Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 9 de 9
Filter
Add more filters










Database
Language
Publication year range
1.
Cell ; 179(1): 268-281.e13, 2019 09 19.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31495573

ABSTRACT

Neuronal cell types are the nodes of neural circuits that determine the flow of information within the brain. Neuronal morphology, especially the shape of the axonal arbor, provides an essential descriptor of cell type and reveals how individual neurons route their output across the brain. Despite the importance of morphology, few projection neurons in the mouse brain have been reconstructed in their entirety. Here we present a robust and efficient platform for imaging and reconstructing complete neuronal morphologies, including axonal arbors that span substantial portions of the brain. We used this platform to reconstruct more than 1,000 projection neurons in the motor cortex, thalamus, subiculum, and hypothalamus. Together, the reconstructed neurons constitute more than 85 meters of axonal length and are available in a searchable online database. Axonal shapes revealed previously unknown subtypes of projection neurons and suggest organizational principles of long-range connectivity.


Subject(s)
Brain/cytology , Brain/diagnostic imaging , Neurites/physiology , Pyramidal Tracts/physiology , Animals , Female , Mice , Mice, Inbred C57BL , Mice, Transgenic , Microscopy, Fluorescence, Multiphoton/methods , Software , Transfection
2.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 114(37): 9978-9983, 2017 Sep 12.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28851830

ABSTRACT

From 1980 to 1992, a series of influential papers reported on the discovery, genetics, and evolution of a periodic cycling of the interval between Drosophila male courtship song pulses. The molecular mechanisms underlying this periodicity were never described. To reinitiate investigation of this phenomenon, we previously performed automated segmentation of songs but failed to detect the proposed rhythm [Arthur BJ, et al. (2013) BMC Biol 11:11; Stern DL (2014) BMC Biol 12:38]. Kyriacou et al. [Kyriacou CP, et al. (2017) Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 114:1970-1975] report that we failed to detect song rhythms because (i) our flies did not sing enough and (ii) our segmenter did not identify many of the song pulses. Kyriacou et al. manually annotated a subset of our recordings and reported that two strains displayed rhythms with genotype-specific periodicity, in agreement with their original reports. We cannot replicate this finding and show that the manually annotated data, the original automatically segmented data, and a new dataset provide no evidence for either the existence of song rhythms or song periodicity differences between genotypes. Furthermore, we have reexamined our methods and analysis and find that our automated segmentation method was not biased to prevent detection of putative song periodicity. We conclude that there is no evidence for the existence of Drosophila courtship song rhythms.


Subject(s)
Drosophila melanogaster/physiology , Sexual Behavior, Animal/physiology , Vocalization, Animal/physiology , Animals , Courtship , Drosophila/genetics , Drosophila Proteins/genetics , Drosophila melanogaster/genetics , Female , Male , Singing/physiology
3.
J Exp Biol ; 219(Pt 10): 1437-48, 2016 05 15.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27207951

ABSTRACT

Mice (Mus musculus) form large and dynamic social groups and emit ultrasonic vocalizations in a variety of social contexts. Surprisingly, these vocalizations have been studied almost exclusively in the context of cues from only one social partner, despite the observation that in many social species the presence of additional listeners changes the structure of communication signals. Here, we show that male vocal behavior elicited by female odor is affected by the presence of a male audience - with changes in vocalization count, acoustic structure and syllable complexity. We further show that single sensory cues are not sufficient to elicit this audience effect, indicating that multiple cues may be necessary for an audience to be apparent. Together, these experiments reveal that some features of mouse vocal behavior are only expressed in more complex social situations, and introduce a powerful new assay for measuring detection of the presence of social partners in mice.


Subject(s)
Acoustics , Cues , Social Behavior , Vocalization, Animal/physiology , Anesthesia , Animals , Female , Male , Mice , Odorants
4.
Elife ; 42015 May 28.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26020291

ABSTRACT

During courtship males attract females with elaborate behaviors. In mice, these displays include ultrasonic vocalizations. Ultrasonic courtship vocalizations were previously attributed to the courting male, despite evidence that both sexes produce virtually indistinguishable vocalizations. Because of this similarity, and the difficulty of assigning vocalizations to individuals, the vocal contribution of each individual during courtship is unknown. To address this question, we developed a microphone array system to localize vocalizations from socially interacting, individual adult mice. With this system, we show that female mice vocally interact with males during courtship. Males and females jointly increased their vocalization rates during chases. Furthermore, a female's participation in these vocal interactions may function as a signal that indicates a state of increased receptivity. Our results reveal a novel form of vocal communication during mouse courtship, and lay the groundwork for a mechanistic dissection of communication during social behavior.


Subject(s)
Courtship/psychology , Social Behavior , Ultrasonics/instrumentation , Ultrasonics/methods , Vocalization, Animal/physiology , Animals , Female , Male , Mice , Mice, Inbred C57BL , Sex Factors
5.
J Exp Biol ; 214(Pt 17): 2864-70, 2011 Sep 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21832129

ABSTRACT

The role of sound in Drosophila melanogaster courtship, along with its perception via the antennae, is well established, as is the ability of this fly to learn in classical conditioning protocols. Here, we demonstrate that a neutral acoustic stimulus paired with a sucrose reward can be used to condition the proboscis-extension reflex, part of normal feeding behavior. This appetitive conditioning produces results comparable to those obtained with chemical stimuli in aversive conditioning protocols. We applied a logistic model with general estimating equations to predict the dynamics of learning, which successfully predicts the outcome of training and provides a quantitative estimate of the rate of learning. Use of acoustic stimuli with appetitive conditioning provides both an alternative to models most commonly used in studies of learning and memory in Drosophila and a means of testing hearing in both sexes, independently of courtship responsiveness.


Subject(s)
Acoustic Stimulation , Conditioning, Classical , Drosophila melanogaster/physiology , Feeding Behavior , Acoustic Stimulation/methods , Animals , Appetitive Behavior , Female , Male , Models, Biological
6.
J Exp Biol ; 213(Pt 8): 1376-85, 2010 Apr.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20348350

ABSTRACT

Recent studies demonstrate that mosquitoes listen to each other's wing beats just prior to mating in flight. Field potentials from sound-transducing neurons in the antennae contain both sustained and oscillatory components to pure and paired tone stimuli. Described here is a direct comparison of these two types of response in the dengue vector mosquito, Aedes aegypti. Across a wide range of frequencies and intensities, sustained responses to one- and two-tone stimuli are about equal in magnitude to oscillatory responses to the beats produced by two-tone stimuli. All of these responses are much larger than the oscillatory responses to one-tone stimuli. Similarly, the frequency range extends up to at least the fifth harmonic of the male flight tone for sustained responses to one- and two-tone stimuli and oscillatory responses at the beat frequency of two-tone stimuli, whereas the range of oscillatory response to a one-tone stimulus is limited to, at most, the third harmonic. Thresholds near the fundamental of the flight tone are lower for oscillatory responses than for sustained deflections, lower for males than for females, and within the behaviorally relevant range. A simple model of the transduction process can qualitatively account for both oscillatory and sustained responses to pure and paired tones. These data leave open the question as to which of several alternative strategies underlie flight tone matching behavior in mosquitoes.


Subject(s)
Acoustic Stimulation , Aedes/physiology , Hearing/physiology , Neurons/physiology , Pitch Perception/physiology , Animals , Dengue/transmission , Female , Flight, Animal/physiology , Humans , Insect Vectors , Male , Sound , Wings, Animal/physiology
7.
Science ; 323(5917): 1077-9, 2009 Feb 20.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19131593

ABSTRACT

The familiar buzz of flying mosquitoes is an important mating signal, with the fundamental frequency of the female's flight tone signaling her presence. In the yellow fever and dengue vector Aedes aegypti, both sexes interact acoustically by shifting their flight tones to match, resulting in a courtship duet. Matching is made not at the fundamental frequency of 400 hertz (female) or 600 hertz (male) but at a shared harmonic of 1200 hertz, which exceeds the previously known upper limit of hearing in mosquitoes. Physiological recordings from Johnston's organ (the mosquito's "ear") reveal sensitivity up to 2000 hertz, consistent with our observed courtship behavior. These findings revise widely accepted limits of acoustic behavior in mosquitoes.


Subject(s)
Aedes/physiology , Animal Communication , Insect Vectors/physiology , Sexual Behavior, Animal , Animals , Auditory Perception , Dengue/transmission , Evoked Potentials , Female , Flight, Animal , Hearing , Male , Pitch Perception , Sense Organs/physiology , Wings, Animal/physiology
8.
J Acoust Soc Am ; 120(3): 1546-9, 2006 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17004476

ABSTRACT

The parasitic fly Ormia ochracea localizes its host, field crickets, by homing in on their calling song. Previous phonotactic studies indicate that their sound localization ability in azimuth is extraordinarily acute, but the fly's ability to localize the elevation of sound sources has not been tested to date. Here we show that in a freely-walking closed-loop Y-maze task elevational performance is well above chance, but slightly below the fly's performance in azimuth. Immobilizing the head or the halteres (sensory organs of balance) slightly lowered elevational discrimination, but performance was still well above chance. Because ormiine ears are thought to be symmetric and the pure-tone models of a cricket's call used in these experiments contained little to no spectral bandwidth, additional studies will be needed to elucidate the underlying cues. Nonetheless, it is clear that while walking, Ormia flies are capable of distinguishing sound sources that differ only in elevation.


Subject(s)
Behavior, Animal/physiology , Diptera/physiology , Sound Localization/physiology , Vocalization, Animal , Animals , Feeding Behavior/physiology , Female , Flight, Animal/physiology , Gryllidae , Motor Activity/physiology , Space Perception/physiology
9.
J Comp Neurol ; 492(1): 110-21, 2005 Nov 07.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16175562

ABSTRACT

Behavioral studies in barn owls indicate that both the optic tectum (OT) and the auditory arcopallium (AAr) mediate sound localization through the presence of neurons that respond only when sound comes from a circumscribed direction in space. The early stages of the computations leading to these so-called space-specific neurons are shared in a common brainstem pathway, which then splits at the level of the inferior colliculus (IC) such that the last computational stage is thought to be duplicated. The study presented here addresses whether the space-specific neurons in OT and AAr are indeed partially independent of each other by using anatomical methods more precise than those used in previous studies. Specifically, projection neurons in IC were retrogradely labelled with injections of fluorescein- and rhodamine-conjugated dextran amines into OT and nucleus ovoidalis (OV), the thalamic nucleus leading to AAr. By labelling the OT-projecting and OV-projecting neurons in the same owl, it was confirmed that neurons in IC project to either OV or OT but not both. However, although a segregation was generally observed between the medially positioned OV-projecting neurons and the laterally positioned OT-projecting neurons, there was also a slight overlap between the two populations. Moreover, electrolytic lesions demarcating physiological tuning properties indicate that many OV-projecting neurons are within the area containing space-specific neurons. These results highlight the need for more detailed studies elucidating the microcircuitry and corresponding physiology of IC, such as have been done in the cortices of the mammalian cerebellum and cerebrum.


Subject(s)
Inferior Colliculi/cytology , Neural Pathways/cytology , Strigiformes/anatomy & histology , Superior Colliculi/cytology , Thalamus/cytology , Animals , Auditory Perception/physiology , Inferior Colliculi/physiology , Neural Pathways/physiology , Neurons/cytology , Neurons/physiology , Staining and Labeling , Strigiformes/physiology , Superior Colliculi/physiology , Thalamus/physiology
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...