Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 7 de 7
Filter
1.
Appetite ; 85: 30-5, 2015 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25450899

ABSTRACT

Food neophobia and disgust are commonly thought to be linked, but this hypothesis is typically implicitly assumed rather than directly tested. Evidence for the connection has been based on conceptually and empirically unsound measures of disgust, unpublished research, and indirect findings. This study (N = 283) provides the first direct evidence of a relationship between trait-level food neophobia and trait-level pathogen disgust. Unexpectedly, we also found that food neophobia varies as a function of sexual disgust and is linked to mating strategy. Using an evolutionary framework, we propose a novel hypothesis that may account for these previously undiscovered findings: the food neophilia as mating display hypothesis. Our discussion centers on future research directions for discriminatively testing this novel hypothesis.


Subject(s)
Choice Behavior , Food Preferences/psychology , Sexual Behavior/psychology , Adolescent , Adult , Emotions , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Young Adult
3.
Hum Nat ; 24(4): 430-43, 2013 Dec.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24105261

ABSTRACT

Food sharing may be used for mate attraction, sexual access, or mate retention in humans, as in many other species. Adult humans tend to perceive more intimacy in a couple if feeding is observed, but the increased perceived intimacy may be due to resource provisioning rather than feeding per se. To address this issue, 210 university students (66 male) watched five short videos, each showing a different mixed-sex pair of adults dining together and including feeding or simple provisioning or no food sharing. A survey concerning attraction and intimacy in the dyad was completed after each video. Both provisioning and feeding produced higher ratings of "Involvement," with feeding producing the highest ratings. Similarly, the perceived attraction of each actor to the other was lowest when no food sharing was shown and highest when feeding was displayed. These findings are consistent with a view of feeding as a courtship display in humans.


Subject(s)
Courtship/psychology , Feeding Behavior/psychology , Interpersonal Relations , Perception , Adolescent , Adult , Female , Food , Humans , Male , Young Adult
4.
Appetite ; 58(3): 1041-5, 2012 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22387714

ABSTRACT

It was expected that viewers watching adult mixed-sex pairs dining together will give higher ratings of the perceived intimacy and involvement of the pair if feeding is displayed while eating, especially if the feeding involves contaminated (i.e., with potential germ transfer) foods. Our hypotheses were tested using a design in which participants viewed five videotapes in varying order. Each video showed different mixed-sex pairs of actors sharing meal and included a distinct form of food sharing or none. These were shown to 50 small groups of young adults in quasi-random sequences to control for order effects. Immediately after each video, viewers were asked about the attractiveness, attraction and intimacy in the dyad they had just observed. As predicted, videos featuring contaminated feeding consistently produced higher ratings on involvement and attraction than those showing uncontaminated feeding which, in turn, mostly produced higher ratings on involvement and attraction than those showing no feeding behaviors.


Subject(s)
Feeding Behavior , Food Contamination , Interpersonal Relations , Social Behavior , Social Desirability , Social Perception , Adolescent , Adult , Female , Humans , Male , Sexual Partners , Videotape Recording , Young Adult
5.
Percept Mot Skills ; 102(3): 683-90, 2006 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16916147

ABSTRACT

Motion sickness is believed to be caused by conflicting sensory signals, a situation that mimics the effects of ingesting certain toxins. Thus, one might suspect that individuals who have experienced a relatively high frequency of motion sickness may be particularly vigilant about avoiding anything that produces nausea, induding potentially nauseating toxins. Consequently, they may be more resistant to trying new foods, i.e., be more food neophobic, since unfamiliar foods can have unexpected adverse effects due to toxins or allergens. Likewise, many highly stimulating experiences can trigger motion sickness, so individuals who are more susceptible may be more prone to avoid such experiences, i.e., be less sensation seeking. Finally, it was expected that food neophobia would be more frequent in individuals low on sensation seeking tendencies. Self-reported motion sickness history in 308 adults (M= 18.8 yr.; SD = 1.6) was correlated with scores on the Arnett Inventory of Sensation Seeking and the Food Neophobia Scale. As predicted, greater history of motion sickness was associated with lower Sensation Seeking scores. Food Neophobia was not correlated with motion sickness history but, as expected, was negatively correlated (r = -.42) with scores on Sensation Seeking. Further research is recommended that measures actual sensitivity to motion sickness.


Subject(s)
Exploratory Behavior , Food , Motion Sickness/epidemiology , Phobic Disorders/epidemiology , Psychological Theory , Sensation , Adolescent , Adult , Comorbidity , Female , Humans , Male , Mass Screening/methods , Motion Sickness/etiology , Motion Sickness/physiopathology , Phobic Disorders/diagnosis , Phobic Disorders/etiology , Prevalence , Surveys and Questionnaires
6.
Adolescence ; 40(158): 273-80, 2005.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16114591

ABSTRACT

This study examined gender stereotypes in peer ratings of femininity and masculinity for adolescent participants in three sports. Following a preliminary study of gender stereotyping of several sports, high school students rated unfamiliar cohorts each of whom was described in a single paragraph as either a male or female dedicated participant in one of three sports. A total of 12 different descriptive paragraphs were used in a 2 (race) x 2 (sex) x 3 (sport) design. Each of these paragraphs, although short, ascribed a variety of traits that could be seen by raters as the independent variables: name (initials only), age, race, gender, hours of practice per week, number of competitions/performances per year, sport, and self-confidence. For this reason, raters were highly unlikely to surmise that sex and sport were the primary independent variables in the study. As predicted, there was a consistent decrease in rated femininity and increase in masculinity for both male and female adolescent targets as they switched from participating in a "feminine" (ballet) to a neutral (tennis) to a "masculine" (karate) sport. These results suggest that sex stereotypes for certain sports may influence who elects to participate and how participants are viewed by others.


Subject(s)
Attitude , Peer Group , Sexuality/psychology , Social Perception , Sports/physiology , Stereotyping , Adolescent , Adolescent Behavior/psychology , Female , Health Surveys , Humans , Male , Psychology, Adolescent , Schools , Social Identification , South Carolina
7.
J Gen Psychol ; 118(3): 201-214, 1991 Jul.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28142506

ABSTRACT

The present study investigated potential sex differences in preferences for spicy, hot or unusual foods and in food aversions or neophobia. This study also reexamined the issue of a gender difference in preference for sweet foods. Questionnaires concerning past and current food use and preferences as well as food and condiment use in one actual meal were completed by 148 people between 17 and 32 years of age. Their responses clearly support the prediction that men tend to have a stronger preference than women for spicy, hot foods. The results also support the prediction that men are more likely than women to seek unusual and new foods. Both sexes showed the same degree of preference for condiment use.

SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...