Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 4 de 4
Filter
Add more filters










Database
Language
Publication year range
1.
Behav Pharmacol ; 14(7): 517-23, 2003 Nov.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-14557719

ABSTRACT

We have shown previously that 15 mg/kg pentobarbital induces a conditioned place preference (CPP), but it is unsuitable for intracranial administration. Since the long-acting barbiturate, sodium barbital, is soluble at a neutral pH, we tested whether it would induce a CPP when administered centrally. Furthermore, because barbital has a long duration of action, and because we obtained a significant CPP to systemically administered barbital using 30-minute conditioning trials, we tested whether longer conditioning trials would produce a more robust CPP. Using a three-compartment apparatus and an unbiased procedure, we found that systemic administration of barbital induced a significant CPP at 8 and 24 mg/kg, but not 2.7 or 72 mg/kg (i.p.). When rats were conditioned to 24 mg/kg barbital for conditioning trials of (1/2), 1, 3, or 6 hours, only the 30-min conditioning trial produced a CPP. Finally, 240 and 480 microg intracerebroventricular (ICV) barbital induced a significant CPP, but 60 or 120 microg did not. These findings suggest that: (1) like pentobarbital, barbital has reinforcing properties measured in the CPP test; (2) the CPP is impaired, rather than enhanced, by increasing the duration of drug-context pairing; and (3) the reinforcing effects of barbiturates are centrally mediated.


Subject(s)
Barbital/pharmacology , Choice Behavior/drug effects , Conditioning, Classical/drug effects , Hypnotics and Sedatives/pharmacology , Social Environment , Animals , Association Learning/drug effects , Dose-Response Relationship, Drug , Injections, Intraperitoneal , Injections, Intraventricular , Male , Motor Activity/drug effects , Rats , Rats, Long-Evans
2.
Psychopharmacology (Berl) ; 157(2): 115-22, 2001 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11594435

ABSTRACT

RATIONALE: Drugs that are self-administered usually produce a conditioned place preference (CPP) but pentobarbital is self-administered by both animals and humans and is reported to be aversive in the CPP test. OBJECTIVES: We tested whether pentobarbital (5, 15, and 25 mg/kg; IP) could produce a place preference and examined the role of GABA, dopamine (DA) and opioid receptors in the pentobarbital CPP. METHODS: Place conditioning was carried out in an apparatus consisting of two compartments connected by an alley at the rear. During the pre-exposure and test phase, the rats were free to wander in the apparatus for 20 min, and were drug-free. During the 6-day conditioning phase, rats were injected with drug (or vehicle), and confined to one compartment for 30 min and on alternate days were injected with vehicle (or drug) and confined to the other compartment. Upon obtaining a CPP, we examined whether pretreatment of the GABAA antagonists picrotoxin or bicuculline (0.5, 1.0, and 2.0 mg/kg; IP), the DA antagonist eticlopride (0.01, 0.05, and 0.25 mg/kg; SC), or the opioid antagonist naloxone (0.02, 0.20, and 2.0 mg/kg; IP) would block the CPP. RESULTS: 15 mg/kg pentobarbital produced a CPP. The pentobarbital CPP was blocked by pretreatment of 1.0 and 2.0 mg/kg bicuculline, but not by 0.5, 1.0, or 2.0 mg/kg picrotoxin. The pentobarbital CPP was also blocked by 0.05 and 0.25 mg/kg eticlopride and by all of the doses of naloxone tested. CONCLUSIONS: Pretreatment with the antagonists bicuculline, eticlopride, and naloxone blocked a 15 mg/kg pentobarbital CPP. This indicates that GABAergic, dopaminergic, and opioid systems play a role in the reinforcing properties of pentobarbital.


Subject(s)
Cardiotonic Agents/pharmacology , Conditioning, Psychological/drug effects , Dopamine/physiology , Hypnotics and Sedatives/pharmacology , Narcotic Antagonists/pharmacology , Pentobarbital/pharmacology , gamma-Aminobutyric Acid/physiology , Animals , Conditioning, Psychological/physiology , Dopamine Antagonists/pharmacology , Dose-Response Relationship, Drug , GABA Antagonists/pharmacology , Hypnotics and Sedatives/antagonists & inhibitors , Male , Pentobarbital/antagonists & inhibitors , Rats , Rats, Long-Evans
3.
Behav Brain Res ; 96(1-2): 93-9, 1998 Nov.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9821546

ABSTRACT

Wheel running activity can shorten the period (tau) of circadian rhythms in rats and mice. The role of serotonin (5HT), in this effect of behavior on circadian pacemaker function, was assessed by measuring tau during wheel-open and wheel-locked conditions in mice sustaining neurotoxic 5HT lesions directed at the suprachiasmatic nucleus (SCN). Intact mice exhibited a significant lengthening of tau (approximately 10 min) within 3 weeks when running wheels were locked. Mice with immunocytochemically confirmed 5HT depletion showed significantly longer tau than intact mice during wheel access, and did not show a significant change in tau up to 6 weeks after wheels were locked. In these mice, variability of tau across wheel access conditions was similar in magnitude to tau variability in intact mice at two time points without wheel access (+/- 3 min). 5HT-depleted mice also exhibited significantly longer activity periods (alpha), and a significantly delayed peak of activity within alpha. Previous studies show that a delayed peak of activity within alpha is associated with longer tau. Group differences in tau, and apparent failure of wheel-locking to lengthen tau in mice with 5HT lesions, may thus be due to loss of a serotonergic behavioral input pathway to the SCN, or to a lesion-induced change in the waveform of the activity rhythm.


Subject(s)
Behavior, Animal/physiology , Circadian Rhythm/physiology , Feedback/physiology , Serotonin/physiology , 5,7-Dihydroxytryptamine/toxicity , Animals , Immunohistochemistry , Male , Mice , Mice, Inbred C57BL , Motor Activity/drug effects , Serotonin Agents/toxicity
4.
Brain Res ; 739(1-2): 12-8, 1996 Nov 11.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8955919

ABSTRACT

This study examined whether the circadian system of rats can serve as a consulted clock for discriminating time of day. Food restricted rats housed in activity wheels were trained to lever press for food in a two-lever T-maze in which the left arm was correct in a morning feeding session, and the right arm in an afternoon session (7 h interval). All six rats learned the task (discrimination ratios > chance on 85-95% of sessions) and exhibited anticipatory wheel running prior to most sessions. Performance was not disrupted by inverting the LD cycle or by omitting 1-3 sessions, indicating that learning was not dependent on light-dark cues, alternation strategies, or physiological states associated with intermeal interval. Five of six additional rats with ablations of the suprachiasmatic nucleus light-entrainable pacemaker acquired the discrimination, indicating that time-of-day cues can be provided by another circadian pacemaker (likely food-entrainable). The results provide the first clear evidence that the circadian system in a mammal can function as a consulted clock that provides discriminative time cues for cognitive processes subserving behavioral plasticity.


Subject(s)
Biological Clocks/physiology , Circadian Rhythm/physiology , Conditioning, Operant/physiology , Discrimination Learning/physiology , Maze Learning/physiology , Suprachiasmatic Nucleus/physiology , Animals , Food Deprivation/physiology , Male , Physical Exertion/physiology , Rats , Rats, Wistar , Reference Values , Running/physiology
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...