Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 20 de 22
Filter
Add more filters










Publication year range
1.
Pain Res Manag ; 2016: 7134825, 2016.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27445624

ABSTRACT

Background. Through real-time behavioral observation systems, pain behaviors are commonly used by clinicians to estimate pain intensity in patients with low back pain. However, little is known about how clinicians rely on pain-related behaviors to make their judgment. According to the Information Integration Theory (IIT) framework, this study aimed at investigating how clinicians value and integrate information from lumbopelvic kinematics (LK), a protective pain behavior, and facial expression intensity (FEI), a communicative pain behavior, to estimate pain in patients with chronic low back pain (cLBP). Methods. Twenty-one experienced clinicians and twenty-one novice clinicians were asked to estimate back pain intensity from a virtual character performing a trunk flexion-extension task. Results. Results revealed that both populations relied on facial expression and that only half of the participants in each group integrated FEI and LK to estimate cLBP intensity. Among participants who integrated the two pain behaviors, averaging rule predominated among others. Results showed that experienced clinicians relied equally on FEI and LK to estimate pain, whereas novice clinicians mostly relied on FEI. Discussion. The use of additive rule of integration does not appear to be systematic when assessing others' pain. When assessing pain intensity, communicative and protective pain behaviors may have different relevance.


Subject(s)
Biomechanical Phenomena/physiology , Facial Expression , Judgment , Low Back Pain/diagnosis , Low Back Pain/psychology , Adult , Analysis of Variance , Female , Humans , Lumbosacral Region/physiology , Male , Middle Aged , Pain Perception/physiology , Pelvis , Posture , Visual Analog Scale , Young Adult
3.
Braz J Biol ; 75(2): 331-8, 2015 May.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26132015

ABSTRACT

The role of greenhouse gas emissions from freshwater reservoirs and their contribution to increase greenhouse gas concentrations in the atmosphere is currently under discussion in many parts of the world. We studied CO2 and CH4 diffusive fluxes from two large neotropical hydropower reservoirs with different climate conditions. We used floating closed-chambers to estimate diffusive fluxes of these gaseous species. Sampling campaigns showed that the reservoirs studied were sources of greenhouse gases to the atmosphere. In the Serra da Mesa Reservoir, the CH4 emissions ranged from 0.530 to 396.96 mg.m(-2).d(-1) and CO2 emissions ranged from -1,738.33 to 11,166.61 mg.m(-2).d(-1) and in Três Marias Reservoir the CH4 fluxes ranged 0.720 to 2,578.03 mg.m(-2).d(-1) and CO2 emission ranged from -3,037.80 to 11,516.64 to mg.m(-2).d(-1). There were no statistically significant differences of CH4 fluxes between the reservoirs, but CO2 fluxes from the two reservoirs studied were significantly different. The CO2 emissions measured over the periods studied in Serra da Mesa showed some seasonality with distinctions between the wet and dry transition season. In Três Marias Reservoir the CO2 fluxes showed no seasonal variability. In both reservoirs, CH4 emissions showed a tendency to increase during the study periods but this was not statistically significant. These results contributed to increase knowledge about the magnitude of CO2 and CH4 emission in hydroelectric reservoirs, however due to natural variability of the data future sampling campaigns will be needed to better elucidate the seasonal influences on the fluxes of greenhouse gases.


Subject(s)
Carbon Dioxide/analysis , Environmental Monitoring , Fresh Water , Methane/analysis , Brazil , Greenhouse Effect , Seasons
4.
Eur J Pain ; 18(1): 110-9, 2014 Jan.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23821536

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: Being able to estimate effort pain in patients is important for health care providers working in physical rehabilitation services. Previous studies have shown that clinicians and physiotherapists underestimate patients' pain. METHODS: The present study examined how two sources of visual information, namely body kinematics (movement speed and postural constraints) and facial expressions are integrated in order to estimate effort pain magnitude experienced by a paraplegic person performing a sitting pivot transfer. In addition, the effect of familiarity with paraplegia on judgment was assessed by comparing performance among physiotherapists, paraplegic patients and unfamiliar participants. Functional measurement was used to determine the psychophysical law of visual information integration carried by pain behaviours (guarding and facial expression). RESULTS: Results indicate that guarding behaviour (specified by movement speed) carried important information for perceived effort pain independently of familiarity. In contrast, facial expression of pain was relevant only to unfamiliar and physiotherapist participants and not to paraplegic participants. Even if physiotherapists underestimated effort pain as compared to the other groups, they relied more strongly on facial expression, than other participants, in their estimation of effort pain expressed by a paraplegic patient. CONCLUSIONS: These results bring further insights into understanding physiotherapists' perception of patients' pain. In order to improve their ability to adapt the difficulty of the rehabilitation sessions, they should learn to raise their global level of pain magnitude estimation (for example, by performing themselves the body movement) instead of overweighting facial pain expression signals.


Subject(s)
Pain Perception/physiology , Pain/diagnosis , Pain/psychology , Adult , Analysis of Variance , Behavior , Biomechanical Phenomena , Facial Expression , Female , Humans , Male , Paraplegia/psychology , Photic Stimulation , Physical Exertion , Physical Therapists , Posture/physiology , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Psychophysics , Young Adult
5.
Brain ; 131(Pt 2): 523-34, 2008 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18178570

ABSTRACT

This study assesses the role of the human medial temporal lobe (MTL) structures in the coordination of spatial information across perspective change and, in particular, in visual perspective taking--namely the capacity to know what another individual is seeing on the visual scene. Fourteen patients with unilateral temporal lobe resection and 21 control subjects performed two tasks, called 'object location memory' and 'viewpoint recognition', respectively. In the object location memory task, subjects had to memorize the position of a target object in the environment from an initial viewpoint. They were then shown the same environment from a new viewpoint and had to indicate whether or not the target object had moved. In the viewpoint recognition task, subjects had to imagine the perspective of an avatar from the initial viewpoint and then decide whether or not the new viewpoint was that of the avatar. The results showed a double dissociation, with left MTL patients being impaired in the object location memory task but not in the viewpoint recognition task and right MTL patients being impaired in the viewpoint recognition task but not in the object location memory task. Furthermore, based on multiple regression analyses between performance and the volumes of the different MTL structures, we discuss the specific involvement of the left temporopolar cortex and of the right hippocampus in different kinds of visual perspective taking.


Subject(s)
Perceptual Disorders/pathology , Space Perception , Temporal Lobe/pathology , Adult , Attention , Epilepsy, Temporal Lobe/surgery , Female , Functional Laterality , Hippocampus/pathology , Hippocampus/physiopathology , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging/methods , Male , Memory , Middle Aged , Motion Perception , Neuropsychological Tests , Perceptual Disorders/physiopathology , Perceptual Disorders/psychology , Photic Stimulation/methods , Recognition, Psychology , Temporal Lobe/physiopathology , Temporal Lobe/surgery
7.
Neuroimage ; 36(3): 955-68, 2007 Jul 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17493835

ABSTRACT

We used functional magnetic resonance imaging to explore the brain mechanisms of changing point of view (PoV) in a visuospatial memory task in 3D space. Eye movements were monitored and BOLD signal changes were measured while subjects were presented with 3D images of a virtual environment. Subjects were required to encode the position of a lamp in the environment and, after changing the PoV (angular difference varied from 0 degrees to 180 degrees in 45 degrees steps), to decide whether the lamp position had been changed too or not. Performance data and a scan-path analysis based on eye movement support the use of landmarks in the environment for coding lamp position and increasing spatial updating costs with increasing changes of PoV indicating allocentric coding strategies during all conditions (0 degrees - to 180 degrees -condition). Subtraction analysis using SPM revealed that a parieto-temporo-frontal network including left medial temporal areas was activated during this 3D visuospatial task, independent of angular difference. The activity of the left parahippocampal area and the left lingual gyrus (but not the hippocampus) correlated with increasing changes of the PoV between encoding and retrieval, emphasizing their specific role in spatial scene memory and allocentric coding. The results suggest that these areas are involved in a continuous matching process between internal representations of the environment and the external status quo. In addition, hippocampal activation correlated with performance was found indicating successful recall of spatial information. Finally, in a prefrontal area comprising, the so-called "deep" frontal eye field, activation was correlated with the amount of saccadic eye movements confirming its role in oculomotor processes.


Subject(s)
Memory, Short-Term/physiology , Space Perception/physiology , Visual Perception/physiology , Adult , Brain Mapping , Cerebral Cortex/physiology , Computer Graphics , Data Interpretation, Statistical , Eye Movements/physiology , Female , Humans , Male , Nerve Net/physiology , Oxygen/blood , Photic Stimulation , Saccades/physiology , Visual Fields/physiology
8.
Osteoporos Int ; 18(5): 703-9, 2007 May.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17506127

ABSTRACT

INTRODUCTION: In view of the increase in the life expectancy of humans and in edentulism of the population above 50 years of age, in which the prevalence of osteoporosis is also higher, it is fundamental to better understand the effects of systemic bone mass loss on the healing process of dental implants and to determine the quality of the bone that surrounds them. The objective of the present study was to compare systemic osteoporosis (axial and femoral) and parameters of mandibular bone quality, and to evaluate osseointegration in postmenopausal women receiving dental implants. METHODS: The sample consisted of 39 women aged 48-70 years, 19 with a densitometric diagnosis of osteoporosis in the lumbar spine and femoral neck and 20 controls with a normal densitometric diagnosis. Bone mineral density was measured in the patients and controls by dual-energy X-ray absorptiometry. Eighty-two osseointegrated dental implants were placed in the mandible, 39 of them in the osteoporosis group and 43 in the control group. Mandibular bone quality was evaluated by classifying mandibular inferior cortical and trabecular bone on panoramic radiographs and by histomorphometric analysis of a mandibular bone biopsy. Osseointegration was analyzed after 9 months. RESULTS: No significant difference was observed between patients with osteoporosis and controls when comparing individuals with a normal cortex and those with a severely or moderately eroded cortex determined on panoramic radiographs, although patients with MEC/SEC had lower femoral neck BMD than those with NC (0.688 +/- 0.17 vs. 0.814+/- 0.144 g/cm2, P<0.012). Histomorphometric analysis also revealed no difference in the parameters of bone formation or resorption between the two groups. Implant failure was observed in only one case. CONCLUSION: We conclude that there is an association between low femoral neck BMD and poor mandibular bone quality as assessed by panoramic radiography. The loss of one implant (1.2%) is compatible with the literature and cannot be attributed to systemic osteoporosis.


Subject(s)
Bone Density/physiology , Dental Implants , Mandible/physiopathology , Osteoporosis/physiopathology , Aged , Female , Femur Neck/physiopathology , Humans , Lumbar Vertebrae/physiopathology , Mandible/diagnostic imaging , Middle Aged , Osseointegration/physiology , Osteoporosis/diagnostic imaging , Prosthesis Failure , Radiography
9.
J Sci Med Sport ; 10(5): 280-7, 2007 Oct.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17188932

ABSTRACT

It is difficult to interpret the training induced changes in middle-distance running, since numerous aerobic and anaerobic determinants of the performance are interdependent. Several aerobic and anaerobic tests are available but their results, particularly those from anaerobic tests, may be discordant, not providing univocal interpretation of training. The purpose of this study is to use a multidimensional approach to distinguish aerobic and anaerobic capacities assessed by two running tests on a track: the maximal anaerobic running test (MART) and V(O2max) tests. Eleven runners carried out two maximal tests on a synthetic track before and after a 4-week training period: (i) a maximal test to determine V(O2max), the velocity associated with V(O2max) (vV(O2max)) and the velocity at the lactate threshold (v(LT)), (ii) a maximal anaerobic running test to estimate anaerobic capacity. An all-out test run at v(LT)+50% of the difference between v(LT) and vV(O2max), known to be affected by both aerobic and anaerobic energy production, was used to test this approach. A principal components analysis (PCA) shows that two components (i.e., aerobic and anaerobic) explained 79% of the variation in the physiological variables. The PCA suggests that V(O2max) and MART tests assess the aerobic and the anaerobic capacities, respectively. In contrast, the performance in the all-out test is affected by both aerobic and anaerobic energy production. The PCA shows that v(LT) and DeltaP (difference between the maximal power of the MART and V(O2max)) are clear markers of the long-term endurance and the anaerobic capacity, respectively. This multidimensional approach can be a useful way to disentangle the aerobic and anaerobic components of track tests.


Subject(s)
Anaerobic Threshold/physiology , Energy Metabolism/physiology , Exercise Test/methods , Physical Exertion/physiology , Running/physiology , Adult , Female , Humans , Lactic Acid/metabolism , Male , Principal Component Analysis
10.
Osteoporos Int ; 17(10): 1494-500, 2006 Oct.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16799754

ABSTRACT

INTRODUCTION: In view of the increase in the life expectancy of humans and in edentulism of the population above 50 years of age, in which the prevalence of osteoporosis is also higher, it is fundamental to better understand the effects of systemic bone mass loss on the healing process of dental implants and to determine the quality of the bone that surrounds them. The objective of the present study was to compare systemic osteoporosis (axial and femoral) and parameters of mandibular bone quality, and to evaluate osseointegration in postmenopausal women receiving dental implants. METHODS: The sample consisted of 39 women aged 48-70 years, 19 with a densitometric diagnosis of osteoporosis in the lumbar spine and femoral neck and 20 controls with a normal densitometric diagnosis. Bone mineral density was measured in the patients and controls by dual-energy X-ray absorptiometry. Eighty-two osseointegrated dental implants were placed in the mandible, 39 of them in the osteoporosis group and 43 in the control group. Mandibular bone quality was evaluated by classifying mandibular inferior cortical and trabecular bone on panoramic radiographs and by histomorphometric analysis of a mandibular bone biopsy. Osseointegration was analyzed after 9 months. RESULTS: No significant difference was observed between patients with osteoporosis and controls when comparing individuals with a normal cortex and those with a severely or moderately eroded cortex determined on panoramic radiographs. Histomorphometric analysis also revealed no difference in the parameters of bone formation or resorption between the two groups. Implant failure was observed in only one case. CONCLUSION: We conclude that there is no association between systemic osteoporosis (axial and femur) and parameters of poor mandibular bone quality. The loss of one implant (1.2%) is compatible with the literature and cannot be attributed to systemic osteoporosis.


Subject(s)
Bone Density , Dental Implants , Mandible/physiopathology , Osteoporosis, Postmenopausal/physiopathology , Aged , Anthropometry , Dental Restoration Failure , Female , Femur Neck/physiopathology , Humans , Lumbar Vertebrae/physiopathology , Mandible/diagnostic imaging , Middle Aged , Osseointegration , Radiography, Panoramic , Risk Factors
11.
Exp Brain Res ; 145(4): 489-97, 2002 Aug.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-12172660

ABSTRACT

While we walk through the environment, we constantly receive inputs from different sensory systems. For us to accomplish a given task, for example to reach a target location, the sensory information has to be integrated to update our knowledge of self-position and self-orientation with respect to the target so that we can correctly plan and perform the remaining trajectory. As has been shown previously, vestibular information plays a minor role in the performance of linear goal-directed locomotion when walking blindfolded toward a previously seen target within a few meters. The present study extends the question of whether vestibular information is a requirement for goal-directed locomotion by studying a more complex task that also involves rotation: walking a triangular path. Furthermore, studying this task provides information about how we walk a given trajectory, how we move around corners, and whether we are able to return to the starting point. Seven young male, five labyrinthine-defective (LD) and five age- and gender-matched control subjects were asked to walk a previously seen triangular path, which was marked on the ground, first without vision (EC) and then with vision (EO). Each subject performed three clockwise (CW) and three counterclockwise (CCW) walks under the EC condition and one CW and CCW walk under the EO condition. The movement of the subjects was recorded by means of a 3D motion analysis system. Analysis of the data showed that LD subjects had, in the EC condition, a significantly larger final arrival error, which was due to increased directional errors during the turns. However, there was no difference between the groups as regards the overall path length walked. This shows that LD subjects were able to plan and execute the given trajectory without vision, but failed to turn correctly around the corners. Hence, the results demonstrate that vestibular information enhances the ability to perform a planned trajectory incorporating whole body rotations when no visual feedback is available.


Subject(s)
Feedback/physiology , Locomotion/physiology , Orientation/physiology , Postural Balance/physiology , Space Perception/physiology , Vestibular Diseases/physiopathology , Vestibule, Labyrinth/physiopathology , Adolescent , Adult , Aged , Blindness/physiopathology , Central Nervous System/physiology , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Rotation , Sensory Deprivation/physiology
12.
Brain Res Cogn Brain Res ; 12(1): 167-70, 2001 Aug.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11489620

ABSTRACT

We investigated the spatiotemporal cortical dynamics during the perception of object-motion and visually-induced self-motion perception in six normal subjects, using a 143-channel neuromagnetometer. Object-motion specific tasks evoked early transient activity over the right temporooccipital cortex, while self-motion perception, or vection, additionally was followed by sustained bilateral activity in the temporoparietal area. The specific signal distributions suggest to represent the different perceptual modes of object-motion and self-motion sensation.


Subject(s)
Cerebral Cortex/physiology , Motion Perception/physiology , Visual Perception/physiology , Adult , Head Movements/physiology , Humans , Magnetoencephalography , Male , Occipital Lobe/physiology , Temporal Lobe/physiology
13.
J Biol Inorg Chem ; 5(4): 527-34, 2000 Aug.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10968624

ABSTRACT

The consecutive structural genes for the iron-sulfur flavoenzyme sulfide dehydrogenase, sudB and sudA, have been identified in the genome of Pyrococcus furiosus. The translated sequences encode a heterodimeric protein with an alpha-subunit, SudA, of 52598 Da and a beta-subunit, SudB, of 30686 Da. The alpha-subunit carries a FAD, a putative nucleotide binding site for NADPH, and a [2Fe-2S]2+,+ prosthetic group. The latter exhibit EPR g-values, 2.035, 1.908, 1.786, and reduction potential, Em,8 = +80 mV, reminiscent of Rieske-type clusters; however, comparative sequence analysis indicates that this cluster is coordinated by a novel motif of one Asp and three Cys ligands. The motif is not only found in the genome of hyperthermophilic archaea and hyperthermophilic bacteria, but also in that of mesophilic Treponema pallidum. The beta-subunit of sulfide dehydrogenase contains another FAD, another putative binding site for NADPH, a [3Fe-4S]+,0 cluster, and a [4Fe-4S]2+,+ cluster. The 3Fe cluster has an unusually high reduction potential, Em,8 = +230 mV. The reduced 4Fe cluster exhibits a complex EPR signal, presumably resulting from magnetic interaction of its S = 1/2 spin with the S=2 spin of the reduced 3Fe cluster. The 4Fe cluster can be reduced with deazaflavin/EDTA/light but not with sodium dithionite; however, it is readily reduced with NADPH. SudA is highly homologous to KOD1-GO-GAT (or KOD1-GltA), a single-gene encoded protein in Pyrococcus kodakaraensis, which has been putatively identified as hyperthermophilic glutamate synthase. However, P. furiosus sulfide dehydrogenase does not have glutamate synthase activity. SudB is highly homologous to HydG, the gamma-subunit of P. furiosus NiFe hydrogenase. The latter enzyme also has sulfide dehydrogenase activity. The P. furiosus genome contains a second set of consecutive genes, sudY and sudX, with very high homology to the sudB and sudA genes, and possibly encoding a sulfide dehydrogenase isoenzyme. Each subunit of sulfide dehydrogenase is a primary structural paradigm for a different class of iron-sulfur flavoproteins.


Subject(s)
Archaeal Proteins/chemistry , Cytochrome c Group/chemistry , Iron-Sulfur Proteins/chemistry , Oxidoreductases/chemistry , Pyrococcus furiosus/enzymology , Amino Acid Motifs , Amino Acid Sequence , Archaeal Proteins/genetics , Cytochrome c Group/genetics , Cytochrome c Group/metabolism , Electron Spin Resonance Spectroscopy , Iron-Sulfur Proteins/genetics , Ligands , Molecular Sequence Data , Oxidation-Reduction , Oxidoreductases/genetics , Oxidoreductases/metabolism , Protein Structure, Quaternary , Pyrococcus furiosus/genetics , Sequence Alignment
14.
J Cogn Neurosci ; 12(4): 569-82, 2000 Jul.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10936911

ABSTRACT

Under appropriate conditions, an observer's memory for the final position of an abruptly halted moving object is distorted in the direction of the represented motion. This phenomenon is called "representational momentum" (RM). We examined the effect of mental imagery instructions on the modulation of spatial orientation processing by testing for RM under conditions of picture versus body rotation perception and imagination. Behavioral data were gathered via classical reaction time and error measurements, whereas brain activity was recorded with the help of magnetoencephalography (MEG). Due to the so-called inverse problem and to signal complexity, results were described at the signal level rather than with the source location modeling. Brain magnetic field strength and spatial distribution, as well as latency of P200m evoked fields were used as neurocognitive markers. A task was devised where a subject examined a rotating sea horizon as seen from a virtual boat in order to extrapolate either the picture motion or the body motion relative to the picture while the latter disappeared temporarily until a test-view was displayed as a final orientation candidate. Results suggest that perceptual interpretation and extrapolation of visual motion in the roll plane capitalize on the fronto-parietal cortical networks involving working memory processes. Extrapolation of the rotational dynamics of sea horizon revealed a RM effect simulating the role of gravity in rotational equilibrium. Modulation of the P200m component reflected spatial orientation processing and a non-voluntary detection of an incongruity between displayed and expected final orientations given the implied motion. Neuromagnetic properties of anticipatory (Contingent Magnetic Variation) and evoked (P200m) brain magnetic fields suggest, respectively, differential allocation of attentional resources by mental imagery instructions (picture vs. body tilt), and a communality of neural structures (in the right centro-parietal region) for the control of both RM and mental rotation processes. Finally, the RM of the body motion is less prone to forward shifts than that of picture motion evidencing an internalization of the implied mass of the virtual body of the observer.


Subject(s)
Imagination/physiology , Magnetoencephalography , Motion Perception/physiology , Orientation/physiology , Space Perception/physiology , Adult , Analysis of Variance , Brain/physiology , Brain Mapping , Cognition/physiology , Cues , Female , Humans , Male , Posture/physiology , Reaction Time/physiology , Set, Psychology
15.
Perception ; 27(1): 69-86, 1998.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9692089

ABSTRACT

An unfamiliar configuration lying in depth and viewed from a distance is typically seen as foreshortened. The hypothesis motivating this research was that a change in an observer's viewpoint even when the configuration is no longer visible induces an imaginal updating of the internal representation and thus reduces the degree of foreshortening. In experiment 1, observers attempted to reproduce configurations defined by three small glowing balls on a table 2 m distant under conditions of darkness following 'viewpoint change' instructions. In one condition, observers reproduced the continuously visible configuration using three other glowing balls on a nearer table while imagining standing at the distant table. In the other condition, observers viewed the configuration, it was then removed, and they walked in darkness to the far table and reproduced the configuration. Even though the observers received no additional information about the stimulus configuration in walking to the table, they were more accurate (less foreshortening) than in the other condition. In experiment 2, observers reproduced distant configurations on a nearer table more accurately when doing so from memory than when doing so while viewing the distant stimulus configuration. In experiment 3, observers performed both the real and imagined perspective change after memorizing the remote configuration. The results of the three experiments indicate that the continued visual presence of the target configuration impedes imaginary perspective-change performance and that an actual change in viewpoint does not increase reproduction accuracy substantially over that obtained with an imagined change in viewpoint.


Subject(s)
Form Perception , Mental Recall , Depth Perception , Humans
16.
Exp Brain Res ; 115(2): 361-8, 1997 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9224864

ABSTRACT

Eight healthy subjects were asked to walk blindfolded along circular paths of different radii after several practice trials with vision. Their task was to stop after completing two full revolutions. They always walked counter-clockwise (CCW) in (a) a control condition (CONTROL), including the instructions mentioned above, (b) with the further instruction to count backwards in twos (MENTAL), (c) with the instruction to count loudly (LOUD). The movement of two markers lying along the head naso-occipital axis was recorded by means of an ELITE system. Total walked distance (DISTANCE), total head turning angle (ANGLE) and average radius (RADIUS) of the trajectories performed were measured. All subjects were able to perform approximately circular trajectories. They consistently overshot the ideal radius independently of the condition and circle size, undershot the total angle and overshot total distance. The LOUD condition induced greater errors in the performance but only on total distance (P<0.05). A strong correlation was found between the errors in radius and total distance but not between distance and total angle. Principal components analysis suggested that radius and distance share a common source of errors while total angle produced independent errors. The results indicate that (a) circular trajectories can be generated starting from spatial and/or motor memory, without the aid of visual information; (b) the task needs some attentional control and does not involve simple automatic processing of afferent information; (c) different sensory information or different processing modes are probably involved in the estimation of the curvature and length of the walked path on the one hand, and of the total rotation angle on the other.


Subject(s)
Blindness/physiopathology , Locomotion/physiology , Memory/physiology , Space Perception/physiology , Adult , Blindness/psychology , Cues , Darkness , Data Interpretation, Statistical , Female , Head Movements/physiology , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Neural Pathways/physiology , Reproducibility of Results
17.
Microbiology (Reading) ; 143 ( Pt 5): 1649-1656, 1997 May.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9168613

ABSTRACT

This work reports the role of both superoxide dismutases-CuZnSOD (encoded by SOD1) and MnSOD (encoded by SOD2)-in the build-up of tolerance to ethanol during growth of Saccharomyces cerevisiae from exponential to post-diauxic phase. Both enzyme activities increase from the exponential phase to the diauxic shift and from the diauxic shift to the post-diauxic phase. The levels of mRNA-SOD1 and mRNA-SOD2 increase from the exponential phase to the diauxic shift; however, during the post-diauxic phase mRNA-SOD1 levels decrease while mRNA-SOD2 levels remain unchanged. These data indicate the existence of two regulatory mechanisms involved in the induction of SOD activity during growth: synthesis de novo of the proteins (until the diauxic shift), and post-transcriptional or post-translational regulation (during the post-diauxic phase). Ethanol does not alter the activities of either enzyme in cells from the diauxic shift or post-diauxic-phases, although the respective mRNA levels decrease in post-diauxic-phase cells treated with ethanol (14% or 20%). Results of experiments with sod1 and sod2 mutants show that MnSOD, but not CuZnSOD, is essential for ethanol tolerance of diauxic-shift and post-diauxic-phase cells. Evidence that ethanol toxicity is correlated with the production of reactive oxygen species in the mitochondria is obtained from results with respiration-deficient mutants. In these cells, the induction of superoxide dismutase activity by ethanol is low; also, the respiratory deficiency restores the capacity of sod2 cells to acquire ethanol tolerance.


Subject(s)
Fungal Proteins/physiology , Isoenzymes/physiology , Mitochondria/enzymology , Saccharomyces cerevisiae/enzymology , Superoxide Dismutase/physiology , Enzyme Induction , Ethanol/pharmacology , Fungal Proteins/genetics , Gene Expression Regulation, Fungal , Isoenzymes/genetics , Oxygen Consumption , RNA, Fungal/biosynthesis , RNA, Messenger/biosynthesis , Saccharomyces cerevisiae/drug effects , Saccharomyces cerevisiae/genetics , Saccharomyces cerevisiae/growth & development , Superoxide Dismutase/genetics
18.
Percept Psychophys ; 59(3): 404-18, 1997 Apr.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9136270

ABSTRACT

In the present study, we compared the effects of two processing modes on the updating of the location and orientation of a previously viewed object in space during a guided walk without vision. In Experiment 1, in order to measure the error for initial perception of object's orientation, 12 subjects rotated a miniature model until it matched the memorized orientation of its counterpart object in space. In Experiment 2, they attempted either to keep track of the object continuously (in the object-centered [OC] task) or to estimate the object's perspective only at the terminal vantage point given the trajectory they walked (in the trajectory-centered [TC] task). Subjects indicated the location of the object by facing it, and then rotated the model in order to indicate its orientation from the new vantage point. Results showed that, with respect to the TC mode, the OC mode induced a slow-down of the subjects' self-paced locomotion velocity for both linear and angular movements, and a decrease of the latencies as well as smaller absolute errors for the orientation-of-the-object response. Mean signed errors on object's orientation were equivalent for both processing modes, suggesting that the latter induced different allocations of processing resources on a common representation of space updated by "path integration."


Subject(s)
Locomotion , Orientation , Visual Perception , Adult , Female , Humans , Male , Random Allocation , Time Factors
19.
Brain Res Cogn Brain Res ; 5(3): 229-39, 1997 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9088559

ABSTRACT

In this study we compared viewer-centered (VC) with object-centered (OC) mental exploration of an imagined clock drawn on the ground. An upper case F portrayed on a computer screen was to be imagined standing up in the center of the clock. In Expt. 1, an adjustment task was used to verify that the discrimination of clock directions rendered by this perspective drawing was quite accurate. Precision was not affected by the imaginary size of the clock. In Expt. 2, subjects either (1) indicated the clock location pointed by the F given their viewing position (VC condition), or (2) their location at the periphery of the clock given the location pointed by the F (OC condition). Response latencies were proportional to the explored imaginary distance and increased with the size of the imagined environment. We found an additional mean processing time of at least 2 s in the OC condition with respect to the VC condition. Results are interpreted within Kosslyn's (Kosslyn, S.M., A cognitive neuroscience of visual cognition: further developments. In: R.H. Logie and M. Denis (Eds.), Mental Images in Human Cognition, Elsevier Science Publishers, Amsterdam, 1991, pp. 351-381 [17]) framework of cognitive neuroscience.


Subject(s)
Brain Mapping/methods , Imagination/physiology , Mental Processes/physiology , Orientation/physiology , Adult , Analysis of Variance , Female , Humans , Linear Models , Male , Reaction Time/physiology , Reproducibility of Results
20.
Acta Astronaut ; 36(8-12): 423-31, 1995.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11540973

ABSTRACT

To investigate changes in spatial orientation ability and walking performance following space flight, 7 astronaut subjects were asked pre- and post-flight to perform a goal directed locomotion paradigm which consisted of walking a triangular path with and without vision. This new paradigm, involving inputs from different sensory systems, allows quantification of several critical parameters, like orientation performance, walking velocities and postural stability, in a natural walking task. The paper presented here mainly focuses on spatial orientation performance quantified by the errors in walking the previously seen path without vision. Errors in length and reaching the corners did not change significantly from pre- to post-flight, while absolute angular errors slightly increased post-flight. The significant decrease in walking velocity and a change in head-trunk coordination while walking around the corners of the path observed post-flight may suggest that during re-adaptation to gravity the mechanisms which are necessary to perform the task have to be re-accomplished.


Subject(s)
Locomotion , Orientation , Space Flight , Space Perception , Spatial Behavior , Weightlessness , Electronic Data Processing , Female , Head Movements , Humans , Male , Sensory Deprivation , Vision, Ocular , Walking
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...