Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 10 de 10
Filter
Add more filters










Publication year range
2.
Horm Behav ; 54(2): 302-11, 2008 Aug.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18471815

ABSTRACT

Callitrichids are cooperative breeders, characterized by obligate twinning, extensive paternal care, and monopolization of reproduction by the dominant female. This is the case in the common marmoset, and in common marmoset groups of more than one adult female, subordinate females are typically acyclic consistent with infertility. However, one callitrichid, Goeldi's monkey, gives birth to singletons and exhibits low paternal care. Given these reproductive traits of Goeldi's monkey, we hypothesized that there would not be suppression of ovarian activity. To test this hypothesis, we applied non-invasive endocrine methods in a step-wise experiment with laboratory groups of both species. In each species, six pairs of sisters were studied alone, in visual contact with an unrelated male and in a polygynous trio with the male, and urine samples were collected for determination of oestrogen titres reflecting ovarian activity. Common marmoset sister pairs exhibited a marked difference in social status: during the study 5 of 6 dominant females conceived but only 1 of 6 subordinate females; the remaining 5 subordinates were acyclic at the end of the study, and instances of ovulation typically resulted in aggression. Goeldi's monkey sister pairs showed no status differences: in all pairs, however, both sisters exhibited a temporary cessation of ovarian cyclicity on trio formation, followed by ovulation and conception. We conclude that these marked differences in ovarian responses reflect the differences in inter-female competition for paternal caregiving resources. In common marmosets with high inter-female competition, suppression of ovulation functions to reduce aggression received by subordinate females; in Goeldi's monkey with low competition, temporary cessation of ovulation could facilitate female choice.


Subject(s)
Callimico/physiology , Callithrix/physiology , Mating Preference, Animal/physiology , Ovary/physiology , Pair Bond , Age Factors , Animal Communication , Animals , Breeding , Callimico/blood , Callithrix/blood , Choice Behavior/physiology , Estradiol/blood , Female , Hydrocortisone/blood , Male , Social Behavior , Species Specificity
3.
Am J Primatol ; 69(11): 1210-22, 2007 Nov.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17387731

ABSTRACT

Neophobia, defined as showing caution toward novel features of the environment, is widespread in birds and mammals; it can be affected by ecology, early experience, and social context. In this study, we aimed to (i) investigate the response to novel food in adult common marmosets and Goeldi's monkeys and (ii) assess the role of social influences. We used an experimental paradigm employed previously with capuchin monkeys and children, in which a subject (observer) was presented with a novel food under three conditions: (i) Presence: group members did not have food; (ii) Different color: group members received familiar food whose color differed from that of the observer's novel food; (iii) Same color: group members received familiar food of the same color as the observer's novel food. Although most common marmosets tasted and/or ate the novel food, none of the Goeldi's monkeys ate it and only two sampled it. Differences in home range size and early social experience might explain the divergent behavior of the two species. Observers of both species similarly attended to group members and their visual attention increased with the number of group members eating, especially when the observer's and group members' foods were perceptually similar. However, we observed social influences on explorative behavior in Goeldi's monkeys but not on explorative or eating behavior in common marmosets. This result might be explained by the different pattern of response to novel food observed in the two species. Moreover, social influences on Goeldi's monkeys' behavior were nonspecific, i.e. they were not based on an appreciation that the food is safe because eaten by group members.


Subject(s)
Behavior, Animal , Callimico/physiology , Callithrix/physiology , Food , Social Behavior , Animals , Color , Female , Male , Species Specificity
4.
Horm Behav ; 46(5): 670-7, 2004 Dec.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15555510

ABSTRACT

Previous studies have found a clear relationship between prolactin (prl) and paternal care in various vertebrate taxa. In New World monkeys, it has been demonstrated in several species that fathers have high prolactin levels even during periods without infant rearing. In this study, we followed the reproductive careers of common marmoset males as they transitioned from being an adult son within their native family to fathering their own offspring for the first time. Specifically, we examined the first experience of elevated prolactin levels in marmoset males. Additionally, we investigated the effects of the total number of experienced births as well as of age on prolactin levels. Our results show that common marmoset males did not experience an increase in prolactin secretion after pairing or shortly before birth of their first infants. However, prolactin levels rose more than twofold after the birth of their first infants and had lowered again 2.5 months after this event. We found no correlation between prolactin levels and the number of previous births experienced or age. Our study demonstrates that further work about a possible enhancing effect of prolactin on paternal care, by means of experimentally reducing hormonal levels, should be conducted in common marmosets using first-time fathers before males experience the first paternal increase in prolactin levels.


Subject(s)
Aging/physiology , Callithrix/urine , Paternal Behavior , Prolactin/urine , Sexual Behavior, Animal/physiology , Animals , Male
5.
Immunogenetics ; 55(11): 785-90, 2004 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-14714152

ABSTRACT

Identification of New World monkey MHC-DRB alleles has previously relied upon labor-intensive cloning and sequencing techniques. Here we describe a rapid and unambiguous way to distinguish DRB alleles in New World monkeys using the polymerase chain reaction (PCR), denaturing gradient gel electrophoresis (DGGE), and direct sequencing. The highly variable second exon of New World monkey DRB alleles was amplified using generic DRB primers and alleles were separated by DGGE. DNA was then reamplified from plugs removed from the gel and alleles were determined using fluorescent-based sequencing. The validity of this typing procedure was confirmed by the identification of all DRB alleles previously characterized by cloning and sequencing techniques from an individual cotton-top tamarin. Importantly, our analysis revealed DRB alleles not previously identified in this reference animal. Following validation of our technique, the protocol was employed for the characterization of MHC-DRB alleles in four other species of New World monkey: the pygmy marmoset, white-faced saki monkey, long-haired spider monkey and owl monkey. Using this technique, we identified five alleles from the cotton-top tamarin, five alleles from the owl monkey, three alleles from the long-haired spider monkey, three alleles from the white-faced saki monkey and two alleles from the pygmy marmoset. On the basis of phylogenetic tree analyses, 13 new DRB alleles were assigned to eight different MHC-DRB lineages. Whereas traditional DRB typing via cloning and sequencing provides limited information, our new technique provides a simple and relatively rapid way of identifying New World monkey MHC-DRB alleles.


Subject(s)
Cebidae/genetics , Histocompatibility Antigens Class II/genetics , Animals , Base Sequence , Electrophoresis, Polyacrylamide Gel , Molecular Sequence Data , Phylogeny , Polymerase Chain Reaction , Sequence Analysis, DNA
6.
J Hum Evol ; 45(5): 389-99, 2003 Nov.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-14624749

ABSTRACT

In biparental species, females are thought to accept and even to seek male assistance in rearing of the young. In this study, we present data that suggests that Goeldi's monkey females (Callimico goeldii) actually reject male parental care for a certain period. In Callimico, which have biparental care, mothers carry the infant exclusively for the first three weeks despite the fact that fathers are interested in their infants directly after birth. Fathers initiated significantly more body contact with their mates when newborn infants were present and retrieved one-day-old infants that were experimentally presented to them. The onset of paternal carrying in our colony was 27.5 days compared to 11.5 days (medians) observed in the field. However, presentation of a live potential predator induced earlier onset of paternal carrying by about 10 days. Additional costs to maternal carrying such as foraging and predator avoidance are likely factors influencing the decision of the mother as to when to share the cost of infant carrying with the father. We conclude that Goeldi's monkey fathers start carrying their infants so late because they do not get them earlier from the mother. We suggest that one ultimate explanation for that delay might be increased infant mortality when infant transfer starts at an earlier stage.


Subject(s)
Behavior, Animal , Callimico , Cooperative Behavior , Maternal Behavior , Paternal Behavior , Animals , Female , Lifting , Male , Motivation , Pair Bond , Sex Factors
7.
J Comp Psychol ; 117(2): 166-75, 2003 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-12856787

ABSTRACT

The authors explored whether prolactin is associated with paternal care in 3 monkey species: titi monkey (Callicebus cupreus), common marmoset (Callithrix jacchus), and Goeldi's monkey (Callimico goeldii). They compared prolactin levels in fathers before and after infant birth as well as between fathers and nonfathers. C. cupreus fathers carry infants almost exclusively, have higher prolactin levels than nonfathers, but show no prolactin increase after infant birth. C. goeldii fathers carry infants only after 3 weeks, show an increase in prolactin levels during the precarrying period, but do not have higher levels than nonfathers. C jacchus fathers are the primary carriers, have higher prolactin levels than nonfathers, and show a trend for a prolactin increase after the birth of infants. In conclusion, species differences in the patterns of prolactin secretion were evident and reflect the different paternal roles.


Subject(s)
Aotidae/urine , Callimico/urine , Callithrix/urine , Helping Behavior , Paternal Behavior , Prolactin/urine , Animals , Animals, Newborn , Behavior, Animal , Fathers , Male
8.
Folia Primatol (Basel) ; 73(2-3): 104-15, 2002.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-12207057

ABSTRACT

Duets from a total of 6 female and 6 male titi monkeys (Callicebus cupreus) were analysed in order to provide an in-depth analysis of the duet structure, to investigate whether duets are pair specific and to examine whether duets of newly formed pairs differ from those of established pairs. Callicebus duets are composed of alternately uttered male and female contributions. Duets were pair specific as a result of a summation of individual attributes of the two mates rather than due to unidirectional or reciprocal song adaptation by mates. The two examined newly formed pairs produced species-typical duets from the first day on, but their duets showed greater variability in sequence durations than those of established pairs.


Subject(s)
Cebidae/physiology , Pair Bond , Vocalization, Animal , Animals , Female , Male , Sex Characteristics , Social Behavior , Sound Spectrography , Species Specificity , Tape Recording
9.
Primates ; 43(3): 201-16, 2002 Jul.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-12145401

ABSTRACT

Under captive conditions common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus) show socially monogamous propensities. Male and female form a social bond as characterized by signs of behavioral arousal during separation of the pairmates, high levels of affiliative interactions between pairmates and agonistic responses towards strange conspecifics. In the present study behavioral and cardiophysiological responses of mated individuals of common marmosets were recorded while the animals were in an unfamiliar environment (1) alone, (2) with the pairmate, or (3) with an opposite-sexed stranger. Pairmates of 6 established pairs were tested in 3 replicates yielding a total of 36 trials per experiment. A trial was divided into three 10-min segments (baseline; unfamiliar environment; reunion). Behavioral responses were videotaped with a remote controlled camera system installed within the cage. Systolic (SBP) and diastolic blood pressure (DBP), and heart rate (HR) as well as locomotor activity (ACT) were recorded telemetrically through peritoneally implanted transmitters. The individuals' responses measured while in an unfamiliar environment was only reduced by the pairmate, but not by an opposite-sexed stranger. No affiliative behaviors occurred between strange conspecifics, whereas aggressive and sexual behaviors were observed. During reunion with the pairmate individuals recovered physiologically. The present study shows that an individualized pair bond exists between pairmates of common marmosets. Further, it becomes evident that establishing a social bond with the pairmate is important for maintaining physiological homeostasis.


Subject(s)
Callithrix/psychology , Housing, Animal , Social Behavior , Stress, Psychological , Animals , Animals, Zoo , Blood Pressure , Environment , Female , Heart Rate , Homeostasis , Locomotion , Male
10.
News Physiol Sci ; 14: 223-231, 1999 Dec.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11390856

ABSTRACT

Prolactin has long been known to play a significant role in maternal care. When behavioral endocrinologists began to examine the endocrinology of fatherhood, prolactin was also found to be connected with paternal care in fish, birds, and mammals including primates.

SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...