia para comparar y establecer diagnósticos diferenciales dentro de la práctica clínica. Asimismo, dentro del ámbito de la investigación se requieren instrumentos que en teoría sean equivalentes en cuanto a la función que evalúan para llevar un seguimiento sobre el funcionamiento cognoscitivo de una población en particular a lo largo del tiempo. La Figura de Taylor se diseñó como una alternativa a la Figura Compleja de Rey-Osterrieth para aplicarla en situaciones de test-retest. Consta de dos modalidades: una de copia que evalúa la praxia de construcción y otra de memoria inmediata. El término <

2.
Salud ment ; 31(3): 213-220, May-June 2008. ilus, tab
Article in Spanish | LILACS-Express | LILACS | ID: lil-632719

ABSTRACT

Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is a neuro-developmental disorder clinically characterized by three core symptoms: deficits in attentional processes, failure in inhibitory control -usually expressed through behavioral and cognitive impulsiveness-, and motor and verbal restlessness. Deficit in attentional resources is the main alteration in patients with this disorder. Attention can be considered as a neurocognitive state of neural preparation that precedes both perception and action. Attention focalizes consciousness in order to filter the constant flux of sensorial information, solve competence between stimuli for parallel processing and recruit and activate cerebral regions necessary to accomplish appropriate responses. Event-related potentials (ERPs) are a technique that has proven useful in the gathering of valuable information in the study of ADHD. One of the most studied ERPs is the P300 component. The most robust finding in the P300 research in ADHD is a decrease in the amplitude of the component in patients when compared to normal controls. This finding is usually interpreted as an evidence for a deficit in attention. ADHD usually presents commorbidity with several disorders; research shows that up to 87% of the children with ADHD present commorbidity with another disorder, up to 60% has either a behavioral or affective disorder commorbid with ADHD. Due to the wide range of disorders that are usually associated with this entity, it is useful in the research of commorbidity to use dimensional diagnostics, that is, a patient with ADHD may have commorbidity with an externalized disorder (EXT) (i.e. oppositionist defiant disorder); an internalized (INT) disorder (i.e. anxiety or affective disorder); or both an externalized and an internalized disorder (MIX). Commorbidity may have important implications in the electrophysiology of ADHD since no homogeneous results have been evident in the scarce research on the subject. Taking into account these considerations, the following experiment was designed in order to answer the question: Patients with the same main diagnostic, ADHD, but different commorbidities (INT and MIX) show different psychophysiological patterns of activation, as measured by ERPs to a continuous performance task? Sixteen patients diagnosed with ADHD by a specialist were recruited. Diagnosis was corroborated by a semi-structured interview, K-SADS-PL-MX, eight of them with an externalized comorbid (EXT) disorder, and eight of them with at least two comorbid disorders, one externalized an one internalized (MIX). A control group (CON) of eight normal subjects with no psychiatric diagnostic and matched by sex was also recruited. All subjects were between 13-16 years old with no history of Central Nervous System damage and normal IQ in the Weschler Intelligence Scale for Children. Brain electrical activity was recorded in the 19 derivations of the 10-20 international system while subjects executed a continuous performance task (CPT). Comparisons of behavioral data between groups showed some significant differences. A one-way ANOVA found differences between groups in the mean reaction time to the first part of the CPT and in the number of false positives in the second part. Electrophysiological data was analyzed with a non-parametrical multivariate test of permutations. When comparing responses to the frequent stimulus with responses to the infrequent, statistically significant differences were found in every subject; such differences share the topography and latency characteristics of the P3b component. When comparing the amplitude of this component between the groups, only two statistically significant differences were found. First, the EXT group presented a greater amplitude of the component elicited by the first part of the task in a latency of 425 to 445 msec in the parietal region of the medial line than the CON group. Second, also in response to the first part of the task, the amplitude of the CON group was bigger than that of the MIX group in a latency between 355 and 420 msec in the left temporal anterior region. No other comparison yielded significant results. When comparing between groups, mean reaction time to the first part of the task was the only behavioral variable that adequately distinguished control and patients. Even though controls executed significantly faster, they maintained the same efficacy as no differences were found in the number of errors or correct responses. This result is not surprising due to the fact that long reaction times usually denote inattention and so the fact that both groups of patients execute slower than the controls may be interpreted as a sign that, in spite of having different commorbidities, inattention is still a common problem in every patient of the sample. On the second part of the task, only the number of false positives showed statistically significant differences. However, in a posterior analysis of the data, it was evident that such differences were only between the EXT and the CON groups, with the EXT group presenting significantly more errors. False positives, especially on the second part of the task, are a measure of behavioral inhibition. Failure in inhibitory control is one of the three main symptoms of ADHD. However, some have proposed it as the main characteristic of the disorder. Analysis of the electrophysiological response to the first part of the task showed characteristic profiles of execution for each group. First, the P300 component was smaller in amplitude in the MIX group than in the control group and, even though differences were significant only in one derivation (T3), several other electrode sites more typically associated with the P300 component (C3, C4, P3, P4 and Pz) showed similar tendencies that did not reach statistical significance. Second, EXT patients had greater amplitude of the same P300 component in Pz than CON subjects. This result may seem to contradict most of the research on ADHD and P300. Nevertheless, considering the behavioral data, specially that no differences in correct responses were found between patients an controls, it is posible to assert that the greater amplitude of the component represents an overactive compensation in attentional circuits, necessary in the EXT group in order to execute at the same level of non-ADHD subjects. The results of this study present with information on a poorly reasearched subject: comorbidity and electrophysiology on ADHD.


La principal función afectada en el trastorno por déficit de atención con hiperactividad (TDAH) es la atención, la cual puede considerarse como un estado neurocognoscitivo cerebral de preparación que precede tanto a la percepción como a la acción. Los potenciales relacionados con eventos (PREs), una técnica útil en el estudio de la atención en esta entidad, pueden definirse como los cambios en la actividad eléctrica cerebral asociados temporalmente con la aparición de un evento, ya sea un estímulo o un proceso cognitivo. Con esta técnica es posible explorar las representaciones eléctricas de los procesos sensoriales y congnoscitivos con una alta resolución temporal. Uno de los PREs más estudiados en el TDAH es el P300, un componente positivo que ocurre en una latencia de alrededor de los 300 mseg. El hallazgo más contundente en el estudio del P300 en pacientes con TDAH es un decremento en la amplitud al compararlos con sujetos controles, lo cual suele ser interpretado como expresión de una atención deficiente. Por otro lado, el TDAH suele presentarse en comorbilidad con diferentes trastornos y siendo la presentación más infrecuente del síndrome es el TDAH <>. El presente estudio tiene como objetivo analizar si pacientes con el mismo diagnóstico principal, pero con diferente comorbilidad, presentan patrones de activación psicofisiológicos diferentes. Se evaluaron 16 pacientes diagnosticados con TDAH-M: ocho con comorbilidad EXT y ocho con comorbilidad MIX, así como ocho sujetos controles sanos. Tanto los pacientes como los controles realizaron las dos partes de una tarea de ejecución continua, mientras se registraba la actividad eléctrica cerebral en las 19 derivaciones del sistema internacional 10-20. En las medidas conductuales, las comparaciones intragrupos no arrrojaron diferencias estadísticamente significativas. Sin embargo, en las comparaciones entre grupos por medio de la prueba ANOVA de una vía sí aparecieron diferencias estadisticamente significativas (F=5.544 y p=0.012) entre los grupos en la media del tiempo de reacción en la parte 1 del CPT y en la variable errores por comisión en la parte 2 (F=3.975 y p=0.034). De las comparaciones electrofisiológicas realizadas entre grupos, sólo dos resultaron estadísticamente significativas. En primer lugar, el grupo EXT presentó mayor amplitud del componente que el grupo CON en una latencia entre 425-445 mseg en la región parietal media. En segundo lugar, el grupo CON tuvo mayor amplitud que el grupo MIX entre los 355-420 mseg en la región temporal anterior izquierda; ambos resultados se obtuvieron en la primera parte de la tarea. Ninguna otra comparación resultó significativa. Que el tiempo de reacción en la primera parte de la tarea fuera la única variable que distinguió entre pacientes y controles parece sugerir que, a pesar de que los pacientes tenían diferentes comorbilidades, la inatención sigue siendo el problema común a todos los pacientes de la muestra. La comparación de los datos electrofisiológicos entre grupos ofreció perfiles de ejecución característicos para cada subgrupo. Por un lado, el componente P300 fue de menor amplitud en los pacientes MIX que en los sujetos control (aunque sólo fue significativo en una derivación T3), lo cual concuerda con la bibliografía científica al respecto. Por otro, que los pacientes EXT presentaran mayor amplitud del componente P300 en Pz que los sujetos CON parece contrario a otros hallazgos de la bibliografía. Sin embargo, si se toma en cuenta que conductualmente tanto pacientes como controles ejecutaron al mismo nivel se puede sugerir que en los pacientes EXT el déficit de atención subyacente se compensa de manera exitosa y tal proceso se refleja en la amplitud de los PREs. Los resultados de este estudio proporcionan datos sobre cómo la comorbilidad incide en la respuesta electrofisiológica de los pacientes con TDAH.

SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL