Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 20 de 47
Filter
1.
Q J Exp Psychol (Hove) ; 72(2): 238-250, 2019 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28856980

ABSTRACT

Perceptions of the effectiveness of a moderate probabilistic cause are influenced by the presence of stronger alternative causes. One important idea is that this influence occurs because the strong cause renders the weaker one statistically redundant. Alternatively, the causes might be contrasted to each other, so the stronger cause may simply overpower perceptions of the weaker one. Causes may have the same polarity (e.g., two generative/excitatory causes or two preventive/inhibitory causes) or be of opposite polarity (e.g., a generative cause versus a preventive or inhibitory cause). Previously, we found that the presence of a stronger redundant alternative cause of the same polarity reduces causal judgements of the moderate cause (i.e., blocking occurs) but a stronger cause of the opposite polarity enhances judgements of the moderate cause (i.e., enhancement). Experiments 1 and 2 further explored these cue competition effects with redundant and non-redundant alternative causes (i.e., correlated versus independent alternatives). We generally found that blocking and enhancement occur with both redundant and non-redundant alternative causes. This is inconsistent with an information processing view of cue competition that relies on statistical redundancy to account for blocking. Although these results are inconsistent with a redundancy information processing account of cue competition and are consistent with our earlier contrast account, we demonstrate here that a simple associative model can account for the sometimes apparently contradictory effects of cue competition.


Subject(s)
Association Learning/physiology , Cues , Thinking/physiology , Visual Perception/physiology , Adult , Female , Humans , Male , Young Adult
2.
J Intell ; 6(1)2018 Feb 27.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31162434

ABSTRACT

Previous experience with discrimination problems that can only be solved by learning about stimulus configurations enhances performance on new configural discriminations. Some of these effects can be explained by a shift toward increased configural processing (learning about combinations of cues rather than about individual elements), or by a tendency to generalize a learned rule to a new training set. We investigated whether fluid abilities influence the extent that previous experience with configural discriminations improves performance on subsequent discriminations. In Experiments 1 and 2 we used patterning discriminations that could be solved by applying a simple rule, whereas in Experiment 3 we used biconditional discriminations that could not be solved using a rule. Fluid abilities predicted the improvement on the second training set in all experiments, including Experiment 3 in which rule-based generalization could not explain the improvement on the second discrimination. This supports the idea that fluid abilities contribute to performance by inducing a shift toward configural processing rather than rule-based generalization. However, fluid abilities also predicted performance on a rule-based transfer test in Experiment 2. Taken together, these results suggest that fluid abilities contribute to both a flexible shift toward configural processing and to rule-based generalization.

3.
Behav Brain Sci ; 37(5): 494-5, 2014 Oct.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25388041

ABSTRACT

We argue that the claim that essence-based causal explanations emerge, hydra-like, from an inherence heuristic is incomplete. No plausible mechanism for the transition from concrete properties, or cues, to essences is provided. Moreover, the fundamental shotgun and storytelling mechanisms of the inherence heuristic are not clearly enough specified to distinguish them, developmentally, from associative or causal networks.


Subject(s)
Cognition , Concept Formation , Learning , Logic , Humans
4.
Exp Psychol ; 61(5): 356-67, 2014.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24614872

ABSTRACT

Many theories of causal learning and causal induction differ in their assumptions about how people combine the causal impact of several causes presented in compound. Some theories propose that when several causes are present, their joint causal impact is equal to the linear sum of the individual impact of each cause. However, some recent theories propose that the causal impact of several causes needs to be combined by means of a noisy-OR integration rule. In other words, the probability of the effect given several causes would be equal to the sum of the probability of the effect given each cause in isolation minus the overlap between those probabilities. In the present series of experiments, participants were given information about the causal impact of several causes and then they were asked what compounds of those causes they would prefer to use if they wanted to produce the effect. The results of these experiments suggest that participants actually use a variety of strategies, including not only the linear and the noisy-OR integration rules, but also averaging the impact of several causes.


Subject(s)
Cognition/physiology , Learning/physiology , Social Behavior , Visual Perception/physiology , Adolescent , Child , Comprehension , Humans , Probability , Problem Solving , Theory of Mind
5.
Q J Exp Psychol (Hove) ; 67(2): 281-303, 2014 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23777427

ABSTRACT

A theory or model of cause such as Cheng's power (p) allows people to predict the effectiveness of a cause in a different causal context from the one in which they observed its actions. Liljeholm and Cheng demonstrated that people could detect differences in the effectiveness of the cause when causal power varied across contexts of different outcome base rates, but that they did not detect similar changes when only the cause-outcome contingency, ∆p, but not power, varied. However, their procedure allowed participants to simplify the causal scenarios and consider only a subsample of observations with a base rate of zero. This confounds p, ∆p, and the probability of an outcome (O) given a cause (C), P(O|C). Furthermore, the contingencies that they used confounded p and P(O|C) in the overall sample. Following the work of Liljeholm and Cheng, we examined whether causal induction in a wider range of situations follows the principles suggested by Cheng. Experiments 1a and 1b compared the procedure used by Liljeholm and Cheng with one that did not allow the sample of observations to be simplified. Experiments 2a and 2b compared the same two procedures using contingencies that controlled for P(O|C). The results indicated that, if the possibility of converting all contexts to a zero base rate situation was avoided, people were sensitive to changes in P(O|C), p, and ∆p when each of these was varied. This is inconsistent with Liljeholm and Cheng's conclusion that people detect only changes in p. These results question the idea that people naturally extract the metric or model of cause from their observation of stochastic events and then, reasonably exclusively, use this theory of a causal mechanism, or for that matter any simple normative theory, to generalize their experience to alternative contexts.


Subject(s)
Association Learning/physiology , Causality , Generalization, Psychological/physiology , Models, Statistical , Female , Humans , Male , Models, Psychological , Predictive Value of Tests , Students , Universities
6.
Q J Exp Psychol (Hove) ; 65(9): 1675-98, 2012.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22512279

ABSTRACT

We tested whether preventive and generative reasoning processes are symmetrical by keeping the training and testing of preventive (inhibitory) and generative (excitatory) causal cues as similar as possible. In Experiment 1, we extinguished excitors and inhibitors in a blocking design, in which each extinguished cause was presented in compound with a novel cause, with the same outcome occurring following the compound and following the novel cause alone. With this novel extinction procedure, the inhibitory cues seemed more likely to lose their properties than the excitatory cues. In Experiment 2, we investigated blocking of excitatory and inhibitory causes and found similar blocking effects. Taken together, these results suggest that acquisition of excitation and inhibition is similar, but that inhibition is more liable to extinguish with our extinction procedure. In addition, we used a variable outcome, and this enabled us to test the predictions of an inferential reasoning account about what happens when the outcome level is at its minimum or maximum (De Houwer, Beckers, & Glautier, 2002). We discuss the predictions of this inferential account, Rescorla and Wagner's (1972) model, and a connectionist model-the auto-associator.


Subject(s)
Association Learning/physiology , Cues , Extinction, Psychological , Inhibition, Psychological , Female , Humans , Male
7.
Learn Behav ; 38(4): 394-407, 2010 Nov.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21048230

ABSTRACT

Two experiments investigated extinction and blocking of a conditioned inhibitor in a human contingency-learning task. Lotz and Lachnit (2009) and Melchers, Wolff, and Lachnit (2006) reported extinction of inhibition only when participants experienced outcome levels lower than those used in training. In Experiment 1, which used more neutral instructions than the previously mentioned studies, we found that extinction of inhibition occurred, whether or not participants experienced lower outcome levels. In Experiment 2, we applied this outcome manipulation to blocking of a conditioned inhibitor. We found blocking of inhibition both when participants had experience with lower outcomes and when they did not. The results of our two experiments are consistent with Rescorla and Wagner's (1972) associative model, and inconsistent with an inferential account of causal learning (De Houwer, Beckers, & Vandorpe, 2005).


Subject(s)
Association Learning , Conditioning, Psychological , Extinction, Psychological , Inhibition, Psychological , Cues , Feedback, Psychological , Female , Food Preferences/psychology , Hormones/blood , Humans , Male , Problem Solving
8.
J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process ; 35(2): 153-68, 2009 Apr.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19364225

ABSTRACT

Three experiments investigated the way participants construct causal chains from experience with the individual links that make up those chains. Participants were presented with contingency information about the relationship between events A and B, as well as events B and C, using trial-by-trial presentations. The A-B and B-C contingencies could be positive, negative, or zero. Although participants had never experienced A and C together, A-C ratings were a multiplicative function of the A-B and B-C contingencies. These findings can be generated by an auto-associator using the delta rule. This explanation is also useful for understanding sensory preconditioning and second-order conditioning.


Subject(s)
Association Learning/physiology , Causality , Discrimination, Psychological/physiology , Judgment/physiology , Bayes Theorem , Choice Behavior/physiology , Concept Formation/physiology , Female , Humans , Male , Psychological Theory
9.
Learn Behav ; 33(2): 160-71, 2005 May.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16075836

ABSTRACT

The relative validity effect (Wagner, Logan, Haberlandt, & Price, 1968) demonstrated that a strong cue or cause reduces responding to, or judgments of, a weaker cue or cause. We report two experiments with human subjects using relative validity preparations in which we investigate one- and two-cue competition effects. Previously, we investigated the effect using instrumental and Pavlovian conditioning preparations with rats. In the first experiment, we used a procedure analogous to the animal preparations. In the second experiment, we used a different probabilistic procedure. The results with humans and rats are very similar. In each species we find similar interference with processing the moderate predictor with one or with two strong competitors. These results are not well predicted by most associative models.


Subject(s)
Association Learning , Mental Processes , Models, Psychological , Adult , Animals , Female , Humans , Male , Rats
10.
Q J Exp Psychol B ; 58(2): 177-92, 2005 Apr.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16095045

ABSTRACT

In cause-outcome contingency judgement tasks, judgements often reflect the actual contingency but are also influenced by the overall probability of the outcome, P(O). Action-outcome instrumental learning tasks can foster a pattern in which judgements of positive contingencies become less positive as P(O) increases. Variable contiguity between the action and the outcome may produce this bias. Experiment 1 recorded judgements of positive contingencies that were largely uninfluenced by P(O) using an immediate contiguity procedure. Experiment 2 directly compared variable versus constant contiguity. The predicted interaction between contiguity and P(O) was observed for positive contingencies. These results stress the sensitivity of the causal learning mechanism to temporal contiguity.


Subject(s)
Association Learning , Conditioning, Operant , Judgment , Probability Learning , Humans , Likelihood Functions , Pattern Recognition, Visual , Psychomotor Performance
11.
J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process ; 30(3): 229-39, 2004 Jul.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15279513

ABSTRACT

Two experiments evaluated the role of conditioned stimulus-unconditioned stimulus (CS-US) contingency in appetitive Pavlovian conditioning in rats. In both experiments, some groups received a positively contingent CS signaling an increased likelihood of the US relative to the absence of the CS. These groups were compared with control treatments in which the likelihood of the US was the same in the presence and absence of the CS. A trial marker served as a trial context. Experiment 1 found contingency sensitivity. There was a reciprocal relationship between responding to the CS and the trial marker. Experiment 2 showed that this result was not stimulus or response specific. These results are consistent with associative explanations and the idea that rats are sensitive to CS-US contingency.


Subject(s)
Association Learning , Conditioning, Classical , Animals , Male , Rats , Rats, Wistar , Reinforcement Schedule , Time Factors
12.
Q J Exp Psychol B ; 56(1): 90-101, 2003 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-12623540

ABSTRACT

In 1973 Mackintosh reported an interference effect that he called learned irrelevance in which exposure to uncorrelated (CS/US) presentation of the unconditional stimulus (US) and the conditioned stimulus (CS) interfered with future Pavlovian conditioning. It has been argued that there is no specific interference effect in learned irrelevance; rather the interference is the sum of independent CS and US exposure effects (CS + US). We review previous research on this question and report two new experiments. We conclude that learned irrelevance is a consequence of a contingency learning and a specific learned irrelevance mechanism. Moreover even the independent exposure controls, used in previous experiments to support the CS and US exposure account, provide support for the correlation learning process.


Subject(s)
Association Learning , Animals , Behavior, Animal , Conditioning, Classical , Rats , Rats, Wistar
13.
Behav Modif ; 25(3): 471-86, 2001 Jul.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11428249

ABSTRACT

The authors evaluated a brief intervention for increasing seat belt use among the front seat occupants of cars at a junior college, in a jurisdiction with a mandatory belt use law. The intervention included public posting of performance feedback and distribution of an informational flyer to cars in target parking lot. Feedback was the display of the proportion of drivers observed wearing seat belts on the previous observation day. Seat belt use among drivers increased from 64% during the baseline phase to 71% during the intervention phase. Seat belt use among front passengers increased from 49% during the baseline phase to 67% during the intervention phase. In both cases, seat belt use at follow-up was comparable to seat belt use during the intervention phase, although a trend toward decreasing belt use was noted. Also found was higher seat belt use among females as compared with males irrespective of their front seat occupant status (driver or passenger). Effects of the intervention are discussed in the context of increasing seat belt use in a hardcore nonuser population of predominantly young adults.


Subject(s)
Behavior Therapy , Psychotherapy, Brief , Seat Belts/legislation & jurisprudence , Students/psychology , Feedback , Humans , Seat Belts/statistics & numerical data , Students/statistics & numerical data
14.
J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process ; 27(2): 137-52, 2001 Apr.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11296489

ABSTRACT

Contextual conditioning during relative validity training was explored in 3 experiments that used an appetitive Pavlovian conditioning preparation with rats. Magazine entries were the conditioned response. In Experiment 1, true-discrimination (TD: AX+, BX-) training generated weaker conditioning of X than did pseudodiscrimination (PD: AX+/-, BX+/-) training. The context showed a similar relative validity effect. Also, both PD training and simple partial reinforcement (X+/-) reduced contextual conditioning more than did unsignaled food, a demonstration of relative validity using partial reinforcement. Experiments 2 and 3 used within-subject and between-subjects designs, respectively, and showed that relative validity was determined by the summation of differences in conditioning to both the common element (X) and the context. Our results are consistent with an attentional model or with a computational comparator model but not with the Rescorla-Wagner (R. A. Rescorla & A. R. Wagner, 1972) model.


Subject(s)
Cues , Animals , Conditioning, Classical/physiology , Discrimination, Psychological , Male , Random Allocation , Rats , Rats, Wistar , Reinforcement, Psychology , Reproducibility of Results
15.
J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process ; 27(1): 59-67, 2001 Jan.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11199515

ABSTRACT

Two experiments investigated the relative validity effect with either 1 or 2 continuously reinforced cues in Wistar rats using appetitive Pavlovian and instrumental preparations. Discrimination training involved 3 compound cues containing a common element (1AX: 1BX: 2CX). In the first true-discrimination group (TD-1), CX was followed by food, but AX and BX were not. In the second true-discrimination group (TD-2), AX and BX but not CX were followed by food. In the third, pseudodiscrimination group (PD), food followed 50% of each compound. Compared with the PD group, there were lower levels of responding to X in Groups TD-1 and TD-2, which did not differ. That is, both TD treatments showed equivalent relative validity effects. There was evidence for a relative validity effect on the context. The Rescorla-Wagner model incorrectly predicts a smaller relative validity effect after the TD-2 than the TD-1 treatment. Comparator theory predicts these results.


Subject(s)
Conditioning, Classical , Discrimination Learning , Animals , Conditioning, Operant , Male , Rats , Rats, Wistar , Reproducibility of Results
16.
J Chromatogr A ; 887(1-2): 379-91, 2000 Jul 28.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10961328

ABSTRACT

Macroporous, monolithic capillary electrochromatography (CEC) columns, featuring a hydrophobic stationary phase, have been applied to the separations of steroids with good column efficiency. Using isocratic and gradient elution runs, mixtures of neutral or conjugated steroids could be resolved. While dansylated ketosteroids were detectable through laser-induced fluorescence at attomole levels, the CEC columns coupled to electrospray-ion-trap mass spectrometry featured femtomole detection limits.


Subject(s)
Electrophoresis, Capillary/methods , Steroids/analysis , Female , Humans , Lasers , Mass Spectrometry/methods , Osmolar Concentration , Pregnancy , Reference Standards , Spectrometry, Fluorescence/methods , Steroids/urine
17.
Anal Chem ; 72(13): 2703-10, 2000 Jul 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10905296

ABSTRACT

Different macroporous, monolithic capillary columns were prepared to separate various bile acid mixtures through capillary electrochromatography (CEC) at high efficiency. These columns are shown to be ideally suitable for coupling to an electrospray ionization/ion trap mass spectrometer. Detection and structural identification of different bile acid derivatives in either the positive- or negative-ion mode necessitated column technologies with different polarities and the capabilities of a reversed electroosmotic flow. High column efficiencies (610,000 theoretical plates/meter for glycocholic acid in normal-phase separation) were preserved in the coupling to mass spectrometry (MS), with the detection limits of approximately 40 femtomole (for cholic acid) and identification through CEC/MS/MS.


Subject(s)
Bile Acids and Salts/analysis , Electrochemistry , Electrophoresis, Capillary , Glycine/analysis , Mass Spectrometry , Taurine/analysis
18.
Mem Cognit ; 28(3): 466-79, 2000 Apr.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10881563

ABSTRACT

We report three experiments in which we tested asymptotic and dynamic predictions of the Rescorla-Wagner (R-W) model and the asymptotic predictions of Cheng's probabilistic contrast model (PCM) concerning judgments of causality when there are two possible causal candidates. We used a paradigm in which the presence of a causal candidate that is highly correlated with an effect influences judgments of a second, moderately correlated or uncorrelated cause. In Experiment 1, which involved a moderate outcome density, judgments of a moderately positive cause were attenuated when it was paired with either a perfect positive or perfect negative cause. This attenuation was robust over a large set of trials but was greater when the strong predictor was positive. In Experiment 2, in which there was a low overall density of outcomes, judgments of a moderately correlated positive cause were elevated when this cause was paired with a perfect negative causal candidate. This elevation was also quite robust over a large set of trials. In Experiment 3, estimates of the strength of a causal candidate that was uncorrelated with the outcome were reduced when it was paired with a perfect cause. The predictions of three theoretical models of causal judgments are considered. Both the R-W model and Cheng's PCM accounted for some but not all aspects of the data. Pearce's model of stimulus generalization accounts for a greater proportion of the data.


Subject(s)
Causality , Concept Formation , Judgment , Probability Learning , Adult , Female , Humans , Male , Models, Psychological
19.
Anal Chem ; 71(14): 2945-50, 1999 Jul 15.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10424178

ABSTRACT

Narrow-bandwidth signals were applied to the end caps of an ion trap mass spectrometer to excite ions during collisional activation. Excitation waveforms were created from a single-frequency component and a random noise component using a multiplier circuit. Tandem and higher order mass spectrometry experiments (MS3) can be performed without optimization of the supplemental frequency applied to the end cap electrodes. The usefulness of this method of ion excitation is demonstrated using singly and multiply protonated peptide ions as well as sodium-cationized carbohydrates.


Subject(s)
Mass Spectrometry/methods , Amino Acid Sequence , Angiotensin II/chemistry , Carbohydrate Sequence , Molecular Sequence Data , Oligosaccharides/chemistry
20.
J Am Soc Mass Spectrom ; 10(7): 613-24, 1999 Jul.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10384725

ABSTRACT

Small molecules formed during lipid peroxidation can react with the basic groups in proteins through different mechanisms. Recently, substituted pyridinium moieties were observed during in vitro incubations of lysine-containing peptides with 2-alkenals. To explore the dissociation behavior of peptides with pyridinium-derivatized lysine residues, the peptide ions created through either matrix-assisted laser desorption/ionization or electrospray ionization were studied with tandem mass spectrometry. The permanently charged pyridinium ions fragment primarily through the charge-remote processes. Under high energy collision-induced dissociation, a number of diagnostic ions were observed that could potentially be used to identify modified residues in proteins. The origins of these ions were studied using deuterium exchange and higher-order mass spectrometry experiments using an ion trap instrument. Rational structures for these ions are proposed.


Subject(s)
Aldehydes/chemistry , Lipid Peroxidation , Peptides/chemistry , Cross-Linking Reagents , Dipeptides/chemistry , Histidine/chemistry , Indicators and Reagents , Mass Spectrometry , Models, Chemical , Pyridinium Compounds/chemistry , Spectrometry, Mass, Matrix-Assisted Laser Desorption-Ionization
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...