Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 17 de 17
Filter
Add more filters










Publication year range
1.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36361038

ABSTRACT

The paper probes the meaning of wellbeing by examining whether ethnic identity is related to private and public conceptualisations of eudaimonic wellbeing. Private and public eudaimonic wellbeing are assessed as positive relations with others and social integration. Ethnic identity is a type of social identity that is meaningful in contexts of enduring inter-ethnic group contact. Fiji and Trinidad and Tobago (TT), nations with contact between two major ethnic groups for over a century, are the contexts for a preliminary exploration. Young adults (Fiji N = 38, 19-26 years old; TT N = 41, 18-25 years old) completed measures of positive relations with others (private eudaimonic wellbeing), social integration (public eudaimonic wellbeing), and ethnic identity development. Across the nations, a stronger sense of ethnic identity, or commitment to the ingroup, predicted better positive relations with others but worse social integration. Ethnic identity thus seems to be a key construct in understanding positive private, but negative public eudaimonic wellbeing among young adults in contexts of ethnic diversity. Findings are discussed by considering how implications of ethnic diversity (competitive inter-group relations, inter-group contact making ethnic group membership salient) might be related to ethnic identity development, and private and public eudaimonic wellbeing.


Subject(s)
Ethnicity , Social Identification , Young Adult , Humans , Adult , Adolescent , Fiji , Trinidad and Tobago
2.
Int J Aging Hum Dev ; 95(2): 166-186, 2022 09.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34410168

ABSTRACT

The current study explored the link between psychological well-being (PWB; self-acceptance, personal growth, and purpose in life) and affective themes, including redemption (positive endings for negative events), contamination (negative endings for positive events), and positive and negative affect (no change in affect) in the life stories of Caribbean adults ranging in age from 19 to 78 (N = 105). How often the memory narrative was rehearsed, and whether or not the theme emerged after being cued in content-coded life story low, high, and turning point scenes were also considered. Affective theme alone did not predict PWB; however, when considering age, rehearsal, and cue, redemption and positive affect predicted personal growth. More work should cue meaning-making in specific ways for different age groups in order to understand why there were no associations for middle-aged adults. Efforts should also be made to understand cross-cultural differences in life stories and PWB.


Subject(s)
Life Change Events , Self Concept , Adult , Humans , Mental Recall , Middle Aged , Narration
3.
Top Cogn Sci ; 11(4): 794-810, 2019 10.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30033676

ABSTRACT

Receiving another's autobiographical story may serve to elicit feelings of liking and empathy for the story sharer. Research has mostly examined social functions of autobiographical stories for in-person communications. The current experiment (N = 60) examined whether levels of liking, closeness, and empathy felt for a stranger (female confederate) after receiving her story depended on if (a) the story was received in-person or through instant message (IM), and (b) the story was positive or negative. Liking and having empathy for the stranger was higher in the in-person conditions compared to IM conditions. This effect was mediated by how engaged participants were with the story. Participants liked the stranger more after receiving the positive autobiographical story, but they felt more empathy toward her after the negative autobiographical story. The discussion considers parameters of the communication platform and people's perceptions of stories as explanations for the results. Limitations are considered.


Subject(s)
Emotions/physiology , Empathy/physiology , Memory, Episodic , Adolescent , Adult , Communication , Female , Humans , Interpersonal Relations , Male , Narration , Perception/physiology , Young Adult
4.
Int J Aging Hum Dev ; 85(2): 135-163, 2017 07.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27940903

ABSTRACT

This study experimentally examined the affective and social consequences of emotion regulation in men and women from young adulthood to old age. Participants were instructed to reappraise, suppress, or given no instructions while recalling a negative memory about their romantic relationship. Participants were 191 adults in a Trinidadian lifespan sample. Engaging in suppression resulted in higher relationship satisfaction, particularly for women, whereas engaging in reappraisal reduced negative affect for middle-aged versus younger adults. Reappraisal was, however, particularly consequential for young women who experienced higher levels of negative affect compared with men of the same age and older aged women. Regardless of instructions, older adults experienced higher relationship satisfaction, higher positive and lower negative affect than younger aged adults. Results are discussed considering the positivity effect for older adults, and how the current and historical climate of Trinidad influences the way women regulate their emotions.


Subject(s)
Aging/psychology , Emotions/physiology , Memory, Episodic , Personal Satisfaction , Sexual Partners/psychology , Adolescent , Adult , Affect/physiology , Age Factors , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Sex Factors , Young Adult
5.
Psychol Aging ; 31(1): 42-57, 2016 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26751005

ABSTRACT

Older adults sometimes exhibit higher levels of off-target verbosity during story recall than do young adults. This appears as the inclusion of extraneous information not directly relevant to the topic. Some production of such material has been clearly related to cognitive decline, particularly older adults' inability to inhibit production of irrelevant information. In tandem, however, research also suggests that some extraneous information is indirectly related to the topic and may reflect age differences in communicative styles. To further elucidate the social-cognitive aspect of this issue, the question of import is: What is the content of the additional information provided by participants during story recall? The present study answers this question. Grounded in the autobiographical memory and life story literatures, we introduce the construct, story asides, and a reliable content-analytic scheme for its assessment. Young and older adults (N = 129) recalled 1 of 2 types of stories: a personal autobiographical memory or an experimenter-generated fictional story. Narratives were reliably coded for story asides. As expected, older adults produced more story asides than young adults only for autobiographical stories. The discussion focuses on the role of story asides in everyday communication including the possibility that they may be a sign of communicative expertise.


Subject(s)
Aging/psychology , Memory, Episodic , Mental Recall , Narration , Speech , Adult , Age Factors , Aged , Cognition Disorders/psychology , Female , Humans , Male
6.
Memory ; 24(4): 423-36, 2016.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25738659

ABSTRACT

The study addresses cultural and person-level factors contributing to emerging adult's use of memory to serve adaptive functions. The focus is on three functions: self-continuity, social-bonding and directing-behaviour. Taiwanese (N = 85, 52 women) and American (N = 95, 51 women) emerging adults completed the Thinking about Life Experiences scale, and measures of trait personality, self-concept clarity and future time perspective. Findings show that individuals from both cultures use memory to serve these three functions, but Taiwanese individuals use memory more frequently than Americans to maintain self-continuity. Culture also interacted with person-level factors: in Taiwan, but not America, memory is more frequently used to create self-continuity in individuals high in conscientiousness. Across cultures, having lower self-concept clarity was related to greater use of memory to create self-continuity. Findings are discussed in terms of how memory serves functions in context and specific aspects of the Taiwanese and American cultural context that may predict the functional use of memory in emerging adulthood.


Subject(s)
Cross-Cultural Comparison , Memory, Episodic , Adolescent , Female , Humans , Male , Object Attachment , Self Concept , Surveys and Questionnaires , Taiwan , United States , Young Adult
7.
Int J Environ Res Public Health ; 12(9): 10861-85, 2015 Sep 02.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26404344

ABSTRACT

Sharing stories is an important social activity in everyday life. This study used fine-grained content analysis to investigate the accuracy of recall of two central story elements: the gist and detail of socially-relevant stories. Younger (M age = 28.06) and older (M age = 75.03) American men and women (N = 63) recalled fictional stories that were coded for (i) accuracy of overall gist and specific gist categories and (ii) accuracy of overall detail and specific detail categories. Findings showed no age group differences in accuracy of overall gist or detail, but differences emerged for specific categories. Older adults more accurately recalled the gist of when the event occurred whereas younger adults more accurately recalled the gist of why the event occurred. These differences were related to episodic memory ability and education. For accuracy in recalling details, there were some age differences, but gender differences were more robust. Overall, women remembered details of these social stories more accurately than men, particularly time and perceptual details. Women were also more likely to accurately remember the gist of when the event occurred. The discussion focuses on how accurate recall of socially-relevant stories is not clearly age-dependent but is related to person characteristics such as gender and episodic memory ability/education.


Subject(s)
Aging/physiology , Mental Recall/physiology , Adult , Aged , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Sex Characteristics , Young Adult
8.
Memory ; 23(1): 1-10, 2015.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25585205

ABSTRACT

This special issue of Memory brings together research from around the globe, from Japanese, Chinese and East Indian cultures, to American and European societies, to the Caribbean, to Turkey and to Australia and New Zealand, which examines how and why people, from childhood to old age, remember the personal past in daily life. This journey highlights the important role of the cultural context in shaping the functional usages of autobiographical memory. We illuminate six major contributions of cross-cultural research to a broader and deeper understanding of the functions of autobiographical memory, and call attention to the filed that memory research must "go global."


Subject(s)
Cross-Cultural Comparison , Memory, Episodic , Humans , Internationality
9.
Memory ; 23(1): 55-68, 2015.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24992649

ABSTRACT

Multiple and interacting contextual (culture, life phase) and person-specific predictors (i.e., personality, tendency to think-talk about the past) of the functions of autobiographical memory were examined using the Thinking about Life Experiences Scale. American (N = 174) and Trinidadian (N = 182) young and older adults self-reported how frequently they remembered the personal past to serve self, social and directive functions, how often they thought and talked about their past overall, and completed a measure of trait personality. Independent contextual and person-specific predictors were found for using memory to serve a social-bonding function: Americans, young adults, those higher in extraversion, lower in conscientiousness and individuals who frequently think and talk about the past more often use autobiographical memory for social bonding. Across cultures, younger adults report more frequently using memory to serve all three functions, whereas Trinidadians who think more often about the past compared with those who reflect less often are more likely to use it for self and directive functions. Findings are discussed in terms of the individual's embeddedness in cultural and life phase contexts when remembering.


Subject(s)
Aging/psychology , Cross-Cultural Comparison , Memory, Episodic , Adolescent , Adult , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Female , Humans , Male , Mental Recall , Middle Aged , Object Attachment , Personality , Personality Inventory , Social Behavior , Trinidad and Tobago , United States , Young Adult
10.
Memory ; 21(1): 44-63, 2013 Jan.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22913451

ABSTRACT

Two studies in different cultures (Study 1: USA, N=174, Study 2: Trinidad, N=167) examined whether meaning making, (i.e., both searching for meaning, and directing behaviour) is positively related to subjective well-being (SWB) by age (younger, older adults). In both studies, participants self-reported engagement in meaning making, and SWB (e.g., affect, future time perspective, psychological well-being). In Study 1, young Americans (compared to older) more frequently used their past to direct behaviour but doing so was unrelated to SWB. In older Americans, both types of meaning making were positively associated with SWB. In Study 2, Trinidadian younger adults were again more likely than older adults to engage in meaning making. Unlike in the American sample, however, directing behaviour was positively related to SWB for both young and older adults. The studies demonstrate that whether meaning making shows benefits for SWB may depend on type of meaning, age and culture. Note that although meaning making was sometimes unrelated to SWB, no detrimental relations to meaning making were found. The discussion focuses on the role of moderators in understanding when meaning making should lead to benefits versus costs to SWB.


Subject(s)
Adaptation, Psychological , Cognition , Cross-Cultural Comparison , Personal Satisfaction , Adolescent , Adult , Age Factors , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Factor Analysis, Statistical , Female , Humans , Male , Memory , Middle Aged , Self Report , Trinidad and Tobago , United States
11.
Memory ; 19(5): 470-86, 2011 Jul.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21864212

ABSTRACT

Theory suggests that autobiographical remembering serves several functions. This research builds on previous empirical efforts (Bluck, Alea, Habermas, & Rubin, 2005) with the aim of constructing a brief, valid measure of three functions of autobiographical memory. Participants (N=306) completed 28 theoretically derived items concerning the frequency with which they use autobiographical memory to serve a variety of functions. To examine convergent and discriminant validity, participants rated their tendency to think about and talk about the past, and measures of future time orientation, self-concept clarity, and trait personality. Confirmatory factor analysis of the function items resulted in a respecified model with 15 items in three factors. The newly developed Thinking about Life Experiences scale (TALE) shows good internal consistency as well as convergent validity for three subscales: Self-Continuity, Social-Bonding, and Directing-Behaviour. Analyses demonstrate factorial equivalence across age and gender groups. Potential use and limitations of the TALE are discussed.


Subject(s)
Memory, Episodic , Psychological Tests/statistics & numerical data , Self-Assessment , Adult , Age Factors , Aged , Factor Analysis, Statistical , Female , Humans , Life Change Events , Male , Models, Psychological , Object Attachment , Reproducibility of Results , Surveys and Questionnaires
12.
Memory ; 18(7): 730-42, 2010 Oct.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20721804

ABSTRACT

The current study begins the exploration of relationship-defining memories (i.e., the first time someone met their spouse) across adulthood. Men and women ranging from 20 to 85 years old (N=267; M age=47.19) completed a measure of marital satisfaction, wrote a relationship-defining memory, and answered questions about the quality of their memory (i.e., vividness, valence, emotional intensity, and rehearsal). Data were collected online. Results indicate that individuals over 70 and those younger than 30 rehearsed relationship-defining memories most often. Women in midlife also reported more vivid memories. The quality of relationship-defining memories also predicted marital satisfaction. Relationship-defining memories that were more vivid, positive, emotionally intense, and rehearsed related to higher marital satisfaction. Age and gender differences were minimal. Results are discussed in the context of the adaptive social function of autobiographical memories, such that these memories might have a role in influencing marital satisfaction across adulthood.


Subject(s)
Love , Marriage/psychology , Mental Recall , Personal Satisfaction , Adult , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Aging/psychology , Emotions/physiology , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Sex Characteristics , Young Adult
13.
Memory ; 18(2): 142-58, 2010 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19675968

ABSTRACT

Two separate studies examined the prevalence and quality of silent (infrequently recalled), socially silent (i.e., recalled but not shared), and disclosed autobiographical memories. In Study 1 young and older men and women remembered positive events. Positive memories were more likely to be disclosed than to be kept socially silent or completely silent. However, socially silent and disclosed memories did not differ in memory quality: the memories were equally vivid, significant, and emotional. Silent memories were less qualitatively rich. This pattern of results was generally replicated in Study 2 with a lifespan sample for both positive and negative memories, and with additional qualitative variables. The exception was that negative memories were kept silent more often. Age differences were minimal. Women disclosed their autobiographical memories more, but men told a greater variety of people. Results are discussed in terms of the functions that memory telling and silences might serve for individuals.


Subject(s)
Choice Behavior , Communication , Interpersonal Relations , Mental Recall , Social Behavior , Adaptation, Psychological , Adult , Age Factors , Aged , Chi-Square Distribution , Disclosure , Emotions , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Self Concept , Sex Factors , Verbal Behavior
14.
Int J Aging Hum Dev ; 69(4): 247-65, 2009.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20235467

ABSTRACT

The characteristics of positive autobiographical memory narratives were examined in younger and older adults. Narratives were content-coded for the extent to which they contained indicators of affect, sensory imagery, and cognition. Affect was additionally assessed through self-report. Young adults expressed more positive affect and less sensory imagery in their memory narratives than did older adults. Age differences in cognitive characteristics also appeared: younger adults showed greater causation-insight, and greater tentativeness in retelling their autobiographical memories. Controlling for episodic memory ability eliminated age differences in positive affect but did not affect age differences on other memory characteristics. Results are discussed in terms of the role that positive autobiographical memories play in daily emotional life across adulthood.


Subject(s)
Affect , Aging/psychology , Cognition , Mental Recall , Vocabulary , Adult , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Young Adult
15.
Community Ment Health J ; 40(4): 309-31, 2004 Aug.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15453084

ABSTRACT

Depression among older adults is a major public health concern in the U.S. Yet, time and again this condition goes undiagnosed, or attributed to other causes. Despite being treatable, few individuals older than age 65 are treated for this disorder. Using a community sample of 404 African-American and Caucasian older adults, the aim of this study was to identify the sources of racial group variance in self-reports of depressive symptoms. Descriptive and multivariate analyses reveal no racial/ethnic differences in the mean level of depressive symptoms, but differences in the correlates of self-reported depression, as well as differences in the distribution of individual indicators of depressive symptoms.


Subject(s)
Black or African American/psychology , Depression/diagnosis , Depression/ethnology , White People/psychology , Adult , Aged , Aging , Demography , Depression/epidemiology , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Residence Characteristics
16.
Exp Aging Res ; 29(4): 437-56, 2003.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-12959877

ABSTRACT

Asking other people for help is a compensatory behavior that may be useful across the life span to enhance functioning. Seventy-two older and younger men and women were either allowed to ask for help or were not allowed to ask for help while solving reasoning problems. Although the older adults answered fewer problems correctly, they did not seek additional help to compensate for their lower levels of performance. Younger adults sought more help. There were no age differences, however, in the types of help sought: indirect help (e.g., hints) was sought more often than direct help (e.g., asking for the answer). Exploratory analyses revealed that one's ability level was a better indicator than age of the utility of help-seeking. Findings are interpreted in the context of social and task-related influences on the use of help-seeking as a compensatory behavior across the life span.


Subject(s)
Aging/psychology , Problem Solving , Adolescent , Adult , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Cognition , Cooperative Behavior , Female , Humans , Male
17.
Memory ; 11(2): 165-78, 2003 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-12820829

ABSTRACT

In an effort to stimulate and guide empirical work within a functional framework, this paper provides a conceptual model of the social functions of autobiographical memory (AM) across the lifespan. The model delineates the processes and variables involved when AMs are shared to serve social functions. Components of the model include: lifespan contextual influences, the qualitative characteristics of memory (emotionality and level of detail recalled), the speaker's characteristics (age, gender, and personality), the familiarity and similarity of the listener to the speaker, the level of responsiveness during the memory-sharing process, and the nature of the social relationship in which the memory sharing occurs (valence and length of the relationship). These components are shown to influence the type of social function served and/or, the extent to which social functions are served. Directions for future empirical work to substantiate the model and hypotheses derived from the model are provided.


Subject(s)
Interpersonal Relations , Memory/physiology , Models, Psychological , Age Factors , Emotions , Humans , Personality , Sex Factors , Time Factors
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...