Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 12 de 12
Filter
1.
Neuropsychol Rehabil ; 32(1): 1-21, 2022 Jan.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32677539

ABSTRACT

Despite evidence of the efficacy of telerehabilitation post-stroke, uptake has lagged behind the development of available technology, slowed by low confidence in user experience. We aimed to explore the issue of acceptability by characterizing the experience of telerehabilitation for service providers and consumers of a memory rehabilitation program. Semi-structured interviews were conducted with nine clinicians (Mage = 33.29; SD = 7.37; range 25-44) and 25 stroke survivors with memory difficulties (Mage = 54.12; SD = 10.99; range 36-82) following a 6-week compensatory memory skills program delivered via in-home videoconferencing. Thematic analysis identified a range of perspectives under three overarching themes: (1) Content and Delivery; (2) Relationship and Connection; and (3) The Role and Benefits of Telerehabilitation. Reports from both respondent groups were overwhelmingly positive about the telerehabilitation experience and identified the benefits of increasing service availability. Videoconferencing technology was usable and occasional reliability issues were not a barrier. Users identified a range of benefits and challenges regarding communication, rapport building, and integration of rehabilitation in the home environment. Furthermore, most stroke survivors reported benefit from the intervention, via the acquisition of memory strategies and improved self-confidence through better understanding of their deficits. Overall, telerehabilitation of memory was acceptable to users.


Subject(s)
Stroke Rehabilitation , Telemedicine , Telerehabilitation , Adult , Home Environment , Humans , Middle Aged , Reproducibility of Results
2.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33036417

ABSTRACT

Identifying who might develop disabling pain or poor mental health after injury is a high priority so that healthcare providers can provide targeted preventive interventions. This retrospective cohort study aimed to identify predictors of disabling pain or probable mental health conditions at 12 months post-injury. Participants were recruited 12-months after admission to a major trauma service for a compensable transport or workplace injury (n = 157). Injury, compensation claim, health services and medication information were obtained from the Victorian Orthopaedic Trauma Outcome Registry, Victorian State Trauma Registry and Compensation Research Database. Participants completed questionnaires about pain, and mental health (anxiety, depression, posttraumatic stress disorder) at 12 months post-injury. One third had disabling pain, one third had at least one probable mental health condition and more than one in five had both disabling pain and a mental health condition at 12 months post-injury. Multivariable logistic regression found mental health treatment 3-6 months post-injury, persistent work disability and opioid use at 6-12 months predicted disabling pain at 12 months post-injury. The presence of opioid use at 3-6 months, work disability and psychotropic medications at 6-12 months predicted a mental health condition at 12 months post-injury. These factors could be used to identify at risk of developing disabling pain who could benefit from timely interventions to better manage both pain and mental health post-injury. Implications for healthcare and compensation system are discussed.


Subject(s)
Disability Evaluation , Mental Health , Pain , Female , Humans , Male , Prognosis , Quality of Life , Retrospective Studies
3.
Pain ; 159(4): 644-655, 2018 04.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29447133

ABSTRACT

Chronic pain is associated with reduced efficiency of cognitive performance, and few studies have investigated methods of remediation. We trialled a computerised cognitive training protocol to determine whether it could attenuate cognitive difficulties in a chronic pain sample. Thirty-nine adults with chronic pain (mean age = 43.3, 61.5% females) were randomised to an 8-week online course (3 sessions/week from home) of game-like cognitive training exercises, or an active control involving watching documentary videos. Participants received weekly supervision by video call. Primary outcomes were a global neurocognitive composite (tests of attention, speed, and executive function) and self-reported cognition. Secondary outcomes were pain (intensity; interference), mood symptoms (depression; anxiety), and coping with pain (catastrophising; self-efficacy). Thirty participants (15 training and 15 control) completed the trial. Mixed model intention-to-treat analyses revealed significant effects of training on the global neurocognitive composite (net effect size [ES] = 0.43, P = 0.017), driven by improved executive function performance (attention switching and working memory). The control group reported improvement in pain intensity (net ES = 0.65, P = 0.022). Both groups reported subjective improvements in cognition (ES = 0.28, P = 0.033) and catastrophising (ES = 0.55, P = 0.006). Depression, anxiety, self-efficacy, and pain interference showed no change in either group. This study provides preliminary evidence that supervised cognitive training may be a viable method for enhancing cognitive skills in persons with chronic pain, but transfer to functional and clinical outcomes remains to be demonstrated. Active control results suggest that activities perceived as relaxing or enjoyable contribute to improved perception of well-being. Weekly contact was pivotal to successful program completion.


Subject(s)
Chronic Pain/complications , Cognition Disorders/etiology , Cognition Disorders/rehabilitation , Cognitive Behavioral Therapy/methods , Therapy, Computer-Assisted/methods , Adaptation, Psychological , Adult , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Neuropsychological Tests , Online Systems , Pain Clinics , Pain Measurement , Prospective Studies , Psychiatric Status Rating Scales , Retrospective Studies , Self Report , Single-Blind Method , Treatment Outcome
4.
BMJ Open ; 7(10): e017350, 2017 Oct 05.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28982828

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVES: Compensable injury increases the likelihood of having persistent pain after injury. Three-quarters of patients report chronic pain after traumatic injury, which is disabling for about one-third of patients. It is important to understand why these patients report disabling pain, in order to develop targeted preventative interventions. This study examined the experience of pain and disability, and investigated their sequential interrelationships with, catastrophising, kinesiophobia and self-efficacy 1 year after compensable and non-compensable injury. DESIGN: Observational registry-based cohort study. SETTING: Metropolitan Trauma Service in Melbourne, Victoria, Australia. PARTICIPANTS: Participants were recruited from the Victorian State Trauma Registry and Victorian Orthopaedic Trauma Outcomes Registry. 732 patients were referred to the study, 82 could not be contacted or were ineligible, 217 declined and 433 participated (66.6% response rate). OUTCOME MEASURES: The Brief Pain Inventory, Glasgow Outcome Scale, EuroQol Five Dimensions questionnaire, Pain Catastrophising Scale, Pain Self-Efficacy Questionnaire, Injustice Experience Questionnaire and the Tampa Scale of Kinesiophobia. METHODS: Direct and indirect relationships (via psychological appraisals of pain/injury) between baseline characteristics (compensation, fault and injury characteristics) and pain severity, pain interference, health status and disability were examined with ordinal, linear and logistic regression, and mediation analyses. RESULTS: Injury severity, compensable injury and external fault attribution were consistently associated with moderate-to-severe pain, higher pain interference, poorer health status and moderate-to-severe disability. The association between compensable injury, or external fault attribution, and disability and health outcomes was mediated via pain self-efficacy and perceived injustice. CONCLUSIONS: Given that the associations between compensable injury, pain and disability was attributable to lower self-efficacy and higher perceptions of injustice, interventions targeting the psychological impacts of pain and injury may be especially necessary to improve long-term injury outcomes.


Subject(s)
Compensation and Redress , Disability Evaluation , Disabled Persons/psychology , Pain/psychology , Wounds and Injuries/psychology , Adolescent , Adult , Cohort Studies , Cross-Sectional Studies , Female , Humans , Linear Models , Logistic Models , Male , Middle Aged , Pain Measurement , Quality of Life/psychology , Registries , Surveys and Questionnaires , Trauma Severity Indices , Victoria , Wounds and Injuries/economics , Young Adult
5.
Clin J Pain ; 33(5): 462-472, 2017 05.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28379916

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVES: Cognitive functioning is commonly disrupted in people living with chronic pain, yet it is an aspect of pain that is often not routinely assessed in pain management settings, and there is a paucity of research on treatments or strategies to alleviate the problem. The purpose of this review is to outline recent research on cognitive deficits seen in chronic pain, to give an overview of the mechanisms involved, advocate cognitive functioning as an important target for treatment in pain populations, and discuss ways in which it may be assessed and potentially remediated. METHODS: A narrative review. RESULTS: There are several options for remediation, including compensatory, restorative, and neuromodulatory approaches to directly modify cognitive functioning, as well as physical, psychological, and medication optimization methods to target secondary factors (mood, sleep, and medications) that may interfere with cognition. DISCUSSION: We highlight the potential to enhance cognitive functions and identify the major gaps in the research literature.


Subject(s)
Chronic Pain/psychology , Cognition , Chronic Pain/diagnosis , Chronic Pain/therapy , Humans
6.
Pain Med ; 17(8): 1497-504, 2016 08.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26814280

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVE: Prolonged exposure to opioids is known to produce neuroplastic changes in animals; however, few studies have investigated the effects of short-term prescription opioid use in humans. A previous study from our laboratory demonstrated a dosage-correlated volumetric decrease in the right amygdala of participants administered oral morphine daily for 1 month. The purpose of this current study was to replicate and extend the initial findings. METHODS: Twenty-one participants with chronic low back pain were enrolled in this double-blind, placebo-controlled study. Participants were randomized to receive daily morphine (n = 11) or a matched placebo (n = 10) for 1 month. High-resolution anatomical images were acquired immediately before and after the treatment administration period. Morphological gray matter changes were investigated using tensor-based morphometry, and significant regions were subsequently tested for correlation with morphine dosage. RESULTS: Decreased gray matter volume was observed in several reward- and pain-related regions in the morphine group, including the bilateral amygdala, left inferior orbitofrontal cortex, and bilateral pre-supplementary motor areas. Morphine administration was also associated with significant gray matter increases in cingulate regions, including the mid cingulate, dorsal anterior cingulate, and ventral posterior cingulate. CONCLUSIONS: Many of the volumetric increases and decreases overlapped spatially with the previously reported changes. Individuals taking placebo for 1 month showed neither gray matter increases nor decreases. The results corroborate previous reports that rapid alterations occur in reward-related networks following short-term prescription opioid use.


Subject(s)
Amygdala/drug effects , Analgesics, Opioid/adverse effects , Gray Matter/drug effects , Low Back Pain/drug therapy , Adult , Amygdala/pathology , Double-Blind Method , Female , Gray Matter/pathology , Humans , Image Interpretation, Computer-Assisted , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Male , Middle Aged , Morphine/adverse effects
7.
Pain ; 157(1): 7-29, 2016 Jan.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26431423

ABSTRACT

Both sympathetic and parasympathetic nervous systems are involved in regulating pain states. The activity of these systems seems to become disturbed in states of chronic pain. This disruption in autonomic balance can be measured through the assessment of heart rate variability (HRV), that is, the variability of the interval between consecutive heart beats. However, there is yet to be a systematic evaluation of the body of literature concerning HRV across several chronic pain conditions. Moreover, modern meta-analytical techniques have never been used to validate and consolidate the extent to which HRV may be decreased in chronic pain. Following the preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses (PRISMA) statement guidelines, this study systematically evaluated and critically appraised the literature concerning HRV in people living with chronic pain. After screening 17,350 sources, 51 studies evaluating HRV in a chronic pain group met the inclusion criteria. Twenty-six moderate-high quality studies were included in quantitative meta-analyses. On average, the quality of studies was moderate. There were 6 frequency-domain and time-domain measures of HRV across a broad range of chronic pain conditions. High heterogeneity aside, pooled results from the meta-analyses reflected a consistent, moderate-to-large effect of decreased high-frequency HRV in chronic pain, implicating a decrease in parasympathetic activation. These effects were heavily influenced by fibromyalgia studies. Future research would benefit from wider use of standardised definitions of measurement, and also investigating the synergistic changes in pain state and HRV throughout the development and implementation of mechanism-based treatments for chronic pain.


Subject(s)
Chronic Pain/physiopathology , Heart Rate/physiology , Parasympathetic Nervous System/physiopathology , Fibromyalgia/physiopathology , Humans
8.
Clin J Pain ; 32(8): 673-80, 2016 08.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26626294

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVES: People with chronic pain may experience impairments in high-level cognitive skills, particularly executive functions. Such impairments are not adequately measured in most clinical pain management settings yet could be a key influence on everyday functioning. We administered a well-validated, well-normed self-report measure to determine which aspects of executive functioning are compromised in the daily experience of patients with chronic pain, and whether these are associated with pain severity, medications, and mood. MATERIALS AND METHODS: Sixty-three patients attending a multidisciplinary pain management clinic, and 66 pain-free age-matched and sex-matched controls, completed the Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function, Adult version (BRIEF-A). The BRIEF-A measures 9 aspects of executive function: Inhibit, Shift, Emotional Control, Initiate, Self-Monitor, Working Memory, Plan/Organize, Task Monitor, and Organization of Materials. Patients completed a battery of mood and pain-related measures. RESULTS: Profile analysis revealed that patients with chronic pain reported significantly greater overall executive function impairments than controls. The patients showed greatest impairments on Working Memory and Emotional Control subscales, with more than half scoring in the clinically elevated range. A significant proportion of the variance in these scores was explained by total medication detriment (but not opioids alone), negative emotional states, and pain interference. Pain intensity and duration were not strong predictors of reported executive dysfunction. DISCUSSION: Multiple factors impact on self-reported executive problems in this population. Specific deficits in Working Memory and Emotional Control have implications for patient engagement with treatment, and retention of information provided in therapy. A screening tool like the BRIEF-A may be useful in pain management settings.


Subject(s)
Affect , Chronic Pain/psychology , Emotional Intelligence , Executive Function , Memory, Short-Term , Adult , Aged , Chronic Pain/drug therapy , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Psychological Tests , Regression Analysis , Self Report , Young Adult
9.
PLoS One ; 8(12): e84001, 2013.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24376780

ABSTRACT

Recent studies have shown that perceiving the pain of others activates brain regions in the observer associated with both somatosensory and affective-motivational aspects of pain, principally involving regions of the anterior cingulate and anterior insula cortex. The degree of these empathic neural responses is modulated by racial bias, such that stronger neural activation is elicited by observing pain in people of the same racial group compared with people of another racial group. The aim of the present study was to examine whether a more general social group category, other than race, could similarly modulate neural empathic responses and perhaps account for the apparent racial bias reported in previous studies. Using a minimal group paradigm, we assigned participants to one of two mixed-race teams. We use the term race to refer to the Chinese or Caucasian appearance of faces and whether the ethnic group represented was the same or different from the appearance of the participant' own face. Using fMRI, we measured neural empathic responses as participants observed members of their own group or other group, and members of their own race or other race, receiving either painful or non-painful touch. Participants showed clear group biases, with no significant effect of race, on behavioral measures of implicit (affective priming) and explicit group identification. Neural responses to observed pain in the anterior cingulate cortex, insula cortex, and somatosensory areas showed significantly greater activation when observing pain in own-race compared with other-race individuals, with no significant effect of minimal groups. These results suggest that racial bias in neural empathic responses is not influenced by minimal forms of group categorization, despite the clear association participants showed with in-group more than out-group members. We suggest that race may be an automatic and unconscious mechanism that drives the initial neural responses to observed pain in others.


Subject(s)
Asian People/psychology , Brain/physiology , Empathy , Pain , White People/psychology , Affect , Female , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Male , Young Adult
10.
J Cogn Neurosci ; 25(11): 1794-806, 2013 Nov.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23767924

ABSTRACT

Interactions between the visual system and the motor system during action observation are important for functions such as imitation and action understanding. Here, we asked whether such processes might be influenced by the cognitive context in which actions are performed. We recorded ERPs in a delayed go/no-go task known to induce bidirectional interference between the motor system and the visual system (visuomotor interference). Static images of hand gestures were presented as go stimuli after participants had planned either a matching (congruent) or nonmatching (incongruent) action. Participants performed the identical task in two different cognitive contexts: In one, they focused on the visual image of the hand gesture shown as the go stimulus (image context), whereas in the other, they focused on the hand gesture they performed (action context). We analyzed the N170 elicited by the go stimulus to test the influence of action plans on action observation (motor-to-visual priming). We also analyzed movement-related activity following the go stimulus to examine the influence of action observation on action planning (visual-to-motor priming). Strikingly, the context manipulation reversed the direction of the priming effects: We found stronger motor-to-visual priming in the action context compared with the image context and stronger visual-to-motor priming in the image context compared with the action context. Taken together, our findings indicate that neural interactions between motor and visual processes for executed and observed actions can change depending on task demands and are sensitive to top-down control according to the context.


Subject(s)
Cognition/physiology , Observation , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Adolescent , Adult , Analysis of Variance , Cues , Electroencephalography , Electrooculography , Evoked Potentials/physiology , Evoked Potentials, Visual/physiology , Female , Functional Laterality/physiology , Gestures , Humans , Male , Motor Cortex/physiology , Movement/physiology , Photic Stimulation , Reaction Time/physiology , Visual Perception/physiology , Young Adult
11.
Neuropsychologia ; 50(5): 715-22, 2012 Apr.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22245011

ABSTRACT

Voluntary, self-initiated actions are preceded by slowly increasing neural activity in pre-motor regions of the brain, beginning up to 2s before the onset of muscle movement. This activity is commonly seen in the scalp-recorded readiness potential, and is an index of movement preparation involving both motor programming and non-motor or cognitive processes such as attention. The specific contribution of cognitive processes, thought to occur during the earliest stage of planning, remains somewhat unclear. We suggest that attention to the timing of movement is a key voluntary process contributing to early-stage cortical activity. As a novel approach to examining this, we recorded EEG throughout a time reproduction task in which participants replicated the interval between two tones with two button-press actions. The first action, i.e. the beginning of the reproduced interval, was somewhat incidental to the task of time reproduction and required minimal attention to the time of initiation, while the second action required explicit attention to the time of initiation. Pre-movement neural activity preceding the first, relatively unattended movement was greatly reduced in amplitude and almost absent in the early stage, in contrast with readiness potentials typically seen prior to voluntary movement. Neural activity preceding explicitly timed movements was significantly larger, with effects emerging in the early component of pre-movement activity over frontal and right frontal scalp regions. We propose that attention to movement timing, i.e. the process of orienting attention in time towards the moment of movement initiation, is a key component of voluntary action preparation that is reflected in the early-stage neural activity we typically see prior to voluntary movement.


Subject(s)
Attention/physiology , Brain Mapping , Brain/physiology , Evoked Potentials, Auditory/physiology , Reading , Time Perception/physiology , Acoustic Stimulation , Adolescent , Adult , Analysis of Variance , Electroencephalography/methods , Female , Humans , Male , Movement/physiology , Reaction Time , Young Adult
12.
Neuropsychologia ; 49(12): 3303-13, 2011 Oct.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21856320

ABSTRACT

The initiation of voluntary action is preceded by up to 2s of preparatory neural activity, originating in premotor and supplementary motor regions of the brain. The function of this extended period of pre-movement activity is unclear. Although recent studies have suggested that pre-movement activity is influenced by attention to action, little is understood about the specific processes that are involved in this preparatory period prior to voluntary action. We recorded readiness potentials averaged from EEG activity as participants made voluntary self-paced finger movements. We manipulated the processing resources available for action preparation using concurrent perceptual load and cognitive working memory load tasks. Results showed that pre-movement activity was significantly reduced only under conditions of high working memory load, when resources for planning action were limited by the concurrent cognitive load task. In contrast, limiting attentional resources in the perceptual load task had no effect on pre-movement readiness activity. This suggests that movement preparatory processes involve mechanisms of cognitive control that are also required for working memory, and not more general engagement of selective attentional resources. We propose that the extended period of pre-movement neural activity preceding voluntary action reflects the engagement of cognitive control mechanisms for endogenously orienting attention in time, in readiness for the initiation of voluntary action.


Subject(s)
Attention/physiology , Contingent Negative Variation/physiology , Intention , Movement/physiology , Visual Perception/physiology , Adult , Analysis of Variance , Brain Mapping , Electroencephalography , Female , Functional Laterality , Humans , Male , Neuropsychological Tests , Photic Stimulation , Psychomotor Performance , Reaction Time , Young Adult
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...