Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 11 de 11
Filter
Add more filters










Publication year range
1.
Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci ; 16(1-2): 117-128, 2021 01 18.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33025001

ABSTRACT

Social neuroscience research has demonstrated that those who are like-minded are also 'like-brained.' Studies have shown that people who share similar viewpoints have greater neural synchrony with one another, and less synchrony with people who 'see things differently.' Although these effects have been demonstrated at the 'group level,' little work has been done to predict the viewpoints of specific 'individuals' using neural synchrony measures. Furthermore, the studies that have made predictions using synchrony-based classification at the individual level used expensive and immobile neuroimaging equipment (e.g. functional magnetic resonance imaging) in highly controlled laboratory settings, which may not generalize to real-world contexts. Thus, this study uses a simple synchrony-based classification method, which we refer to as the 'neural reference groups' approach, to predict individuals' dispositional attitudes from data collected in a mobile 'pop-up neuroscience' lab. Using functional near-infrared spectroscopy data, we predicted individuals' partisan stances on a sociopolitical issue by comparing their neural timecourses to data from two partisan neural reference groups. We found that partisan stance could be identified at above-chance levels using data from dorsomedial prefrontal cortex. These results indicate that the neural reference groups approach can be used to investigate naturally occurring, dispositional differences anywhere in the world.


Subject(s)
Attitude , Brain/diagnostic imaging , Emotions , Neuroimaging/methods , Humans , Male , Prefrontal Cortex/diagnostic imaging , Spectroscopy, Near-Infrared/methods
2.
Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci ; 13(6): 628-636, 2018 06 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29733408

ABSTRACT

Activity in medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) during persuasive messages predicts future message-consistent behavior change, but there are significant limitations to the types of persuasion processes that can be invoked inside an MRI scanner. For instance, real world persuasion often involves multiple people in conversation. Functional near infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) allows us to move out of the scanner and into more ecologically valid contexts. As a first step, the current study used fNIRS to replicate an existing fMRI persuasion paradigm (i.e. the sunscreen paradigm) to determine if mPFC shows similar predictive value with this technology. Consistent with prior fMRI work, activity in mPFC was significantly associated with message-consistent behavior change, above and beyond self-reported intentions. There was also a difference in this association between previous users and non-users of sunscreen. Activity differences based on messages characteristics were not observed. Finally, activity in a region of right dorsolateral PFC (dlPFC), which has been observed with counterarguing against persuasive messages, correlated negatively with future behavior. The current results suggest it is reasonable to use fNIRS to examine persuasion paradigms that go beyond what is possible in the MRI scanner environment.


Subject(s)
Persuasive Communication , Sunscreening Agents , Adult , Brain Mapping , Female , Health Behavior , Humans , Male , Prefrontal Cortex/diagnostic imaging , Prefrontal Cortex/physiology , Spectroscopy, Near-Infrared , Young Adult
3.
Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci ; 12(2): 283-297, 2017 02 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27521303

ABSTRACT

Designing persuasive content is challenging, in part because people can be poor predictors of their actions. Medial prefrontal cortex (MPFC) activation during message exposure reliably predicts downstream behavior, but past work has been largely atheoretical. We replicated past results on this relationship and tested two additional framing effects known to alter message receptivity. First, we examined gain- vs. loss-framed reasons for a health behavior (sunscreen use). Consistent with predictions from prospect theory, we observed greater MPFC activity to gain- vs. loss-framed messages, and this activity was associated with behavior. This relationship was stronger for those who were not previously sunscreen users. Second, building on theories of action planning, we compared neural activity during messages regarding how vs. why to enact the behavior. We observed rostral inferior parietal lobule and posterior inferior frontal gyrus activity during action planning ("how" messages), and this activity was associated with behavior; this is in contrast to the relationship between MPFC activity during the "why" (i.e., gain and loss) messages and behavior. These results reinforce that persuasion occurs in part via self-value integration-seeing value and incorporating persuasive messages into one's self-concept-and extend this work to demonstrate how message framing and action planning may influence this process.


Subject(s)
Health Behavior/physiology , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Motivation/physiology , Persuasive Communication , Prefrontal Cortex/physiology , Skin Neoplasms/prevention & control , Skin Neoplasms/psychology , Sunscreening Agents/administration & dosage , Adolescent , Cooperative Behavior , Female , Humans , Intention , Male , Self Concept , Young Adult
4.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 112(12): 3599-605, 2015 Mar 24.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25733850

ABSTRACT

Existing moral psychology research commonly explains certain phenomena in terms of a motivation to blame. However, this motivation is not measured directly, but rather is inferred from other measures, such as participants' judgments of an agent's blameworthiness. The present paper introduces new methods for assessing this theoretically important motivation, using tools drawn from animal-model research. We test these methods in the context of recent "harm-magnification" research, which shows that people often overestimate the damage caused by intentional (versus unintentional) harms. A preliminary experiment exemplifies this work and also rules out an alternative explanation for earlier harm-magnification results. Exp. 1 asks whether intended harm motivates blame or merely demonstrates the actor's intrinsic blameworthiness. Consistent with a motivational interpretation, participants freely chose blaming, condemning, and punishing over other appealing tasks in an intentional-harm condition, compared with an unintentional-harm condition. Exp. 2 also measures motivation but with converging indicators of persistence (effort, rate, and duration) in blaming. In addition to their methodological contribution, these studies also illuminate people's motivational responses to intentional harms. Perceived intent emerges as catalyzing a motivated social cognitive process related to social prediction and control.


Subject(s)
Intention , Motivation , Perception , Psychology/methods , Social Behavior , Adult , Animals , Cognition , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Models, Animal , Models, Statistical , Public Policy , Young Adult
5.
J Cogn Neurosci ; 27(4): 655-64, 2015 Apr.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25244122

ABSTRACT

Effective real-world communication requires the alignment of multiple individuals to a common perspective or mental framework. To study how this alignment occurs at the level of the brain, we measured BOLD response during fMRI while participants (n = 24) listened to a series of vignettes either in the presence or absence of a valid contextual cue. The valid contextual cue was necessary to understand the information in each vignette. We then examined where and to what extent the shared valid context led to greater intersubject similarity of neural processing. Regions of the default mode network including posterior cingulate cortex and medial pFC became more aligned when participants shared a valid contextual framework, whereas other regions, including primary sensory cortices, responded to the stimuli reliably regardless of contextual factors. Taken in conjunction with previous research, the present results suggest that default mode regions help the brain to organize incoming verbal information in the context of previous knowledge.


Subject(s)
Brain Mapping , Brain/physiology , Cognition/physiology , Nerve Net/physiology , Neural Pathways/physiology , Acoustic Stimulation , Adolescent , Adult , Brain/blood supply , Comprehension , Cues , Female , Humans , Image Processing, Computer-Assisted , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Male , Models, Neurological , Neural Pathways/blood supply , Oxygen/blood , Reaction Time/physiology , Young Adult
6.
Psychol Sci ; 24(9): 1755-62, 2013 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23878021

ABSTRACT

People and societies seek to combat harmful events. However, because resources are limited, every wrong righted leaves another wrong left unchecked. Responses must therefore be calibrated to the magnitude of the harm. One underappreciated factor that affects this calibration may be people's oversensitivity to intent. Across a series of studies, people saw intended harms as worse than unintended harms, even though the two harms were identical. This harm-magnification effect occurred for both subjective and monetary estimates of harm, and it remained when participants were given incentives to be accurate. The effect was fully mediated by blame motivation. People may therefore focus on intentional harms to the neglect of unintentional (but equally damaging) harms.


Subject(s)
Intention , Judgment/physiology , Morals , Social Behavior , Female , Humans , Male , Motivation/physiology
7.
Neuroimage ; 83: 599-608, 2013 Dec.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23850465

ABSTRACT

How do people maintain consistent impressions of other people when other people are often inconsistent? The present research addresses this question by combining recent neuroscientific insights with ecologically meaningful behavioral methods. Participants formed impressions of real people whom they met in a personally involving situation. fMRI and supporting behavioral data revealed that outcome dependency (i.e., depending on another person for a desired outcome) alters previously identified neural dynamics of impression formation. Consistent with past research, a functional localizer identified a region of dorsomedial PFC previously linked to social impression formation. In the main task, this ROI revealed the predicted patterns of activity across outcome dependency conditions: greater BOLD response when information confirmed (vs. violated) social expectations if participants were outcome-independent, and the reverse pattern if participants were outcome-dependent. We suggest that, although social perceivers often discount expectancy-disconfirming information as noise, being dependent on another person for a desired outcome focuses impression-formation processing on the most diagnostic information, rather than on the most tractable information.


Subject(s)
Brain Mapping , Brain/physiology , Social Behavior , Social Perception , Female , Humans , Image Processing, Computer-Assisted , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Male , Young Adult
8.
J Cogn Neurosci ; 23(4): 857-66, 2011 Apr.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20350058

ABSTRACT

People often make shortsighted decisions to receive small benefits in the present rather than large benefits in the future, that is, to favor their current selves over their future selves. In two studies using fMRI, we demonstrated that people make such decisions in part because they fail to engage in the same degree of self-referential processing when thinking about their future selves. When participants predicted how much they would enjoy an event in the future, they showed less activity in brain regions associated with introspective self-reference--such as the ventromedial pFC (vMPFC)--than when they predicted how much they would enjoy events in the present. Moreover, the magnitude of vMPFC reduction predicted the extent to which participants made shortsighted monetary decisions several weeks later. In light of recent findings that the vMPFC contributes to the ability to simulate future events from a first-person perspective, these data suggest that shortsighted decisions result in part from a failure to fully imagine the subjective experience of one's future self.


Subject(s)
Affect/physiology , Choice Behavior/physiology , Prefrontal Cortex/physiology , Brain Mapping , Female , Humans , Image Processing, Computer-Assisted , Magnetic Resonance Imaging/methods , Male , Neuropsychological Tests , Oxygen/blood , Predictive Value of Tests , Prefrontal Cortex/blood supply , Young Adult
9.
Asian J Soc Psychol ; 13(2): 72-82, 2010 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23874143

ABSTRACT

Cultural neuroscience issues from the apparently incompatible combination of neuroscience and cultural psychology. A brief literature sampling suggests, instead, several preliminary topics that demonstrate proof of possibilities: cultural differences in both lower-level processes (e.g. perception, number representation) and higher-order processes (e.g. inferring others' emotions, contemplating the self) are beginning to shed new light on both culture and cognition. Candidates for future cultural neuroscience research include cultural variations in the default (resting) network, which may be social; regulation and inhibition of feelings, thoughts, and actions; prejudice and dehumanization; and neural signatures of fundamental warmth and competence judgments.

10.
J Cogn Neurosci ; 21(3): 594-604, 2009 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18564038

ABSTRACT

Recent research has focused on the disparate mechanisms that support the human ability to "mentalize" about the thoughts and feelings of others. One such process may rely on precompiled, semantic beliefs about the characteristics common to members of a social group, that is, on stereotypes; for example, judging that a woman may be more likely than a man to have certain interests or opinions. In the current study, we identified a pattern of neural activity associated with the use of stereotypes to judge another person's psychological characteristics. During fMRI scanning, participants mentalized about the likely responses of a female and male target to a series of questions, some of which were related to gender stereotypes (e.g., "enjoys shopping for new clothes"). Trials on which participants applied a stereotype were segregated from those on which participants avoided stereotype use. The BOLD response in an extensive region of the right frontal cortex differentiated stereotype-applied from -unapplied trials. Moreover, this neural difference was correlated with a behavioral index of gender associations-the Implicit Association Test-administered after scanning. Results suggest that stereotype application may draw on cognitive processes that more generally subserve semantic knowledge about categories.


Subject(s)
Brain Mapping , Cerebral Cortex/physiology , Concept Formation/physiology , Judgment/physiology , Stereotyping , Adolescent , Cerebral Cortex/blood supply , Female , Humans , Image Processing, Computer-Assisted/methods , Magnetic Resonance Imaging/methods , Male , Oxygen/blood , Reaction Time/physiology , Sex Factors , Young Adult
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...