Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 20 de 32
Filter
Add more filters










Publication year range
1.
J Neurophysiol ; 131(6): 1311-1327, 2024 Jun 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38718414

ABSTRACT

Tinnitus is the perception of a continuous sound in the absence of an external source. Although the role of the auditory system is well investigated, there is a gap in how multisensory signals are integrated to produce a single percept in tinnitus. Here, we train participants to learn a new sensory environment by associating a cue with a target signal that varies in perceptual threshold. In the test phase, we present only the cue to see whether the person perceives an illusion of the target signal. We perform two separate experiments to observe the behavioral and electrophysiological responses to the learning and test phases in 1) healthy young adults and 2) people with continuous subjective tinnitus and matched control subjects. We observed that in both parts of the study the percentage of false alarms was negatively correlated with the 75% detection threshold. Additionally, the perception of an illusion goes together with increased evoked response potential in frontal regions of the brain. Furthermore, in patients with tinnitus, we observe no significant difference in behavioral or evoked response in the auditory paradigm, whereas patients with tinnitus were more likely to report false alarms along with increased evoked activity during the learning and test phases in the visual paradigm. This emphasizes the importance of integrity of sensory pathways in multisensory integration and how this process may be disrupted in people with tinnitus. Furthermore, the present study also presents preliminary data supporting evidence that tinnitus patients may be building stronger perceptual models, which needs future studies with a larger population to provide concrete evidence on.NEW & NOTEWORTHY Tinnitus is the continuous phantom perception of a ringing in the ears. Recently, it has been suggested that tinnitus may be a maladaptive inference of the brain to auditory anomalies, whether they are detected or undetected by an audiogram. The present study presents empirical evidence for this hypothesis by inducing an illusion in a sensory domain that is damaged (auditory) and one that is intact (visual). It also presents novel information about how people with tinnitus process multisensory stimuli in the audio-visual domain.


Subject(s)
Auditory Perception , Bayes Theorem , Illusions , Tinnitus , Humans , Tinnitus/physiopathology , Pilot Projects , Male , Female , Adult , Auditory Perception/physiology , Illusions/physiology , Visual Perception/physiology , Young Adult , Electroencephalography , Acoustic Stimulation , Cues
2.
Psychol Rev ; 129(4): 732-741, 2022 07.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34968134

ABSTRACT

Early direction-selective neurons in the primary visual cortex are widely considered to be the main neural basis underlying motion perception even though motion perception can also rely on attentively tracking the position of objects. Because of their small receptive fields, early direction-selective neurons suffer from the aperture problem, which is assumed to be overcome by integrating inputs from many early direction-selective neurons. Because the perceived motion of objects sometimes depends on static form information and does not always match the mean direction of local motion signals, the general consensus is that motion integration is form dependent and complex. Based on the fact that early direction-selective neurons respond to motion only within a short temporal window, the present study used stroboscopic motion to test their contribution to motion perception of objects. For conditions under which the perceived motion was impaired by stroboscopic motion, the perceived motion matched the mean direction of local motion signals and was form independent. For classic conditions under which the perceived motion could not be explained by a simple form independent averaging of local motion signals, neutralizing the contribution of early direction-selective neurons using stroboscopic motion had little impact on the perceived motion, which demonstrates that the perceived motion relied on position tracking, not on early direction-selective neurons. When the perceived motion relies on position tracking, assuming that motion perception relies on early direction-selective neurons can lead to erroneously postulate the existence of complex or form-dependent integration of inputs from early direction-selective neurons. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).


Subject(s)
Motion Perception , Visual Cortex , Humans , Motion Perception/physiology , Neurons/physiology , Photic Stimulation , Visual Cortex/physiology
3.
Front Neurol ; 12: 596615, 2021.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34899549

ABSTRACT

Motor control deficits outlasting self-reported symptoms are often reported following mild traumatic brain injury (mTBI). The exact duration and nature of these deficits remains unknown. The current study aimed to compare postural responses to static or dynamic virtual visual inputs and during standard clinical tests of balance in 38 children between 9 and 18 years-of-age, at 2 weeks, 3 and 12 months post-concussion. Body sway amplitude (BSA) and postural instability (vRMS) were measured in a 3D virtual reality (VR) tunnel (i.e., optic flow) moving in the antero-posterior direction in different conditions. Measures derived from standard clinical balance evaluations (BOT-2, Timed tasks) and post-concussion symptoms (PCSS-R) were also assessed. Results were compared to those of 38 healthy non-injured children following a similar testing schedule and matched according to age, gender, and premorbid level of physical activity. Results highlighted greater postural response with BSA and vRMS measures at 3 months post-mTBI, but not at 12 months when compared to controls, whereas no differences were observed in post-concussion symptoms between mTBI and controls at 3 and 12 months. These deficits were specifically identified using measures of postural response in reaction to 3D dynamic visual inputs in the VR paradigm, while items from the BOT-2 and the 3 timed tasks did not reveal deficits at any of the test sessions. PCSS-R scores correlated between sessions and with the most challenging condition of the BOT-2 and as well as with the timed tasks, but not with BSA and vRMS. Scores obtained in the most challenging conditions of clinical balance tests also correlated weakly with BSA and vRMS measures in the dynamic conditions. These preliminary findings suggest that using 3D dynamic visual inputs such as optic flow in a controlled VR environment could help detect subtle postural impairments and inspire the development of clinical tools to guide rehabilitation and return to play recommendations.

4.
Front Aging Neurosci ; 13: 744444, 2021.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34955808

ABSTRACT

Age-related decline in visual perception is usually attributed to optical factors of the eye and neural factors. However, the detection of light by cones converting light into neural signals is a crucial intermediate processing step of vision. Interestingly, a novel functional approach can evaluate many aspects of the visual system including the detection of photons by cones. This approach was used to investigate the underlying cause of age-related visual decline and found that the detection rate of cones was considerably affected with healthy aging. This functional test enabling to evaluate the detection of photons by cones could be particularly useful to screen for retinal pathologies affecting cones such as age-related macular degeneration. However, the paradigm used to functionally measure the detection of photons was complex as it was evaluating many other properties of the visual system. The aim of the current mini review is to clarify the underlying rationale of functionally evaluating the detection of photons by cones, describe a simpler approach to evaluate it, and review the impact of aging on the detection rate of cones.

5.
Atten Percept Psychophys ; 83(3): 1094-1105, 2021 Apr.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33506351

ABSTRACT

Ensemble statistics of a visual scene can be estimated to provide a gist of the scene without detailed analysis of all individual items. The simplest and most widely studied ensemble statistic is mean estimation, which requires averaging an ensemble of elements. Averaging is useful to estimate the mean of an ensemble and discard the variance. The source of variance can be external (i.e., variance across the physical elements) or internal (i.e., imprecisions in the estimates of the elements by the visual system). The equivalent noise paradigm is often used to measure the impact of the internal variance (i.e., the equivalent input noise). This paradigm relies on the assumption that the averaging process is equally effective independently of the main source of variance, internal or external, so any difference between the processing when the main source of variance is internal or external must be assumed not to affect the averaging efficiency. The current fMRI study compared the neural activity when the main variance is caused by the stimulus (i.e., high variance) and when it is caused by imprecisions in the estimates of the elements by the visual system (i.e., low variance). The results showed that the right superior frontal and left middle frontal gyri can be significantly more activated when the variance in the orientation of the Gabors was high than when it was low. Consequently, the use of the equivalent noise paradigm requires the assumption that such additional neural activity in high variance does not affect the averaging efficiency.


Subject(s)
Noise , Orientation , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging
6.
Sci Rep ; 10(1): 16521, 2020 10 05.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33020552

ABSTRACT

Motion perception is affected by healthy aging, which impairs the ability of older adults to perform some daily activities such as driving. The current study investigated the underlying causes of age-related motion contrast sensitivity losses by using an equivalent noise paradigm to decompose motion contrast sensitivity into calculation efficiency, the temporal modulation transfer function (i.e., temporal blur) and 3 sources of internal noise: stochastic absorption of photons by photoreceptors (i.e., photon noise), neural noise occurring at the retinal level (i.e., early noise) and at the cortical level (i.e., late noise). These sources of internal noise can be disentangled because there impacts on motion contrast sensitivity vary differently as a function of luminance intensity. The impact of healthy aging on these factors was evaluated by measuring motion contrast sensitivity of young and older healthy adults at different luminance intensities, temporal frequencies and with/without external noise. The older adults were found to have higher photon noise, which suggests a lower photon absorption rate of cones. When roughly equating the amount of photons being absorbed by the photoreceptors, older adults had lower calculation efficiencies, but no significant aging effect was found on temporal modulation transfer function, early noise and late noise.


Subject(s)
Aging/physiology , Contrast Sensitivity/physiology , Motion Perception/physiology , Adult , Age Factors , Aged , Artifacts , Female , Humans , Male , Motion , Photic Stimulation/methods , Sensory Thresholds/physiology , Vision, Ocular/physiology
7.
Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci ; 60(2): 544-551, 2019 02 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30716150

ABSTRACT

Purpose: Vision decline with healthy aging is a major public health concern with the unceasing growth of the aged population. In order to prevent or remedy the age-related visual loss, a better understanding of the underlying causes is needed. The current psychophysical study used a novel noise paradigm to investigate the causes of age-related contrast sensitivity loss by estimating the impact of optical factors, absorption rate of photon by photoreceptors, neural noise, and calculation efficiency on contrast sensitivity. Methods: The impact of these factors on contrast sensitivity was assessed by measuring contrast thresholds with and without external noise over a wide range of spatial frequencies (0.5-16 cycles per degree [cyc/deg]) and different luminance intensities for 20 young (mean = 26.5 years, SD = 3.79) and 20 older (mean = 75.9 years, SD = 4.30) adults, all having a good visual acuity (≥6/7.5). Results: The age-related contrast sensitivity losses were explained by older observers absorbing considerably fewer photons (4×), having more neural noise (1.9×), and a lower processing efficiency (1.4×). The aging effect on optical factors was not significant. Conclusions: The age-related contrast sensitivity loss was mostly due to less efficient cones absorbing four times fewer photons than young adults. Thus, besides the ocular factors known to be considerably affected with aging, the decline of absorption efficiency of cones is also responsible for a considerable age-related visual decline, especially under dim light.


Subject(s)
Healthy Aging/physiology , Photons , Retinal Cone Photoreceptor Cells/physiology , Vision, Ocular/radiation effects , Adult , Aged , Contrast Sensitivity/physiology , Female , Humans , Male , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Psychophysics , Sensory Thresholds , Young Adult
8.
Sci Rep ; 8(1): 2596, 2018 02 07.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29416068

ABSTRACT

Contrast sensitivity varies substantially as a function of spatial frequency and luminance intensity. The variation as a function of luminance intensity is well known and characterized by three laws that can be attributed to the impact of three internal noise sources: early spontaneous neural activity limiting contrast sensitivity at low luminance intensities (i.e. early noise responsible for the linear law), probabilistic photon absorption at intermediate luminance intensities (i.e. photon noise responsible for de Vries-Rose law) and late spontaneous neural activity at high luminance intensities (i.e. late noise responsible for Weber's law). The aim of this study was to characterize how the impact of these three internal noise sources vary with spatial frequency and determine which one is limiting contrast sensitivity as a function of luminance intensity and spatial frequency. To estimate the impact of the different internal noise sources, the current study used an external noise paradigm to factorize contrast sensitivity into equivalent input noise and calculation efficiency over a wide range of luminance intensities and spatial frequencies. The impact of early and late noise was found to drop linearly with spatial frequency, whereas the impact of photon noise rose with spatial frequency due to ocular factors.

9.
Sci Rep ; 7: 43140, 2017 02 21.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28220883

ABSTRACT

Visual perception generally improves under brighter environments. For instance, motion sensitivity is known to improve with luminance intensity especially at high temporal frequencies. However, the current study counter-intuitively shows that increasing luminance intensity can impair motion sensitivity in noise. Motion sensitivity was measured with and without noise added to a drifting Gabor patch as a function of the temporal frequency and luminance intensity. As expected, motion sensitivity in absence of noise reached a ceiling performance at a relatively low luminance intensity (about 35 td) for low temporal frequencies and improved with luminance intensity up to the highest luminance intensity tested (353 td) for high temporal frequencies. In noise, reducing mean luminance intensity facilitated motion sensitivity (up to a factor of about 1.7) for temporal frequencies up to 7.5 Hz and impaired sensitivity at higher temporal frequencies (15 and 30 Hz). We conclude that reducing luminance intensity is effectively equivalent to applying a low-pass filter, which can improve motion sensitivity in noise to low and middle temporal frequencies. This counterintuitive facilitation effect can be explained by two known properties of the visual system: decreasing luminance intensity impairs the visibility of high temporal frequencies (equivalent to a low-pass filter) and motion detectors are broadly tuned.

10.
J Vis ; 17(2): 5, 2017 02 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28196375

ABSTRACT

External noise paradigms are widely used to characterize sensitivity by comparing the effect of a variable on contrast threshold when it is limited by internal versus external noise. A basic assumption of external noise paradigms is that the processing properties are the same in low and high noise. However, recent studies (e.g., Allard & Cavanagh, 2011; Allard & Faubert, 2014b) suggest that this assumption could be violated when using spatiotemporally localized noise (i.e., appearing simultaneously and at the same location as the target) but not when using spatiotemporally extended noise (i.e., continuously displayed, full-screen, dynamic noise). These previous findings may have been specific to the crowding and 0D noise paradigms that were used, so the purpose of the current study is to test if this violation of noise-invariant processing also occurs in a standard contrast detection task in white noise. The rationale of the current study is that local external noise triggers the use of recognition rather than detection and that a recognition process should be more affected by uncertainty about the shape of the target than one involving detection. To investigate the contribution of target knowledge on contrast detection, the effect of orientation uncertainty was evaluated for a contrast detection task in the absence of noise and in the presence of spatiotemporally localized or extended noise. A larger orientation uncertainty effect was observed with temporally localized noise than with temporally extended noise or with no external noise, indicating a change in the nature of the processing for temporally localized noise. We conclude that the use of temporally localized noise in external noise paradigms risks triggering a shift in process, invalidating the noise-invariant processing required for the paradigm. If, instead, temporally extended external noise is used to match the properties of internal noise, no such processing change occurs.


Subject(s)
Contrast Sensitivity/physiology , Noise , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Adult , Female , Humans , Male , Orientation , Psychophysics , Sensory Thresholds/physiology , Uncertainty , Young Adult
11.
J Vis ; 17(1): 17, 2017 01 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28114492

ABSTRACT

The photopic motion sensitivity function of the energy-based motion system is band-pass peaking around 8 Hz. Using an external noise paradigm to factorize the sensitivity into equivalent input noise and calculation efficiency, the present study investigated if the variation in photopic motion sensitivity as a function of the temporal frequency is due to a variation of equivalent input noise (e.g., early temporal filtering) or calculation efficiency (ability to select and integrate motion). For various temporal frequencies, contrast thresholds for a direction discrimination task were measured in presence and absence of noise. Up to 15 Hz, the sensitivity variation was mainly due to a variation of equivalent input noise and little variation in calculation efficiency was observed. The sensitivity fall-off at very high temporal frequencies (from 15 to 30 Hz) was due to a combination of a drop of calculation efficiency and a rise of equivalent input noise. A control experiment in which an artificial temporal integration was applied to the stimulus showed that an early temporal filter (generally assumed to affect equivalent input noise, not calculation efficiency) could impair both the calculation efficiency and equivalent input noise at very high temporal frequencies. We conclude that at the photopic luminance intensity tested, the variation of motion sensitivity as a function of the temporal frequency was mainly due to early temporal filtering, not to the ability to select and integrate motion. More specifically, we conclude that photopic motion sensitivity at high temporal frequencies is limited by internal noise occurring after the transduction process (i.e., neural noise), not by quantal noise resulting from the probabilistic absorption of photons by the photoreceptors as previously suggested.


Subject(s)
Color Vision/physiology , Contrast Sensitivity/physiology , Motion Perception/physiology , Motion , Sensory Thresholds/physiology , Humans , Noise
12.
Behav Res Methods ; 49(4): 1278-1290, 2017 08.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27604602

ABSTRACT

Noise-masking experiments are widely used to investigate visual functions. To be useful, noise generally needs to be strong enough to noticeably impair performance, but under some conditions, noise does not impair performance even when its contrast approaches the maximal displayable limit of 100 %. To extend the usefulness of noise-masking paradigms over a wider range of conditions, the present study developed a noise with great masking strength. There are two typical ways of increasing masking strength without exceeding the limited contrast range: use binary noise instead of Gaussian noise or filter out frequencies that are not relevant to the task (i.e., which can be removed without affecting performance). The present study combined these two approaches to further increase masking strength. We show that binarizing the noise after the filtering process substantially increases the energy at frequencies within the pass-band of the filter given equated total contrast ranges. A validation experiment showed that similar performances were obtained using binarized-filtered noise and filtered noise (given equated noise energy at the frequencies within the pass-band) suggesting that the binarization operation, which substantially reduced the contrast range, had no significant impact on performance. We conclude that binarized-filtered noise (and more generally, truncated-filtered noise) can substantially increase the energy of the noise at frequencies within the pass-band. Thus, given a limited contrast range, binarized-filtered noise can display higher energy levels than Gaussian noise and thereby widen the range of conditions over which noise-masking paradigms can be useful.


Subject(s)
Noise , Perceptual Masking , Humans
13.
Front Hum Neurosci ; 10: 81, 2016.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27014018

ABSTRACT

In the furrow illusion (Anstis, 2012), the perceived path of a moving target follows the veridical path orientation when viewed foveally, but follows the orientation of the texture when viewed peripherally. These radically different motion percepts depending on whether the stimulus is viewed foveally or peripherally has led Anstis to conclude that the furrow illusion reveals "profound differences in the way that the periphery and fovea process visual motion." In the current study, we rather argue that the different percepts can be explained by reduced position acuity with eccentricity and therefore do not imply different ways of processing motion per se. If feature tracking, which is position-based, is involved in the perception of the veridical motion direction, then impairing the feature tracking motion system should strengthen the illusion. To reduce contribution of the feature tracking motion system, we used a crowding paradigm consisting in presenting many nearby targets. We found that under crowding conditions, the furrow illusion was stronger. We conclude that feature tracking was involved in the perception of the veridical motion direction, which is compatible with the hypothesis that the different motion percepts at fixation and in the periphery are due to a reduced position acuity with eccentricity affecting feature tracking, not to different ways of processing motion per se.

14.
Front Psychol ; 6: 1707, 2015.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26635647
15.
J Vis ; 14(8): 2, 2014 Jul 03.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24993016

ABSTRACT

To conclude that there is a dedicated color motion system, the hypothesis that the luminance motion pathway is processing color motion due to some nonlinearity must be rejected. Many types of nonlinearities have been considered. Cavanagh and Anstis (1991) considered interunit variability in equiluminance, but they found that adding a color-defined modulation to a luminance-defined drifting modulation increased the contribution to motion. This color contribution to motion cannot be due to interunit variability in equiluminance alone because such a luminance artifact would increase the effective luminance contrast for some luminance-sensitive units and decrease it for the others, resulting in no additional contribution to motion on average. Cavanagh and Anstis considered this color contribution to motion as evidence of a dedicated color motion system, but here we show that such a color contribution to motion varies with the phase difference between the luminance and color modulations, which would not be expected if luminance- and color-defined motion were processed separately. Specifically, the contribution to motion was greater when the luminance and color modulations were aligned (i.e., 0 degrees or 180 degrees phase difference), than when they were not (90 degrees or 270 degrees phase difference). Such a luminance-color phase interaction was also observed when spatially interleaving luminance and color information, which suggests that the interaction occurs after some spatial integration (i.e., not at the photoreceptors). To our knowledge, this luminance-color phase interaction cannot be explained by any previously considered nonlinearity. However, it can be explained by an expansive nonlinearity occurring before the summation of the L- and M-cone pathways (i.e., before ganglion cells) and after some spatial integration (i.e., after the photoreceptors). We conclude that there is a nonlinearity that has not been considered before, enabling some color motion processing by the luminance motion system.


Subject(s)
Color Perception/physiology , Motion Perception/physiology , Retinal Cone Photoreceptor Cells/physiology , Humans , Light , Retinal Ganglion Cells/physiology
16.
Front Psychol ; 5: 749, 2014.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25071693

ABSTRACT

Adding noise to a stimulus is useful to characterize visual processing. To avoid triggering a processing strategy shift between the processing in low and high noise, Allard and Cavanagh (2011) recommended using noise that is extended as a function of all dimensions such as space, time, frequency and orientation. Contrariwise, to avoid cross-channel suppression affecting contrast detection, Baker and Meese (2012) suggested using noise that is localized as a function of all dimensions, namely "0D noise," which basically consists in randomly jittering the target contrast (and, for blank intervals or catch trials, jittering the contrast of an identical zero-contrast signal). Here we argue that contrast thresholds in extended noise are not contaminated by noise-induced cross-channel suppression because contrast gains affect signal and noise by the same proportion leaving the signal-to-noise ratio intact. We also review empirical findings showing that detecting a target in 0D noise involves a different processing strategy than detecting in absence of noise or in extended noise. Given that internal noise is extended as a function of all dimensions, we therefore recommend using external noise that is also extended as a function of all dimensions when assuming that the same processing strategy operates in low and high noise.

17.
Front Psychol ; 5: 426, 2014.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24860538

ABSTRACT

Contrast thresholds for discriminating orientation and direction of a drifting, oriented grating are usually similar to contrast detection thresholds, which suggest that the most sensitive detectors are labeled for both orientation and direction (Watson and Robson, 1981). This was found to be true in noiseless condition, but Arena et al. (2013) recently found that this was not true in localized noise (i.e., noise having the same spatiotemporal window as the target) as thresholds for discriminating direction were higher than for discriminating orientation. They suggested that this could be explained by the fact that there are more neurons selective to orientation than direction. Another possible interpretation is that, unlike contrast thresholds in absence of noise, the most sensitive detectors in localized noise were labeled for orientation, but not for direction. This hypothesis is supported by recent findings showing different processes operating in localized and extended noise (i.e., full-screen, continuously displayed noise, Allard and Cavanagh, 2011). In the current study, we evaluated contrast thresholds for orientation and direction discrimination tasks in noiseless conditions, and in noise that was either spatially localized or extended, and temporally localized or extended. We found similar orientation and direction thresholds in absence of noise and in temporally extended noise, but greater direction thresholds in temporally localized noise. This suggests that in noiseless and temporally extended noise the most sensitive detectors were labeled for both orientation and direction (e.g., direction-selective complex cells), whereas in temporally localized noise the most sensitive detectors were labeled for orientation but not direction (e.g., simple cells). We conclude that to avoid violating the noise-invariant processing assumption, external noise paradigms investigating motion processing should use noise that is temporally extended, not localized.

18.
Vision Res ; 92: 47-52, 2013 Nov.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24070688

ABSTRACT

At least three studies have used external noise paradigms to investigate the cause of contrast sensitivity losses due to healthy aging. These studies have used noise that was spatiotemporally localized on the target. Yet, Allard and Cavanagh (2011) have recently shown that the processing strategy can change with localized noise thereby violating the noise-invariant processing assumption and compromising the application of external noise paradigms. The present study reassessed the cause of age-related contrast sensitivity losses using spatiotemporally extended external noise (i.e., full-screen, continuously displayed dynamic noise). Contrast thresholds were measured for young (mean=24 years) and older adults (mean=69 years) at 3 spatial frequencies (1, 3 and 9 cpd) and 3 noise conditions (noise-free, local noise and extended noise). At the two highest spatial frequencies, the results were similar with local and extended noise: the sensitivity loss was mainly due to lower calculation efficiency. At the lowest spatial frequency, age-related contrast sensitivity losses were attributed to the internal equivalent noise when using extended noise and, like in previous studies, due to calculation efficiency with local noise. These results show that the interpretation of external noise paradigms can drastically differ depending on the noise type suggesting that external nose paradigms should use external noise that is spatiotemporally extended like internal noise to avoid triggering a processing strategy change. Contrary to previous studies, we conclude that healthy aging does not affect the calculation efficiency of the detection process at low spatial frequencies.


Subject(s)
Aging/physiology , Contrast Sensitivity/physiology , Adult , Aged , Analysis of Variance , Discrimination, Psychological/physiology , Female , Humans , Male , Noise , Photic Stimulation/methods , Psychophysics , Sensory Thresholds/physiology , Space Perception/physiology , Young Adult
20.
J Vis ; 13(11)2013 Sep 06.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24013864

ABSTRACT

The existence of a second-order motion system distinct from both the first-order and feature tracking motion systems remains controversial even though many consider it well established. In the present study, the texture contribution to motion was measured within and beyond the spatial acuity of attention by presenting the stimuli in the near periphery where the spatial resolution of attention is low. The logic was that when moving elements are too close one to another for attention to individually select them (i.e., crowding), it is not possible to track them. To test the existence of a dedicated second-order motion system, the texture contribution to motion was measured when neutralizing both the feature tracking motion system and the contribution of the first-order motion system due to preprocessing nonlinearities introducing residual distortion products. When the contribution of distortion products was not neutralized, texture substantially contributed to motion for spatial frequencies within and beyond the spatial acuity of attention. When neutralizing the contribution of distortion products, texture substantially contributed to motion for spatial frequencies within the spatial acuity of attention, but not for spatial frequencies beyond the spatial acuity of attention. We conclude that there is no dedicated second-order motion system; the texture contribution to motion is mediated solely by the first-order (due to residual distortion products) and feature tracking (at frequencies within spatiotemporal acuity of attention) motion systems.


Subject(s)
Contrast Sensitivity/physiology , Light , Motion Perception/physiology , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Humans , Psychophysics
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...