Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 20 de 26
Filter
1.
J Fluency Disord ; 80: 106059, 2024 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38640516

ABSTRACT

INTRODUCTION: Preschool-age children use mental state verbs (MSVs; e.g., think, know) to reference thoughts and other cognitive states. In play-based language, MSV use requires conversational flexibility, as speakers shift from discussion of actions happening in the here-and-now to more abstract discussion of mental states. Some evidence suggests that children who stutter (CWS) demonstrate subtle differences in shifting on experimental tasks of cognitive flexibility, differences which may extend to conversational flexibility. This study explored MSV use in conversational language between CWS and their mothers. METHODS: Thirty-five preschool-age CWS and 35 age- and gender-matched children who do not stutter (CWNS), all performing within the typical range on standardized language testing, conversed with their mothers during play. Samples were transcribed and coded for MSV use. RESULTS: No between-group differences were observed in MSV use, either between the CWS and CWNS or between the groups of mothers. Age and language skills were positively associated with MSV use in the CWNS group only. For both groups of dyads, mothers' MSV use corresponded at least to some extent to their children's language skills. Finally, correspondence between CWNS and their mothers was observed for two conversational language measures, representing lexical diversity and morphosyntax; this overall pattern was not observed in the CWS dyad group. CONCLUSIONS: Although these findings point to similar use of MSVs among the groups of children and their mothers, for the CWS group, the patterns of use in relation to age and language skills are somewhat different from developmental expectations.


Subject(s)
Mothers , Play and Playthings , Stuttering , Humans , Stuttering/psychology , Child, Preschool , Female , Male , Mothers/psychology , Play and Playthings/psychology , Mother-Child Relations/psychology , Child Language , Case-Control Studies
2.
J Fluency Disord ; 70: 105877, 2021 12.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34534900

ABSTRACT

PURPOSE: The primary purpose of this study was to examine complex nonverbal response inhibition and stopping impulsivity in children who do (CWS) and do not stutter (CWNS). METHOD: A total of 30 CWS and 30 CWNS between the ages of 3;1 and 6;0 (years; months) performed the Peg-Tapping Task (PTT; Diamond & Taylor, 1996; Luria, 1966), in which children were required to tap a dowel once when an examiner taps twice and vice versa. The main dependent variables were the number of practice trials, response accuracy, response latency for accurate responses, and the number of extra taps (i.e., tapping more than two times). RESULTS: The CWS were less accurate and slower on the PTT than the CWNS, with no differences in the number of practice trials. Furthermore, the CWS, especially boys, produced more extra taps than the CWNS. CONCLUSIONS: Findings revealed that preschool CWS have weaknesses in complex response inhibition and stopping impulsivity in the nonverbal domain compared to CWNS. Taken together, these findings along with those of previous studies indicate that CWS may have weaknesses in inhibition and impulsivity in the nonverbal domain as well as the verbal domain, suggesting a domain-general weakness.


Subject(s)
Stuttering , Case-Control Studies , Child , Child, Preschool , Humans , Impulsive Behavior , Infant , Inhibition, Psychological , Male , Reaction Time
3.
J Fluency Disord ; 67: 105825, 2021 03.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33429306

ABSTRACT

PURPOSE: The purpose of the study was to examine the performance of Persian speaking children who stutter (CWS) and children who do not stutter (CWNS) on three nonword repetition tasks, while also focusing on which task and scoring method best differentiates the two groups of children. METHOD: Thirty CWS and 30 CWNS between the ages of 5;0 to 6;6 completed three nonword repetition tasks that varied in complexity. Each task was scored using two methods: nonwords correct and phonemes correct. Between-group differences in performance on each task were examined, along with disfluencies for CWS and the task and scoring method that best differentiated the CWS and CWNS. RESULTS: The findings revealed that, across all three nonword repetition tasks, the CWS consistently produced fewer nonwords correct and phonemes correct than the CWNS group at virtually all syllable lengths. The CWS produced more disfluencies on longer nonwords than shorter nonwords in all three nonword repetition tasks. The nonword repetition task with lower wordlikeness and more phonologically complex items best differentiated the two groups of children. Findings further revealed that discriminative accuracy was highest for scoring based on the number of phonemes produced correctly. CONCLUSION: Findings provide further evidence to suggest that CWS may have difficulty with phonological working memory and/or phonological processing.


Subject(s)
Stuttering , Case-Control Studies , Child , Child, Preschool , Humans , Language Tests , Linguistics , Speech Production Measurement
4.
J Fluency Disord ; 66: 105792, 2020 12.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33032169

ABSTRACT

PURPOSE: Attention develops gradually from infancy to the preschool years and beyond. Exogenous attention, consisting of automatic responses to salient stimuli, develops in infancy, whereas endogenous attention, or voluntary attention, begins to develop later, in the preschool years. The purpose of this study was to examine (a) exogenous and endogenous attention in young children who stutter (CWS) and children who do not stutter (CWNS) through two conditions of a visual sustained selective attention task, and (b) visual short-term memory (STM) between groups within the context of this task. METHOD: 42 CWS and 42 CWNS, ages 3;0-5;5 (years;months), were pair-matched in age, gender (31 males, 11 females per group), and socioeconomic status. Children completed a visual tracking task (Track-It Task; Fisher et al., 2013) requiring sustained selective attention and engaging exogenous and endogenous processes. Following each item, children were asked to recall the item they had tracked, as a memory check. RESULTS: The CWS group demonstrated significantly less accuracy in overall tracking and visual memory for the tracked stimuli, compared to the CWNS group. Across groups, the children performed better in sustained selective attention when the target stimuli were more salient (the condition tapping both exogenous and endogenous attention) than when stimuli were less so (the condition tapping primarily endogenous processes). CONCLUSIONS: Relative to peers, preschool-age CWS, as a group, display weaknesses in visual sustained selective attention and visual STM.


Subject(s)
Attention/physiology , Executive Function/physiology , Memory, Short-Term/physiology , Stuttering/physiopathology , Case-Control Studies , Child, Preschool , Female , Humans , Male , Reaction Time , Stuttering/diagnosis , Stuttering/psychology
5.
J Speech Lang Hear Res ; 63(11): 3659-3679, 2020 11 13.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33108236

ABSTRACT

Purpose The purpose of this study was to examine cognitive flexibility for semantic and perceptual information in preschool children who stutter (CWS) and who do not stutter (CWNS). Method Participants were 44 CWS and 44 CWNS between the ages of 3;0 and 5;11 (years;months). Cognitive flexibility was measured using semantic and perceptual categorization tasks. In each task, children were required to match a target object with two different semantic or perceptual associates. Main dependent variables were reaction time and accuracy. Results The accuracy with which CWS and CWNS shifted between one semantic and perceptual representation to another was similar, but the CWS did so significantly more slowly. Both groups of children had more difficulty switching between perceptual representations than semantic ones. Conclusion CWS are less efficient (slower), though not less accurate, than CWNS in their ability to switch between different representations in both the verbal and nonverbal domains.


Subject(s)
Semantics , Stuttering , Case-Control Studies , Child, Preschool , Cognition , Humans , Infant , Reaction Time
6.
Semin Speech Lang ; 40(4): 305-319, 2019 08.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31311055

ABSTRACT

Developmental stuttering is a complex disorder and children who stutter form a heterogeneous group. Most contemporary researchers would agree that multiple factors, including those associated with linguistic, motor, sensory, and emotional processes, are likely involved in its development and/or maintenance. There is growing evidence, however, that cognitive processes also play a role. In this article, we briefly review behavioral and parent-report studies of executive function in children who stutter, the findings of which have generally suggested that these skills may be challenging for at least some children who stutter. We then consider how deficits in executive function could provide an explanatory account for not only the multifactorial nature of developmental stuttering but also the considerable amount of variability that exists among individuals who stutter.


Subject(s)
Executive Function , Stuttering/diagnosis , Adolescent , Child , Child, Preschool , Cognition , Female , Humans , Inhibition, Psychological , Male , Mental Recall , Risk Factors , Stuttering/etiology , Stuttering/psychology
7.
J Speech Lang Hear Res ; 62(3): 644-667, 2019 03 25.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30950742

ABSTRACT

Purpose The purpose of this study was to examine the verbal short-term memory skills of children who stutter (CWS) and children who do not stutter (CWNS) in 2 experiments, focusing on the influence of phonological and semantic similarity. Method Participants were 42 CWS and 42 CWNS between the ages of 3;0 and 5;11 (years;months). In Experiment 1, children completed the phonological similarity task, in which they listened to lists of phonologically similar and dissimilar words and then repeated them when signaled to do so. In Experiment 2, children completed another forward span task, the semantic category task, which is similar to the phonological similarity task, except that it consisted of lists of semantically homogeneous and heterogeneous words. Main dependent variables were cumulative memory span, proportion of errors by type, and speech reaction time (SRT) for correct responses. Results The CWS exhibited significantly shorter memory spans for phonologically dissimilar words and were less affected by the phonological qualities of the words than the CWNS in Experiment 1, based on the findings of both between-groups and within-group analyses. In Experiment 2, although the groups did not differ in their performance in either condition, within-group analyses revealed that the CWNS benefitted from semantic similarity, whereas the CWS did not. The between-groups difference in absolute difference scores, however, did not reach significance. The CWS produced more omissions and false alarms than the CWNS in both experiments, but the 2 groups of children were otherwise comparable in SRT, although the CWS exhibited overall faster SRT than the CWNS in Experiment 2. Conclusions Verbal short-term memory is one domain-general cognitive process in which CWS display weakness relative to typically fluent peers. These weaknesses are likely due, in part, to differences in phonological and, perhaps, semantic processing of words to aid memory.


Subject(s)
Memory, Short-Term , Phonetics , Semantics , Stuttering/psychology , Verbal Learning , Case-Control Studies , Child, Preschool , Female , Humans , Male , Reaction Time
8.
J Speech Lang Hear Res ; 61(7): 1626-1648, 2018 07 13.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29984373

ABSTRACT

Purpose: This study presents a meta-analytic review of differences in verbal short-term memory, inhibition, and attention between children who stutter (CWS) and children who do not stutter (CWNS). Method: Electronic databases and reference sections of articles were searched for candidate studies that examined verbal short-term memory, inhibition, and attention using behavioral and/or parent report measures. Twenty-nine studies met the eligibility criteria, which included, among other things, children between the ages of 3 and 18 years and the availability of quantitative data for effect size calculations. Data were extracted, coded, and analyzed, with the magnitude of the difference between the 2 groups of children being estimated using Hedge's g (Hedges & Olkin, 1985). Results: Based on the random-effects model (Hunter & Schmidt, 2004), findings revealed that CWS scored lower than CWNS on measures of nonword repetition (Hedges' g = -0.62), particularly at lengths of 2 and 3 syllables (Hedges' g = -0.62 and - 0.50, respectively), and forward span (Hedges' g = -0.40). Analyses further revealed that the parents of CWS rated their children as having weaker inhibition (Hedges' g = -0.44) and attentional focus/persistence (Hedges' g = -0.36) skills than the parents of CWNS, but there were no significant differences between CWS and CWNS in behavioral measures of inhibition and attention. Conclusion: The present findings were taken to suggest that cognitive processes are important variables associated with developmental stuttering.


Subject(s)
Attention , Inhibition, Psychological , Memory, Short-Term , Stuttering/psychology , Adolescent , Child , Child, Preschool , Female , Humans , Male
9.
J Fluency Disord ; 56: 18-32, 2018 06.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29443692

ABSTRACT

PURPOSE: The purpose of this study was to examine the executive function (EF) abilities of preschool children who do (CWS) and do not stutter (CWNS) using a parent-report questionnaire and a behavioral task. METHOD: Participants were 75 CWS and 75 CWNS between the ages of 3;0 and 5;11 (years; months). Parents rated their children's EF abilities using the Behavioral Rating Inventory of Executive Function-Preschool Version (BRIEF-P; Gioia, Espy, & Isquith, 2003). Children's ability to integrate cognitive flexibility, inhibitory control, and working memory was measured using a behavioral task, the Head-Toes-Knees-Shoulders (HTKS; Cameron Ponitz, McClelland, Matthews, & Morrison, 2009). RESULTS: The CWS were judged by their parents as being less proficient in working memory, shift/flexibility, and overall EF than the parents of the CWNS. Children in the CWS group were also 2½ to 7 times more likely than children in the CWNS group to exhibit clinically significant difficulties with EF. Behavioral task findings revealed that 3-year old CWS performed more poorly than their peers on the HTKS. Parental ratings of executive function and working memory were significantly and moderately correlated with receptive and expressive vocabulary skills only for the CWNS group. CONCLUSION: CWS have more difficulty with EF in everyday life and may experience early delays in their ability to integrate aspects of attention and EF compared to CWNS.


Subject(s)
Cognition/physiology , Executive Function/physiology , Stuttering/diagnosis , Child, Preschool , Female , Humans , Male , Parents
10.
J Speech Lang Hear Res ; 60(4): 836-852, 2017 04 14.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28384673

ABSTRACT

Purpose: The purpose of this study was to examine (a) explicit and implicit verbal response inhibition in preschool children who do stutter (CWS) and do not stutter (CWNS) and (b) the relationship between response inhibition and language skills. Method: Participants were 41 CWS and 41 CWNS between the ages of 3;1 and 6;1 (years;months). Explicit verbal response inhibition was measured using a computerized version of the grass-snow task (Carlson & Moses, 2001), and implicit verbal response inhibition was measured using the baa-meow task. Main dependent variables were reaction time and accuracy. Results: The CWS were significantly less accurate than the CWNS on the implicit task, but not the explicit task. The CWS also exhibited slower reaction times than the CWNS on both tasks. Between-group differences in performance could not be attributed to working memory demands. Overall, children's performance on the inhibition tasks corresponded with parents' perceptions of their children's inhibition skills in daily life. Conclusions: CWS are less effective and efficient than CWNS in suppressing a dominant response while executing a conflicting response in the verbal domain.


Subject(s)
Child Language , Inhibition, Psychological , Stuttering , Verbal Behavior , Analysis of Variance , Child , Child, Preschool , Conflict, Psychological , Female , Hearing Tests , Humans , Language Tests , Male , Memory, Short-Term , Motor Skills , Parents , Reaction Time
11.
Child Obes ; 11(6): 674-82, 2015 Dec.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26478951

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: The Healthy Homes/Healthy Kids Preschool (HHHK-Preschool) pilot program is an obesity prevention intervention integrating pediatric care provider counseling and a phone-based program to prevent unhealthy weight gain among 2- to 4-year-old children at risk for obesity (BMI percentile between the 50th and 85th percentile and at least one overweight parent) or currently overweight (85th percentile ≤ BMI < 95th percentile). The aim of this randomized, controlled pilot study was to evaluate the feasibility, acceptability, and potential efficacy of the HHHK-Preschool intervention. METHODS: Sixty parent-child dyads recruited from pediatric primary care clinics were randomized to: (1) the Busy Bodies/Better Bites Obesity Prevention Arm or the (2) Healthy Tots/Safe Spots safety/injury prevention Contact Control Arm. Baseline and 6-month data were collected, including measured height and weight, accelerometry, previous day dietary recalls, and parent surveys. Intervention process data (e.g., call completion) were also collected. RESULTS: High intervention completion and satisfaction rates were observed. Although a statistically significant time by treatment interaction was not observed for BMI percentile or BMI z-score, post-hoc examination of baseline weight status as a moderator of treatment outcome showed that the Busy Bodies/Better Bites obesity prevention intervention appeared to be effective among children who were in the overweight category at baseline relative to those who were categorized as at risk for obesity (p = 0.04). CONCLUSIONS: HHHK-Preschool pilot study results support the feasibility, acceptability, and potential efficacy in already overweight children of a pediatric primary care-based obesity prevention intervention integrating brief provider counseling and parent-targeted phone coaching. What's New: Implementing pediatric primary care-based obesity interventions is challenging. Previous interventions have primarily involved in-person sessions, a barrier to sustained parent involvement. HHHK-preschool pilot study results suggest that integrating brief provider counseling and parent-targeted phone coaching is a promising approach.


Subject(s)
Counseling , Health Education/methods , Parents/education , Pediatric Obesity/prevention & control , Pediatrics , Adult , Body Mass Index , Child, Preschool , Diet , Exercise , Female , Humans , Male , Overweight , Physicians, Primary Care , Pilot Projects , Primary Health Care , Risk Factors , Television , Weight Gain
12.
J Phys Act Health ; 12(6): 861-8, 2015 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25133750

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: Although the prevalence of obesity in young children highlights the importance of early interventions to promote physical activity (PA), there are limited data on activity patterns in this age group. The purpose of this study was to describe activity patterns in preschool-aged children and explore differences by weight status. METHODS: Analyses use baseline data from Healthy Homes/Healthy Kids-Preschool, a pilot obesity prevention trial of preschool-aged children who are overweight or at risk for being overweight. A modified parent-reported version of the previous-day PA recall was used to summarize types of activity. Accelerometry was used to summarize daily and hourly activity patterns. RESULTS: "Playing with toys" accounted for the largest proportion of a child's previous day, followed by "meals and snacks" and "chores." Accelerometry-measured daily time spent in sedentary behavior, light PA, and moderate-to-vigorous PA (MVPA) was 412, 247, and 69 minutes, respectively. Percentage of hourly time spent in MVPA ranged from 3% to 13%, peaking in the late morning and evening hours. There were no statistically significant MVPA differences by weight status. CONCLUSIONS: This study extends our understanding of activity types, amounts, and patterns in preschool-aged children and warrants further exploration of differences in PA patterns by weight status.


Subject(s)
Accelerometry/methods , Child, Preschool/statistics & numerical data , Exercise/physiology , Obesity/complications , Child , Female , Humans , Male , Obesity/epidemiology
13.
Contemp Clin Trials ; 36(1): 228-43, 2013 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23816490

ABSTRACT

Pediatric primary care is an important setting in which to address obesity prevention, yet relatively few interventions have been evaluated and even fewer have been shown to be effective. The development and evaluation of cost-effective approaches to obesity prevention that leverage opportunities of direct access to families in the pediatric primary care setting, overcome barriers to implementation in busy practice settings, and facilitate sustained involvement of parents is an important public health priority. The goal of the Healthy Homes/Healthy Kids (HHHK 5-10) randomized controlled trial is to evaluate the efficacy of a relatively low-cost primary care-based obesity prevention intervention aimed at 5 to 10 year old children who are at risk for obesity. Four hundred twenty one parent/child dyads were recruited and randomized to either the obesity prevention arm or a Contact Control condition that focuses on safety and injury prevention. The HHHK 5-10 obesity prevention intervention combines brief counseling with a pediatric primary care provider during routine well child visits and follow-up telephone coaching that supports parents in making home environmental changes to support healthful eating, activity patterns, and body weight. The Contact Control condition combines the same provider counseling with telephone coaching focused on safety and injury prevention messages. This manuscript describes the study design and baseline characteristics of participants enrolled in the HHHK 5-10 trial.


Subject(s)
Counseling/organization & administration , Parents , Pediatric Obesity/prevention & control , Primary Health Care/organization & administration , Accelerometry , Body Mass Index , Child , Child, Preschool , Cost-Benefit Analysis , Diet , Exercise , Female , Humans , Male , Overweight/prevention & control , Parent-Child Relations , Risk Factors , Safety , Sedentary Behavior , Socioeconomic Factors
14.
Prev Med ; 56(3-4): 171-7, 2013 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23276775

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVE: The Keep It Off trial evaluated the efficacy of a phone-based weight loss maintenance intervention among adults who had recently lost weight in Minnesota (2007-2010). METHODS: 419 adults who had recently lost ≥ 10% of their body weight were randomized to the "Guided" or "Self-Directed" intervention. Guided participants received a 10 session workbook, 10 biweekly, eight monthly and six bimonthly phone coaching calls, bimonthly weight graphs and tailored letters based on self-reported weights. Self-Directed participants received the workbook and two calls. Primary outcomes are weight change and maintenance (regain of <2.5% of baseline body weight). RESULTS: Mixed model repeated-measures analysis examining weight change revealed a significant time by treatment group interaction (p<0.0085). Guided participants regained significantly less weight than the Self-Directed participants at 12 and 24 months. The odds of 24 month maintenance were 1.37 (95% CI: 0.97-2.03) times greater in the Guided than in the Self-Directed group. When maintenance rates were compared across all follow-ups, there was a consistently higher maintenance rate for Guided participants (HR 1.31, 95% CI: 1.12-1.54). CONCLUSIONS: A sustained, supportive phone- and mail-based intervention promotes weight loss maintenance relative to a brief intervention for participants who have recently lost weight.


Subject(s)
Obesity/therapy , Weight Reduction Programs , Adult , Body Mass Index , Female , Follow-Up Studies , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Secondary Prevention , Treatment Outcome , Weight Loss
15.
J Fluency Disord ; 37(3): 188-201, 2012 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22682320

ABSTRACT

UNLABELLED: The purpose of the present study was to explore the phonological working memory of adults who stutter through the use of a non-word repetition and a phoneme elision task. Participants were 14 adults who stutter (M=28 years) and 14 age/gender matched adults who do not stutter (M=28 years). For the non-word repetition task, the participants had to repeat a set of 12 non-words across four syllable lengths (2-, 3-, 4-, and 7-syllables) (N=48 total non-words). For the phoneme elision task, the participants repeated the same set of non-words at each syllable length, but with a designated target phoneme eliminated. Adults who stutter were significantly less accurate than adults who do not stutter in their initial attempts to produce the longest non-words (i.e., 7-syllable). Adults who stutter also required a significantly higher mean number of attempts to accurately produce 7-syllable non-words than adults who do not stutter. For the phoneme elision task, both groups demonstrated a significant reduction in accuracy as the non-words increased in length; however, there was no significant interaction between group and syllable length. Thus, although there appear to be advancements in the phonological working memory for adults who stutter relative to children who stutter, preliminary data from the present study suggest that the advancements may not be comparable to those demonstrated by adults who do not stutter. EDUCATIONAL OBJECTIVES: At the end of this activity the reader will be able to (a) summarize the nonword repetition data that have been published thus far with children and adults who stutter; (b) describe the subvocal rehearsal system, an aspect of the phonological working memory that is critical to nonword repetition accuracy; (c) employ an alternative means to explore the phonological working memory in adults who stutter, the phoneme elision task; and (d) discuss both phonological and motoric implications of deficits in the phonological working memory.


Subject(s)
Phonetics , Stuttering/physiopathology , Adolescent , Adult , Case-Control Studies , Female , Humans , Male , Memory, Short-Term , Speech , Speech Production Measurement , Young Adult
16.
Contemp Clin Trials ; 32(4): 551-60, 2011 Jul.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21453791

ABSTRACT

Long-term weight-loss maintenance is notoriously difficult to achieve and promote. As the novelty of weight loss treatment fades, enthusiasm for diet and exercise tends to wane in the maintenance phase. Given the recognition of obesity as a chronic disorder requiring continued engagement in weight-control behaviors, there is a need to identify cost-effective and supportive therapies that can sustain motivation. In this paper, we describe the study design and baseline characteristics of participants enrolled in a trial to evaluate a program (Keep It Off) developed specifically for weight-loss maintenance using therapeutic phone contact with recent weight losers throughout the period in which they are at highest risk for weight regain. In the Keep It Off randomized clinical trial we are evaluating this phone-based intervention that focuses on key weight-loss maintenance behaviors followed by continued self-monitoring, reporting of weight, feedback, and outreach in members of a Minnesota managed-care organization. The goal of the intervention is to flatten the typical relapse curve. Moreover, data from this trial will inform our understanding of weight-loss maintenance, including predictors and behaviors that increase the likelihood of success over the long term.


Subject(s)
Directive Counseling , Obesity/therapy , Weight Loss , Adult , Clinical Protocols , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Obesity/psychology , Patient Selection , Regression Analysis , Research Design , Telephone , Treatment Outcome
17.
J Fluency Disord ; 35(3): 216-34, 2010 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20831969

ABSTRACT

UNLABELLED: Relatively recently, experimental studies of linguistic processing speed in children who stutter (CWS) have emerged, some of which suggest differences in performance among CWS compared to children who do not stutter (CWNS). What is not yet well understood is the extent to which underlying cognitive skills may impact performance on timed tasks of linguistic performance. The purpose of this study was to explore possible relationships between measures of linguistic processing speed and two aspects of cognition: phonological working memory and attention. Participants were 9 CWS and 14 CWNS between the ages of 3;6 and 5;2. Children participated in a computerized picture naming task (an index of linguistic processing speed) and a nonword repetition task (an index of phonological working memory). Parents completed a temperament behavior questionnaire, from which information about the children's attentional skills was collected. Findings revealed that the groups did not differ from each other on speed of picture naming or attention; however, the CWS performed significantly worse in nonword repetition. In addition, after partialling out the effects of age, (a) for CWS only, there was a significant negative relationship between picture naming speed and nonword repetition; (b) there were no significant relationships for either group between aspects of attention and picture naming speed; and (c) only the CWNS showed a significant relationship between nonword repetition and focused attentional skills. These results underscore the need to consider the underlying skills associated with lexically related aspects of language production when examining the task performances of CWS and CWNS. EDUCATIONAL OBJECTIVES: The reader will be able to: (a) summarize findings from previous studies examining the speech and language performance of children who do (CWS) and do not stutter (CWNS); (b) describe findings of previous studies related to nonword repetition and attention in CWS; (c) compare the results of the present study with previous work in this area; and (d) discuss speculations concerning the relationship between linguistic processing speed, phonological working memory, and attention in CWS and CWNS.


Subject(s)
Attention , Memory, Short-Term , Speech , Stuttering/psychology , Adult , Case-Control Studies , Child, Preschool , Female , Humans , Linguistics , Male , Reaction Time , Statistics, Nonparametric , Surveys and Questionnaires , Temperament
18.
J Speech Lang Hear Res ; 53(5): 1260-1262, 2010 Oct.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31819344

ABSTRACT

PURPOSE: Findings from Anderson (2007) were broadly interpreted according to several psycholinguistic-oriented theories of stuttering, including EXPLAN (Howell, 2004). Although the study was not explicitly designed to test EXPLAN, it was generally concluded that the evidence does not provide appreciable support for EXPLAN. Howell (2010) objected to this conclusion on several grounds, claiming that the findings do, in fact, support EXPLAN. Anderson responds to Howell's objections in this letter. METHOD: Background and perspective on the original study were presented. Howell's comments on EXPLAN's predictions concerning stuttering on "easy" and "difficult" words and the relationship between speech errors and stuttering are disputed in this reply to Howell's commentary. RESULTS: Howell's claims that the findings of Anderson support EXPLAN are largely refuted on the basis that (a) the data had not been analyzed by grammatical class and (b) there are no clear predictions regarding the association between variables affecting speech errors and stuttering in EXPLAN. CONCLUSIONS: Howell's commentary seemingly represents an attempt to provide confirmatory evidence for EXPLAN using data that are simply not suitable for this purpose.

19.
J Fluency Disord ; 34(4): 257-78, 2009 Dec.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20113770

ABSTRACT

UNLABELLED: The purpose of this investigation was to replicate the methods of Anderson, Pellowski, and Conture (2005) to determine whether a different sample of preschool children who stutter (CWS) exhibit more dissociations in speech-language abilities than children who do not stutter (CWNS; Study 1) and to examine the relation between dissociations and specific characteristics of stuttering (e.g., most common disfluency type) using a much larger sample size (Study 2). Participants for Study 1 were 40 CWS and 40 CWNS between the ages of 3;0 and 5;11. Participants for Study 2 were the same as for Study 1 plus the 45 CWS and 45 CWNS used by Anderson et al. (2005) for a total of 85 CWS and 85 CWNS. Participants were administered five standardized speech-language (sub)tests and a conversational speech sample was obtained from each participant for the analyses of speech disfluencies/stuttering. Standard scores from the standardized speech-language tests were analyzed using a correlation-based statistical procedure (Bates, Applebaum, Sacedo, Saygin, & Pizzamiglio, 2003) to identify possible dissociations among the speech-language measures. Findings from Study 1 supported Anderson et al.'s findings that CWS exhibited significantly more speech-language dissociations than CWNS. Results from Study 2 further revealed that CWS who exhibited dissociations were more likely to exhibit non-stuttered (other) disfluencies as their most common disfluency type. Findings provide further support for the possibility that dissociations among various aspects of the speech-language system may contribute to the difficulties that some children have establishing normally fluent speech. EDUCATIONAL OBJECTIVES: The reader will be able to: (a) summarize findings from previous studies examining the speech and language performance of children who do and do not stutter; (b) describe the concept of "dissociations" in the speech and language skills of young children; (c) compare the results of the present study with previous work in this area; and (d) discuss speculations concerning the manner in which dissociations might affect fluency development in children who stutter.


Subject(s)
Child Language , Linguistics , Stuttering , Child, Preschool , Female , Humans , Interpersonal Relations , Language Tests , Male , Models, Statistical , Psycholinguistics , Speech , Speech Production Measurement , Vocabulary
20.
J Fluency Disord ; 33(2): 135-55, 2008.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18617053

ABSTRACT

UNLABELLED: The effects of age of acquisition and repetition priming on picture naming latencies and errors were studied in 22 children who stutter (CWS) and 22 children who do not stutter (CWNS) between the ages of 3;1 and 5;7. Children participated in a computerized picture naming task where they named pictures of both early and late acquired (AoA) words in two consecutive stages. Findings revealed that all children's picture naming latencies and errors were reduced following repetition priming and in response to early AoA words relative to late AoA words. AoA and repetition priming effects were similar for children in both talker groups, with one exception. Namely, CWS benefitted significantly more, in terms of error reduction, than CWNS from repetition priming for late AoA words. In addition, CWNS exhibited a significant, positive association between linguistic speed and measures of vocabulary, but CWS did not. These findings were taken to suggest that the (a) semantic-phonological connections of CWS may not be as strong as those of CWNS, and (b) existing lexical measures may not be sensitive enough to differentiate CWS from CWNS in lexically related aspects of language production. EDUCATIONAL OBJECTIVES: After reading this article, the learner will be able to: (a) describe the effects of repetition priming and age of word acquisition in speech production; (b) summarize the performance similarities and differences of children who stutter and children who do not stutter on a computerized picture naming task; and (c) compare the results of the present study with previous work in this area.


Subject(s)
Stuttering/epidemiology , Verbal Behavior , Verbal Learning , Visual Perception , Age Factors , Child , Child, Preschool , Female , Humans , Language , Male , Speech Production Measurement
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...