Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 20 de 37
Filter
1.
J Vis ; 22(10): 14, 2022 09 02.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36107124

ABSTRACT

Short-term deprivation of one eye by monocular patching causes a temporary increase in the contribution of that eye to binocular vision when the eye patch is removed. This effect, known as ocular dominance plasticity, provides a model of neuroplasticity within the human binocular visual system. We investigated whether physical exercise and the non-invasive brain stimulation technique transcranial random noise stimulation (tRNS), two interventions that may increase visual cortex neuroplasticity, enhance ocular dominance plasticity when delivered individually or in combination. Ocular dominance was measured using a grating rivalry test and a dichoptic letter contrast polarity judgment test. We observed robust ocular dominance changes for both outcome measures following 2-hour monocular deprivation; however, the magnitude of the effect was not influenced by exercise or tRNS. Ocular dominance plasticity may already be maximal after 2 hours of monocular deprivation in those with normal vision and therefore cannot be augmented by interventions designed to enhance neuroplasticity.


Subject(s)
Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation , Visual Cortex , Adult , Dominance, Ocular , Exercise , Humans , Vision, Monocular/physiology , Visual Cortex/physiology
2.
Clin Exp Optom ; 104(7): 773-779, 2021 09.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33689654

ABSTRACT

Clinical relevance: Home-based videogame treatments are increasingly popular for amblyopia treatment. However, at-home treatments tend to be done in short sessions and with frequent disruptions, which may reduce the effectiveness of binocular visual stimulation. These treatment adherence patterns need to be accounted for when considering dose-response relationships and treatment effectiveness.Background: Home-based videogame treatments are increasingly being used for various sensory conditions, including amblyopia ('lazy eye'), but treatment adherence continues to limit success. To examine detailed behavioural patterns associated with home-based videogame treatment, we analysed in detail the videogame adherence data from the Binocular tReatment of Amblyopia with VideOgames (BRAVO) clinical trial (ACTRN12613001004752).Methods: Children (7-12 years), teenagers (13-17 years) and adults (≥ 18 years) with unilateral amblyopia were loaned iPod Touch devices with either an active treatment or placebo videogame and instructed to play for a total of 1-2 hours/day for six weeks at home. Objectively-recorded adherence data from device software were used to analyse adherence patterns such as session length, daily distribution of gameplay, use of the pause function, and differences between age groups. Objectively-recorded adherence was also compared to subjectively-reported adherence from paper-based diaries.Results: One hundred and five of the 115 randomised participants completed six weeks of videogame training. Average adherence was 65% (SD 37%) of the minimum hours prescribed. Game training was generally performed in short sessions (mean 21.5, SD 11.2 minutes), mostly in the evening, with frequent pauses (median every 4.1 minutes, IQR 6.1). Children played in significantly shorter sessions and paused more frequently than older age groups (p < 0.0001). Participants tended to over-report adherence in subjective diaries compared to objectively-recorded gameplay time.Conclusion: Adherence to home-based videogame treatment was characterised by short sessions interspersed with frequent pauses, suggesting regular disengagement. This complicates dose-response calculations and may interfere with the effectiveness of treatments like binocular treatments for amblyopia, which require sustained visual stimulation.


Subject(s)
Amblyopia , Video Games , Adolescent , Adult , Aged , Amblyopia/therapy , Child , Humans , Sensory Deprivation , Treatment Outcome , Vision, Binocular , Visual Acuity
3.
Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci ; 61(10): 21, 2020 08 03.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32780865

ABSTRACT

Purpose: We compared the adaptive capacities of reflexive fusional convergence and divergence in 10 participants with untreated convergence insufficiency (CI) to 10 age-matched binocularly normal controls (BNCs) in an effort to elucidate the functional basis of CI. Methods: Vergence responses were monitored binocularly at 250 Hz using video-based infrared oculography, while single and double-step disparity stimuli were viewed dichoptically. The double-step stimuli were designed to induce an adaptive increase in the convergence or divergence reflexive fusional response dynamics. Results: As expected, convergence responses in the CI population were significantly slower at baseline (BNC 12.0 ± 1.8°/s vs. CI 7.4 ± 2.5°/s; P < 0.001), but divergence response velocities were similar between groups (P = 0.38). Critically, we observed an impaired adaptive change in convergence peak velocities in the CI group when compared to BNCs (-18.2% ± 27.3% vs. 25.4% ± 9.8%; P < 0.001). Adaptive changes in reflexive fusional divergence responses were similar between groups (P > 0.5) and significantly less robust when compared to BNC convergence. Conclusions: The results support the hypothesis that the adaptive capacities of vergence are related to the strength of the underlying reflexive fusional response. Combined, the evidence suggests that the clinical condition of convergence insufficiency is underpinned by an underdeveloped or perturbated reflexive fusional vergence response mechanism. We relate these observations to different clinical guidelines for the management and treatment of this condition.


Subject(s)
Convergence, Ocular/physiology , Ocular Motility Disorders/physiopathology , Adaptation, Physiological/physiology , Adult , Case-Control Studies , Electrooculography/methods , Humans , Photic Stimulation , Young Adult
4.
Brain Stimul ; 13(1): 215-228, 2020.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31427273

ABSTRACT

INTRODUCTION: The vergence oculomotor system possesses two robust adaptive mechanisms; a fast "dynamic" and a slow "tonic" system that are both vital for single, clear and comfortable binocular vision. The neural substrates underlying these vergence adaptive mechanisms in humans is unclear. METHODS: We investigated the role of the posterior cerebellum in convergence adaptation using inhibitory continuous theta-burst repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (cTBS) within a double-blind, sham controlled design while eye movements were recorded at 250hz via infrared oculography. RESULTS: In a preliminary experiment we validated our stimulation protocols by reproducing results from previous work on saccadic adaptation during the classic double-step adaptive shortening paradigm. Following this, across a series of three separate experiments we observed a clear dissociation in the effect of cTBS on convergence adaptation. Dynamic adaptation was substantially reduced while tonic adaptation was unaffected. Baseline dynamic fusional vergence response were also unaffected by stimulation. CONCLUSIONS: These results indicate a differential role for the posterior cerebellum in the adaptive control of convergence eye movements and provide initial evidence that repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation is a viable tool to investigate the neurophysiology of vergence control. The results are discussed in the context of the current models of implicit motor adaptation of vergence and their application to clinical populations and technology design in virtual and augmented head mounted display architectures. SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT: The cerebellum plays a critical role in the adaptive control of motor systems. Vergence eye movements shift our gaze in depth allowing us to see in 3D and exhibit two distinct adaptive mechanisms that are engaged under a range of conditions including reading, wearing head-mounted displays and using a new spectacle prescription. It is unclear what role the cerebellum plays in these adaptive mechanisms. To answer this, we temporarily disrupted the function of the posterior cerebellum using non-invasive brain stimulation and report impairment of only one adaptive mechanism, providing evidence for neural compartmentalization. The results have implications for vergence control models and applications to comfort and experience studies in head-mounted displays and the rehabilitation of clinical populations exhibiting vergence dysfunctions.


Subject(s)
Adaptation, Physiological , Cerebellum/physiology , Convergence, Ocular , Adult , Humans , Saccades
5.
J Vis ; 19(4): 9, 2019 04 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30943531

ABSTRACT

The aim of this study was to assess the relationship between binocular vision and fixation stability (FS). Across three experiments, we investigated (a) whether fixation was more stable during binocular versus monocular viewing across a range of stimulus contrasts in normal observers (n = 11), (b) whether binocular rivalry affected FS in normal observers (n = 14), and (c) whether FS was affected by interocular contrast differences in normal observers (n = 8) and patients with anisometropic amblyopia (n = 5). FS was quantified using global bivariate contour ellipse area, and microsaccades were detected using an unsupervised cluster-detection method. In normal observers, binocular viewing showed more stable fixation at all stimulus contrasts, and binocular rivalry did not affect FS. When interocular contrast was manipulated under dichoptic viewing conditions, normal observers exhibited less stable fixation for an eye that viewed 0% contrast (no fixation target). In anisometropic amblyopia, fixation was less stable in both eyes when the fellow eye viewed at 0% contrast. No effects were observed at other interocular contrast differences. Overall, binocular FS was impaired in both eyes in anisometropic amblyopia compared to normal observers. We conclude that binocular vision influences FS in normal observers but in an all-or-nothing fashion, whereby the presence or absence of a binocular target is important rather than the relative contrast of the targets in each eye. In anisometropic amblyopia, the fellow eye appears to control FS of both eyes under dichoptic viewing conditions.


Subject(s)
Amblyopia/physiopathology , Eye Movements/physiology , Fixation, Ocular/physiology , Vision, Binocular/physiology , Adult , Female , Humans , Male , Visual Acuity/physiology
6.
J. optom. (Internet) ; 12(1): 22-29, ene.-mar. 2019. grab, tab
Article in English | IBECS | ID: ibc-178509

ABSTRACT

Background: Accommodation is often recorded at a low sampling rate using devices such as autorefractors that are designed to measure the static refractive error. It is therefore important to determine if that resolution is sufficient to accurately measure the dynamic properties of accommodation. The current study provides both theoretical and empirical evidence on the ideal sampling rate necessary to measure a dynamic response. Methods: Accommodative and disaccommodative step stimuli ranging from 1-3 D (1 D steps) were presented using a Badal optical system. Responses from 12 children (8-13 years) and 6 adults (20-35 years) were recorded using a dynamic photorefractor (DPR). Fast Fourier transformation was applied to the unsmoothed dynamic responses including position, velocity and acceleration. Also, velocity and acceleration main sequence (MS) characteristics were compared between three photorefractor conditions on 3 subjects. Results: The Nyquist sampling limit necessary to accurately estimate position, velocity and acceleration was at least 5, 10 and 70 Hz, respectively. Peak velocity and acceleration were significantly underestimated at a lower rate (p < 0.5). However, the slope of MS remained invariant with sampling rate (p > 0.5). Conclusion: Contrary to the previous findings, a dynamic accommodative response exhibited frequencies larger than 10Hz. Stimulus direction and amplitude had no influence on the frequencies present in the dynamic response. Peak velocity and acceleration can be significantly underestimated when sampled at a lower rate. Taken as a whole, low sampling rate instruments can accurately estimate static accommodation, however, caution needs to be exercised when using them for dynamic accommodation


La acomodación se registra a menudo a una tasa de muestreo baja, utilizando dispositivos tales como los autorrefractómetros que están diseñados para medir el error refractivo estático. Por tanto, es importante determinar si dicha resolución es suficiente para medir con precisión las propiedades dinámicas de la acomodación. El estudio actual aporta evidencia tanto teórica como empírica acerca de la tasa de muestreo necesaria para medir una respuesta dinámica. Métodos: Se presentaron estímulos de alteraciones de estimulación y relajación (desacomodación) de la acomodación que oscilaron entre 1 y 3 D (pasos de 1 D) utilizando un sistema óptico Badal. Se registraron las respuestas de 12 niños (de 8 a 13 años) y 6 adultos (de 20 a 35 años) utilizando un sistema de fotorrefracción dinámico (DPR). La transformación rápida de Fourier se aplicó a las respuestas dinámicas no uniformes incluyendo posición, velocidad y aceleración. También se compararon las características de la secuencia principal de velocidad y aceleración entre las tres situaciones del sistema de fotorrefracción en 3 sujetos. Resultados: El límite de muestreo de Nyquist necesario para calcular con precisión la posición, velocidad y aceleración fue de al menos 5, 10 y 70Hz respectivamente. La velocidad y aceleración máximas se subestimaron significativamente a una tasa inferior (p < 0,5). Sin embargo, la pendiente de la secuencia principal permaneció invariable con la tasa de muestreo (p > 0,5). Conclusión: Contrariamente a los hallazgos anteriores, la respuesta acomodativa dinámica mostró unas frecuencias superiores a 10Hz. La dirección y amplitud del estímulo no influyeron en las frecuencias presentes en la respuesta dinámica. La velocidad y aceleración máximas pueden subestimarse significativamente cuando se muestrean a una tasa menor. En conjunto, los instrumentos de baja tasa de muestreo pueden calcular con precisión la acomodación estática; sin embargo, debe actuarse con precaución a la hora de calcular la acomodación dinámica


Subject(s)
Humans , Male , Female , Child , Adolescent , Young Adult , Adult , Accommodation, Ocular/physiology , Optometry/methods , Refractometry/methods , Fourier Analysis , Optometry/instrumentation , Refraction, Ocular/physiology , Refractometry/instrumentation
7.
J Optom ; 12(1): 22-29, 2019.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29580938

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: Accommodation is often recorded at a low sampling rate using devices such as autorefractors that are designed to measure the static refractive error. It is therefore important to determine if that resolution is sufficient to accurately measure the dynamic properties of accommodation. The current study provides both theoretical and empirical evidence on the ideal sampling rate necessary to measure a dynamic response. METHODS: Accommodative and disaccommodative step stimuli ranging from 1-3D (1D steps) were presented using a Badal optical system. Responses from 12 children (8-13 years) and 6 adults (20-35 years) were recorded using a dynamic photorefractor (DPR). Fast Fourier transformation was applied to the unsmoothed dynamic responses including position, velocity and acceleration. Also, velocity and acceleration main sequence (MS) characteristics were compared between three photorefractor conditions on 3 subjects. RESULTS: The Nyquist sampling limit necessary to accurately estimate position, velocity and acceleration was at least 5, 10 and 70Hz, respectively. Peak velocity and acceleration were significantly underestimated at a lower rate (p<0.5). However, the slope of MS remained invariant with sampling rate (p>0.5). CONCLUSION: Contrary to the previous findings, a dynamic accommodative response exhibited frequencies larger than 10Hz. Stimulus direction and amplitude had no influence on the frequencies present in the dynamic response. Peak velocity and acceleration can be significantly underestimated when sampled at a lower rate. Taken as a whole, low sampling rate instruments can accurately estimate static accommodation, however, caution needs to be exercised when using them for dynamic accommodation.


Subject(s)
Accommodation, Ocular/physiology , Optometry/methods , Refractometry/methods , Adolescent , Adult , Child , Female , Fourier Analysis , Humans , Male , Optometry/instrumentation , Refraction, Ocular/physiology , Refractometry/instrumentation , Young Adult
8.
Vision Res ; 154: 142-153, 2019 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30472331

ABSTRACT

School aged children with progressive myopia show large accommodative lags to blur only cue which is suggestive of a large depth of focus (DOF). While DOF measures are lacking in this age group, their blur detection and discrimination capacities appear to be similar to their non-myopic peers. Accordingly, the current study quantified DOF and blur detection ability in progressive myopic children showing large accommodative lags compared to their non-myopic peers and adults. Blur sensitivity measures were taken from 12 children (8-13 years, 6 myopes and 6 emmetropes) and 6 adults (20-35 years). DOF was quantified using step changes in the lens induced defocus while the subjects viewed a high contrast target through a Badal lens at either 2 or 4D demand. Blur detection thresholds (BDT) were tested using a similar high contrast target in a 2-alternate forced-choice paradigm (2AFC) at both the demands. In addition to the large accommodative lags, micro fluctuations and DOF were significantly larger in myopic children compared to the other groups. However, BDTs were similar across the three groups. When limited to blur cues, the findings of a large DOF coupled with large response lags suggests that myopes are less sensitive to retinal defocus. However, in agreement to a previous study, refractive error had no influence on their BDTs suggesting that the reduced sensitivity to the defocus in a myopic eye appears to be compensated by some form of an adjustment in the higher visual processes to preserve the subjective percept even with a poor retinal image quality.


Subject(s)
Accommodation, Ocular/physiology , Contrast Sensitivity/physiology , Myopia, Degenerative/physiopathology , Vision Disorders/physiopathology , Adolescent , Adult , Child , Depth Perception/physiology , Female , Humans , Male
9.
Vision Res ; 149: 66-76, 2018 08.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29940192

ABSTRACT

Divergence is known to differ from convergence across a wide range of clinical parameters. We have postulated that a limited neural substrate results in reduced fusional divergence velocities and subsequently a reduced capacity to adapt tonic vergence to uncrossed disparities. We further investigated this hypothesis by characterizing the degree of plasticity in reflexive fusional vergence to repetitive end-point errors using a disparity-based double-step paradigm. 10 adults completed 4 study visits where reflexive fusional convergence or divergence was measured (250 Hz infrared oculography) to a 2° disparity step and then lengthened or shortened via a repeated double-step (2°â€¯±â€¯1.5°). Stimuli were presented dichoptically at 40 cm. Adaptive modification of vergence responses was similar between directions for the shortening conditions, suggesting a common neural mechanism responds to overshooting errors. In comparison, adaptive lengthening of convergence was slower, but of equal magnitude, suggesting a second neural mechanism with a longer time constant for undershooting errors. Divergence response velocities were slower at baseline and did not increase after adaptive lengthening. Instead, increases in divergence response amplitudes were a result of increased response duration, implying saturation of the reflexive, preprogrammed response. Adaptive responses serving to increase or decrease reflexive fusional vergence recruitment were asymmetric. Adaptive lengthening of convergence and divergence identified further directional asymmetries. The results support the hypothesis that the neural substrate underlying divergence is attenuated, resulting in reduced reflexive plasticity when compared to convergence. The clinical and technological implications of these results are discussed.


Subject(s)
Adaptation, Ocular/physiology , Convergence, Ocular/physiology , Motor Activity/physiology , Adult , Analysis of Variance , Female , Humans , Male , Photic Stimulation/methods , Young Adult
10.
Ophthalmic Physiol Opt ; 38(2): 129-143, 2018 03.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29356022

ABSTRACT

PURPOSE: Optical treatment alone can improve visual acuity (VA) in children with amblyopia, thus clinical trials investigating additional amblyopia therapies (such as patching or videogames) for children require a preceding optical treatment phase. Emerging therapies for adult patients are entering clinical trials. It is unknown whether optical treatment is effective for adults with amblyopia and whether an optical correction phase is required for trials involving adults. METHODS: We examined participants who underwent optical treatment in the Binocular Treatment for Amblyopia using Videogames (BRAVO) clinical trial (ANZCTR ID: ACTRN12613001004752). Participants were recruited in three age groups (7 to 12, 13 to 17, or ≥18 years), and had unilateral amblyopia due to anisometropia and/or strabismus, with amblyopic eye VA of 0.30-1.00 logMAR (6/12 to 6/60, 20/40 to 20/200). Corrective lenses were prescribed based on cycloplegic refraction to fully correct any anisometropia. VA was assessed using the electronic visual acuity testing algorithm (e-ETDRS) test and near stereoacuity was assessed using the Randot Preschool Test. Participants were assessed every four weeks up to 16 weeks, until either VA was stable or until amblyopic eye VA improved to better than 0.30 logMAR, rendering the participant ineligible for the trial. RESULTS: Eighty participants (mean age 24.6 years, range 7.6-55.5 years) completed four to 16 weeks of optical treatment. A small but statistically significant mean improvement in amblyopic eye VA of 0.05 logMAR was observed (S.D. 0.08 logMAR; paired t-test p < 0.0001). Twenty-five participants (31%) improved by ≥1 logMAR line and of these, seven (9%) improved by ≥2 logMAR lines. Stereoacuity improved in 15 participants (19%). Visual improvements were not associated with age, presence of strabismus, or prior occlusion treatment. Two adult participants withdrew due to intolerance to anisometropic correction. Sixteen out of 80 participants (20%) achieved better than 0.30 logMAR VA in the amblyopic eye after optical treatment. Nine of these participants attended additional follow-up and four (44%) showed further VA improvements. CONCLUSIONS: Improvements from optical treatment resulted in one-fifth of participants becoming ineligible for the main clinical trial. Studies investigating additional amblyopia therapies must include an appropriate optical treatment only phase and/or parallel treatment group regardless of patient age. Optical treatment of amblyopia in adult patients warrants further investigation.


Subject(s)
Amblyopia/therapy , Eyeglasses , Visual Acuity/physiology , Adolescent , Adult , Amblyopia/physiopathology , Child , Double-Blind Method , Female , Follow-Up Studies , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Sensory Deprivation , Treatment Outcome , Young Adult
11.
JAMA Ophthalmol ; 136(2): 172-181, 2018 02 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29302694

ABSTRACT

Importance: Binocular amblyopia treatment using contrast-rebalanced stimuli showed promise in laboratory studies and requires clinical trial investigation in a home-based setting. Objective: To compare the effectiveness of a binocular video game with a placebo video game for improving visual functions in older children and adults. Design, Setting, and Participants: The Binocular Treatment of Amblyopia Using Videogames clinical trial was a multicenter, double-masked, randomized clinical trial. Between March 2014 and June 2016, 115 participants 7 years and older with unilateral amblyopia (amblyopic eye visual acuity, 0.30-1.00 logMAR; Snellen equivalent, 20/40-20/200) due to anisometropia, strabismus, or both were recruited. Eligible participants were allocated with equal chance to receive either the active or the placebo video game, with minimization stratified by age group (child, age 7 to 12 years; teenager, age 13 to 17 years; and adult, 18 years and older). Interventions: Falling-blocks video games played at home on an iPod Touch for 1 hour per day for 6 weeks. The active video game had game elements split between eyes with a dichoptic contrast offset (mean [SD] initial fellow eye contrast, 0.23 [0.14]). The placebo video game presented identical images to both eyes. Main Outcomes and Measures: Change in amblyopic eye visual acuity at 6 weeks. Secondary outcomes included compliance, stereoacuity, and interocular suppression. Participants and clinicians who measured outcomes were masked to treatment allocation. Results: Of the 115 included participants, 65 (56.5%) were male and 83 (72.2%) were white, and the mean (SD) age at randomization was 21.5 (13.6) years. There were 89 participants (77.4%) who had prior occlusion. The mean (SD) amblyopic eye visual acuity improved 0.06 (0.12) logMAR from baseline in the active group (n = 56) and 0.07 (0.10) logMAR in the placebo group (n = 59). The mean treatment difference between groups, adjusted for baseline visual acuity and age group, was -0.02 logMAR (95% CI, -0.06 to 0.02; P = .25). Compliance with more than 25% of prescribed game play was achieved by 36 participants (64%) in the active group and by 49 (83%) in the placebo group. At 6 weeks, 36 participants (64%) in the active group achieved fellow eye contrast greater than 0.9 in the binocular video game. No group differences were observed for any secondary outcomes. Adverse effects included 3 reports of transient asthenopia. Conclusions and Relevance: The specific home-based binocular falling-blocks video game used in this clinical trial did not improve visual outcomes more than the placebo video game despite increases in fellow eye contrast during game play. More engaging video games with considerations for compliance may improve effectiveness. Trial Registration: anzctr.org.au Identifier: ACTRN12613001004752.


Subject(s)
Amblyopia/rehabilitation , Computers, Handheld , Refraction, Ocular/physiology , Video Games , Vision, Binocular/physiology , Visual Acuity/physiology , Adolescent , Adult , Amblyopia/physiopathology , Child , Double-Blind Method , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Treatment Outcome , Young Adult
12.
J Vis ; 17(5): 4, 2017 05 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28505662

ABSTRACT

Horizontal vergence eye movements are controlled by two processes, phasic and slow-tonic. Slow-tonic responses are hypothesized to be stimulated by the faster, pulse-step neural output of the phasic system. This suggests that the general behavior of each system should be similar; however, this relationship has yet to be investigated directly. We characterize the relationship between phasic and tonic vergence by quantifying directional asymmetries in the response properties of each mechanism to the same disparity amplitudes. Four subjects viewed symmetric steps in disparity dichoptically at 40 cm while eye movements were recorded with infrared oculography. First- and second-order phasic and slow-tonic convergence response properties increased linearly with disparity demand (p < 0.01), whereas divergence responses did not (p > 0.05). Phasic divergence responses were slower than convergence (p = 0.012) and were associated with a higher frequency of saccades (p < 0.001). The average rate of slow-tonic change was correlated to the average peak velocity of phasic vergence at the same vergence demand in both directions, r = 0.78, p < 0.0001. Clear directional asymmetries were observed in phasic and tonic vergence responses. The response properties of the slow-tonic mechanism varied directly with the peak velocity of the complementary phasic system. These results provide empirical evidence of the relationship between phasic and slow-tonic vergence, suggesting that the latter depends on the motor function of the former, specifically the peak velocity. The recruitment of additional oculomotor mechanisms, such as saccades, improved the phasic response properties of the slower divergence mechanism but did not directly influence the response behavior of the slow-tonic mechanism.


Subject(s)
Convergence, Ocular/physiology , Saccades/physiology , Adult , Eye Movements , Female , Humans , Male , Vision, Binocular/physiology
13.
Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci ; 58(4): 2329-2340, 2017 04 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28431435

ABSTRACT

Purpose: Previously, we have mapped amblyopic eye suppression within the central 20° of the visual field and observed a gradient of suppression that is strongest in central vision and weakens with increasing eccentricity. In this study, using a large dichoptic display, we extend our novel suppression mapping approach further into the periphery (from 20°-60°) to assess whether suppression continues to decline with eccentricity or plateaus. Methods: Sixteen participants with amblyopia (10 with strabismus, 6 with anisometropia without strabismus; mean age: 37.9 ± 11 years) and six normal observers (mean age: 28.3 ± 5 years) took part. The visual stimulus (60° diameter), viewed from 57 cm, was composed of four concentric annuli (5° radius) with alternate contrast polarities starting from an eccentricity of 10°. Each annulus was divided into eight sectors subtending 45° of visual angle. Participants adjusted the contrast of a single sector presented to the fellow eye to match the perceived contrast of the remaining stimulus elements that were presented to the amblyopic eye. A matching contrast that was lower in the fellow eye than the amblyopic eye indicated suppression. Results: Patients with strabismus exhibited significantly stronger interocular suppression than controls across all eccentricities (P = 0.01). Patients with anisometropia did not differ from controls (P = 0.58). Suppression varied significantly with eccentricity (P = 0.005) but this effect did not differ between patient groups (P = 0.217). Conclusions: In amblyopia, suppression is present beyond the central 10° in patients with strabismus. Suppression becomes weaker at greater eccentricities and this may enable peripheral fusion that could be used by binocular treatment methods.


Subject(s)
Amblyopia/physiopathology , Anisometropia/complications , Contrast Sensitivity/physiology , Sensory Thresholds , Strabismus/complications , Vision, Binocular/physiology , Visual Fields/physiology , Adult , Amblyopia/complications , Anisometropia/physiopathology , Female , Humans , Male , Photic Stimulation/methods , Strabismus/physiopathology , Visual Acuity
15.
Vision Res ; 130: 9-21, 2017 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27876513

ABSTRACT

Children with a progressing myopia exhibit an abnormal pattern of high accommodative lags coupled with high accommodative convergence (AC/A) and high accommodative adaptation. This is not predicted by the current models of accommodation and vergence. Reduced accommodative plant gain and reduced sensitivity to blur have been suggested as potential causes for this abnormal behavior. These etiologies were tested by altering parameters (sensory, controller and plant gains) in the Simulink model of accommodation. Predictions were then compared to the static and dynamic blur accommodation (BA) measures taken using a Badal optical system on 12 children (6 emmetropes and 6 myopes, 8-13years) and 6 adults (20-35years). Other critical parameters such as CA/C, AC/A, and accommodative adaptation were also measured. Usable BA responses were classified as either typical or atypical. Typical accommodation data confirmed the abnormal pattern of myopia along with an unchanged CA/C. Main sequence relationship remained invariant between myopic and nonmyopic children. An overall reduction was noted in the response dynamics such as peak velocity and acceleration with age. Neither a reduced plant gain nor reduced blur sensitivity could predict the abnormal accommodative behavior. A model adjustment reflecting a reduced accommodative sensory gain (ASG) coupled with an increased AC cross-link gain and reduced vergence adaptive gain does predict the empirical findings. Empirical measures also showed a greater frequency of errors in accommodative response generation (atypical responses) in both myopic and control children compared to adults.


Subject(s)
Accommodation, Ocular/physiology , Adaptation, Ocular/physiology , Convergence, Ocular/physiology , Myopia/physiopathology , Adaptation, Physiological/physiology , Adolescent , Adult , Child , Female , Humans , Male , Models, Theoretical , Young Adult
16.
Trials ; 17(1): 504, 2016 10 18.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27756405

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: Amblyopia is a common neurodevelopmental disorder of vision that is characterised by visual impairment in one eye and compromised binocular visual function. Existing evidence-based treatments for children include patching the nonamblyopic eye to encourage use of the amblyopic eye. Currently there are no widely accepted treatments available for adults with amblyopia. The aim of this trial is to assess the efficacy of a new binocular, videogame-based treatment for amblyopia in older children and adults. We hypothesise that binocular treatment will significantly improve amblyopic eye visual acuity relative to placebo treatment. METHODS/DESIGN: The BRAVO study is a double-blind, randomised, placebo-controlled multicentre trial to assess the effectiveness of a novel videogame-based binocular treatment for amblyopia. One hundred and eight participants aged 7 years or older with anisometropic and/or strabismic amblyopia (defined as ≥0.2 LogMAR interocular visual acuity difference, ≥0.3 LogMAR amblyopic eye visual acuity and no ocular disease) will be recruited via ophthalmologists, optometrists, clinical record searches and public advertisements at five sites in New Zealand, Canada, Hong Kong and Australia. Eligible participants will be randomised by computer in a 1:1 ratio, with stratification by age group: 7-12, 13-17 and 18 years and older. Participants will be randomised to receive 6 weeks of active or placebo home-based binocular treatment. Treatment will be in the form of a modified interactive falling-blocks game, implemented on a 5th generation iPod touch device viewed through red/green anaglyphic glasses. Participants and those assessing outcomes will be blinded to group assignment. The primary outcome is the change in best-corrected distance visual acuity in the amblyopic eye from baseline to 6 weeks post randomisation. Secondary outcomes include distance and near visual acuity, stereopsis, interocular suppression, angle of strabismus (where applicable) measured at baseline, 3, 6, 12 and 24 weeks post randomisation. Treatment compliance and acceptability will also be assessed along with quality of life for adult participants. DISCUSSION: The BRAVO study is the first randomised controlled trial of a home-based videogame treatment for older children and adults with amblyopia. The results will indicate whether a binocular approach to amblyopia treatment conducted at home is effective for patients aged 7 years or older. TRIAL REGISTRATION: This trial was registered in Australia and New Zealand Clinical Trials Registry ( ACTRN12613001004752 ) on 10 September 2013.


Subject(s)
Amblyopia/therapy , Clinical Trials as Topic , Video Games , Adolescent , Adult , Child , Double-Blind Method , Humans , Young Adult
17.
Eur J Neurosci ; 43(12): 1553-60, 2016 06.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26991129

ABSTRACT

Adaptation to changing environmental demands is central to maintaining optimal motor system function. Current theories suggest that adaptation in both the skeletal-motor and oculomotor systems involves a combination of fast (reflexive) and slow (recalibration) mechanisms. Here we used the oculomotor vergence system as a model to investigate the mechanisms underlying slow motor adaptation. Unlike reaching with the upper limbs, vergence is less susceptible to changes in cognitive strategy that can affect the behaviour of motor adaptation. We tested the hypothesis that mechanisms of slow motor adaptation reflect early neural processing by assessing the linearity of adaptive responses over a large range of stimuli. Using varied disparity stimuli in conflict with accommodation, the slow adaptation of tonic vergence was found to exhibit a linear response whereby the rate (R(2)  = 0.85, P < 0.0001) and amplitude (R(2)  = 0.65, P < 0.0001) of the adaptive effects increased proportionally with stimulus amplitude. These results suggest that this slow adaptive mechanism is an early neural process, implying a fundamental physiological nature that is potentially dominated by subcortical and cerebellar substrates.


Subject(s)
Accommodation, Ocular , Adaptation, Physiological , Convergence, Ocular , Eye Movement Measurements , Humans
18.
19.
Vision Res ; 111(Pt A): 105-13, 2015 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25891521

ABSTRACT

This research tested the hypothesis that the successful treatment of convergence insufficiency (CI) with vision-training (VT) procedures, leads to an increased capacity of vergence adaptation (VAdapt) allowing a more rapid downward adjustment of the convergence accommodation cross-link. Nine subjects with CI were recruited from a clinical population, based upon reduced fusional vergence amplitudes, receded near point of convergence or symptomology. VAdapt and the resulting changes to convergence accommodation (CA) were measured at specific intervals over 15 min (pre-training). Separate clinical measures of the accommodative convergence cross link, horizontal fusion limits and near point of convergence were taken and a symptomology questionnaire completed. Subjects then participated in a VT program composed of 2.5h at home and 1h in-office weekly for 12-14 weeks. Clinical testing was done weekly. VAdapt and CA measures were retaken once clinical measures normalized for 2 weeks (mid-training) and then again when symptoms had cleared (post-training). VAdapt and CA responses as well as the clinical measures were taken on a control group showing normal clinical findings. Six subjects provided complete data sets. CI clinical findings reached normal levels between 4 and 7 weeks of training but symptoms, VAdapt, and CA output remained significantly different from the controls until 12-14 weeks. The hypothesis was retained. The reduced VAdapt and excessive CA found in CI were normalized through orthoptic treatment. This time course was underestimated by clinical findings but matched symptom amelioration.


Subject(s)
Accommodation, Ocular/physiology , Adaptation, Ocular/physiology , Convergence, Ocular/physiology , Ocular Motility Disorders/physiopathology , Ocular Motility Disorders/therapy , Orthoptics/methods , Vision Disparity/physiology , Adolescent , Adult , Analysis of Variance , Case-Control Studies , Child , Female , Humans , Male , Young Adult
20.
Vision Res ; 101: 51-61, 2014 Aug.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24954685

ABSTRACT

Investigations into the progression of myopia in children have long considered the role of accommodation as a cause and solution. Myopic children show high levels of accommodative adaptation, coupled with accommodative lag and high response AC/A (accommodative convergence per diopter of accommodation). This pattern differs from that predicted by current models of interaction between accommodation and vergence, where weakened reflex responses and a high AC/A would be associated with a low not high levels of accommodative adaptation. However, studies of young myopes were limited to only part of the accommodative vergence synkinesis and the reciprocal components of vergence adaptation and convergence accommodation were not studied in tandem. Accordingly, we test the hypothesis that the accommodative behavior of myopic children is not predicted by current models and whether that departure is explained by differences in the accommodative plant of the myopic child. Responses to incongruent stimuli (-2D, +2D adds, 10 prism diopter base-out prism) were investigated in 28 myopic and 25 non-myopic children aged 7-15 years. Subjects were divided into phoria groups - exo, ortho and eso based upon their near phoria. The school aged myopes showed high levels of accommodative adaptation but with reduced accommodation and high AC/A. This pattern is not explained by current adult models and could reflect a sluggish gain of the accommodative plant (ciliary muscle and lens), changes in near triad innervation or both. Further, vergence adaptation showed a predictable reciprocal relationship with the high accommodative adaptation, suggesting that departures from adult models were limited to accommodation not vergence behavior.


Subject(s)
Accommodation, Ocular/physiology , Adaptation, Ocular/physiology , Convergence, Ocular/physiology , Myopia/physiopathology , Adaptation, Physiological/physiology , Adolescent , Analysis of Variance , Child , Female , Humans , Male
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...