Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 15 de 15
Filter
1.
Animals (Basel) ; 14(14)2024 Jul 21.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-39061585

ABSTRACT

Companion cat caregivers ascribe complex emotions and cognitions to their cats, and these attributions are greater with a stronger attachment to their animals. We compared attributions of emotional and cognitive complexity to cats in cat caregivers and non-caregivers. We measured attitudes toward animals, belief in animal minds, caregivers' strength of attachment, and attachment style with a particular companion cat in 448 university students, of whom 251 had owned a cat. We asked the extent to which respondents thought that cats were capable of expressing primary and secondary emotions and cognitive capabilities. Caregivers significantly differed from non-caregivers only in the attribution of primary emotions. Belief in animal minds predicted the attribution of secondary emotions and cognition. For caregivers only, avoidant attachment style was negatively associated with the attribution of primary emotions, whereas attachment strength was positively associated with the attribution of secondary emotions and cognition. These abilities may have greater discriminatory power as most respondents ascribe primary emotions to cats, perhaps for anthropomorphic reasons. Housing conditions (indoor and outdoor) were not associated with attributions, suggesting that bonds are more important than time spent together within the home in predicting the attribution of emotional and cognitive complexity in cats.

2.
Animals (Basel) ; 13(23)2023 Nov 24.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38066995

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: For sustainable and healthy relationships with pets, attributing some degree of human abilities to the pet (anthropomorphization) might be necessary. We hypothesize that the tendency to anthropomorphize pet animals is related to relationship behaviors (communication and making up) and the experience of social support. Perceiving the pet in a human social role (e.g., family member or friend) might mediate this relationship. METHOD: Associations were tested in a mixed sample of cat and dog owners by means of multiple linear regression, moderation, and (moderated) mediation analyses. The differences between cat and dog owners were examined with pet type as the moderator in a moderated mediation analysis. RESULTS: Dog owners anthropomorphize their pets more than cat owners. The social role of the pet mediates the association between anthropomorphization and relationship behavior and social support. The mediation effects were stronger for cat owners than for dog owners. Moreover, our newly developed comparative anthropomorphism measure was a better predictor than the commonly used general anthropomorphism measure (IDAQ).

3.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35010452

ABSTRACT

Describing the relationship with one's cat in human terms might reflect an underlying anthropomorphic view of the relationship which might be associated with an owner's behavior towards their cat and the cat's living environment. Owners self-categorized the relationship with their cat as either a 'member of the family', 'as a child', 'best friend', or 'a pet animal'. The extent to which owner- and cat-related factors influence these four relationship descriptions are examined in survey data of approximately 1800 cat owners. Differences in outdoor access, care during absence of the owner, and access to the bedroom are examined between the four relationship perceptions. The owner's age and household composition, ideas about their cat's equality, support, and dependency, and whether their cat is a pedigree were significantly associated with relationship description and explained 46% of the variance. Owners who perceive their cat as a child or best friend see their cat as loyal, empathetic, equal to family, and dependent on them for love and care. Their cats are less often left in the care of others, are allowed more often in the bedroom and have less often (unrestricted) outdoor access. Moreover, cats perceived as children are more likely to live in a multi-cat household. Our results provide insight in the factors that are related to different (anthropomorphic) perceptions of the human-cat relationship and how perceptions relate to the living environment of cats.


Subject(s)
Family , Friends , Animals , Cats , Humans , Surveys and Questionnaires
4.
Animals (Basel) ; 10(5)2020 Apr 29.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32365588

ABSTRACT

In the framework of the early prevention of problems in the owner-dog relationship, it is important to have a broad perspective on the development of this relationship over time, starting before people actually acquire a dog. People who currently (or previously) own(ed) a dog can rely on their experiences when considering a new dog, while this knowledge is unavailable to first time dog-owners. In this study, we explore how self-efficacy, social comparison, perceptions about the (dis)advantages of ownership and commitment to the dog (so-called social cognitive factors), problematic canine behaviors, perceived costs, and satisfaction with the dog change over time. We examine changes from the motivational phase of relationship development (before acquisition of the dog) into the experience phase (six and twelve months after acquisition of the dog). We explore if patterns are different in experienced (previous (n = 73) and current (n = 80)) versus unexperienced (first time (n = 30) dog owners. The respondents filled in three online questionnaires-once before and twice after acquisition of their dog. From T0 (before acquisition of the dog) to T1 (having the dog for six months) participants (especially those with no ownership experience) had to adjust their perceptions about dogs and dog ownership. Experiencing the relationship for an additional year (from T1 to T2) barely changed the social cognitive factors, satisfaction, and perceived costs. A small decline in problematic canine behaviors was present among the experienced dog owners between T1 and T2. To conclude, perceptions about dogs and dog ownership change over time, but after testing these perceptions with reality, they become stable after about six months.

5.
PLoS One ; 8(2): e54821, 2013.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23457454

ABSTRACT

In adolescence, being socially successful depends to a large extent on being popular with peers. Even though some youths have what it takes to be popular, they are not, whereas others seem to have a secret ingredient that just makes the difference. In this study the G-allele of a functional polymorphism in the promotor region of the 5HT2A serotonin receptor gene (-G1438A) was identified as a secret ingredient for popularity among peers. These findings build on and extend previous work by Burt (2008, 2009). Tackling limitations from previous research, the role of the 5HT2A serotonin receptor gene was examined in adolescent males (N = 285; average age 13) using a unique sample of the TRAILS study. Carrying the G-allele enhanced the relation between aggression and popularity, particularly for those boys who have many female friends. This seems to be an "enhancer" effect of the G-allele whereby popularity relevant characteristics are made more noticeable. There is no "popularity gene", as the G-allele by itself had no effect on popularity.


Subject(s)
Polymorphism, Genetic , Receptor, Serotonin, 5-HT2A/genetics , Achievement , Adolescent , Aggression , Alleles , Female , Friends , Genotype , Humans , Interpersonal Relations , Male , Peer Group
6.
Psychoneuroendocrinology ; 38(5): 659-66, 2013 May.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22963816

ABSTRACT

Clinical and epidemiological studies, further supported by meta-analytic studies, indicate a possible association between chronicity (i.e., persistence or recurrence) of depression and hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis responsiveness to psychosocial stress. In the present study, we examined whether and how chronicity of depressive problems predicts cortisol responses to a standardized social stress test in adolescents. Data were collected in a high-risk focus sample (n=351) of the Tracking Adolescents' Individual Lives Survey (TRAILS) cohort, a large prospective population study with bi- to triennial measurements. Depressive problems were assessed around age 11, 13.5, and 16. Cortisol levels were measured in saliva, sampled before, during, and after the Groningen Social Stress Test (GSST), to determine the cortisol response to psychosocial stress. The area under the curve with respect to the increase (AUCi) (i.e., change from baseline) of the cortisol response was used as a measure of HPA axis response. By means of linear regression analysis and repeated-measures analysis of variance, it was examined whether chronicity of depressive problems predicted the cortisol response to the GSST around the age of 16. Chronicity of depressive problems was significantly associated with cortisol stress responses. The relationship was curvilinear, with recent-onset depressive problems predicting an increased cortisol response, and more chronic depressive problems a blunted response. The results of this study suggest that depressive problems initially increase cortisol responses to stress, but that this pattern reverses when depressive problems persist over prolonged periods of time.


Subject(s)
Chronobiology Phenomena , Depression , Hydrocortisone/metabolism , Stress, Psychological/metabolism , Adolescent , Child , Chronobiology Phenomena/physiology , Cohort Studies , Depression/metabolism , Depression/physiopathology , Female , Humans , Hydrocortisone/analysis , Hypothalamo-Hypophyseal System/physiology , Male , Neuropsychological Tests , Pituitary-Adrenal System/physiology , Saliva/chemistry , Saliva/metabolism , Young Adult
7.
Psychophysiology ; 48(4): 441-52, 2011 Apr.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21361964

ABSTRACT

Studies regarding the interrelation of perceived and physiological stress indices have shown diverging results. Using a population sample of adolescents (N=715, 50.9% girls, mean age 16.11 years, SD=0.59), we tested three hypotheses: (1) perceived responses during social stress covary with concurrent physiological stress responses; (2) high pretest levels of perceived stress predict large physiological responses; and (3) large physiological responses to social stress predict low posttest perceived stress levels. Perceived arousal, unpleasantness, and dominance were related to heart rate, respiratory sinus arrhythmia, and cortisol responses to a laboratory social stress test. Although effect sizes were small, the results suggest covariation of perceived stress and concurrent physiological stress responses in both the ANS and the HPA axis, as well as inverse associations between heart rate responsiveness and the subsequent appraisal of stress.


Subject(s)
Stress, Physiological/physiology , Stress, Psychological/psychology , Adolescent , Arousal , Arrhythmia, Sinus/physiopathology , Autonomic Nervous System/physiopathology , Female , Heart Rate/physiology , Humans , Hydrocortisone/blood , Hypothalamo-Hypophyseal System/physiology , Hypothalamo-Hypophyseal System/physiopathology , Male , Mental Disorders/physiopathology , Mental Disorders/psychology , Pituitary-Adrenal System/physiology , Pituitary-Adrenal System/physiopathology , Social Environment
8.
Neurosci Biobehav Rev ; 35(8): 1757-70, 2011 Aug.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21040743

ABSTRACT

Adolescence is characterized by major biological, psychological, and social challenges, as well as by an increase in depression rates. This review focuses on the association between stressful experiences and depression in adolescence, and the possible role of the hypothalamus-pituitary-adrenal cortex (HPA-)axis in this link. Adolescent girls have a higher probability to develop depressive symptoms than adolescent boys and preadolescents. Increasing evidence indicates that girls' higher risk of depression is partly brought about by an increased sensitivity for stressful life events, particularly interpersonal stressors, which are highly prevalent in adolescent girls. Genetic risk factors for depression, as well as those for stress sensitivity, are often expressed differently in girls and boys. Also environmental adversity tends to affect girls' stress responses more than those of boys. These gender-specific association patterns have been reported for both sensitivity to stressful life events and HPA-axis responses to social stress. Together, the findings suggest that girls are more malleable than boys in response to internal and external influences. This postulated greater malleability may be adaptive in many circumstances, but also brings along risk, such as an increased probability of depression.


Subject(s)
Critical Period, Psychological , Depressive Disorder/etiology , Hypothalamo-Hypophyseal System/physiopathology , Pituitary-Adrenal System/physiopathology , Psychology, Adolescent , Stress, Psychological/complications , Adaptation, Physiological , Adolescent , Adolescent Development , Depression/etiology , Depression/physiopathology , Depressive Disorder/physiopathology , Female , Humans , Male , Sex Factors , Stress, Psychological/physiopathology
9.
Psychoneuroendocrinology ; 36(6): 854-63, 2011 Jul.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21185125

ABSTRACT

Depression runs in families and is considered a stress-related disorder. Familial risk for depression may be transmitted via deregulated psychophysiological stress responses from parent to child. In this study, we examined the association between self-assessed lifetime parental depressive problems (PDP) and adolescent offspring' cortisol responses to a social stress test. Data were collected as part of the third assessment wave of TRAILS (TRacking Adolescents' Individual Lives Survey), a large prospective population study of Dutch adolescents. Data of 330 adolescents (mean age 16.04; 40.9% girls) who participated in a laboratory session, including a standardized performance-related social stress task (public speaking and mental arithmetic) were examined. Four saliva cortisol samples were collected before, during and after the social stress task which were analyzed with repeated measures Analysis of Variance. Lifetime parental depressive problems were assessed by self-reports from both biological parents. PDP was associated with daughter' cortisol responses (F(3,133)=3.90, p=.02), but no association was found in sons (F(3,193)=0.27, p=.78). Girls whose parents ever experienced depressive symptoms displayed a blunted cortisol response to the standardized social stress test, while girls whose parents never had such problems displayed the characteristic curvilinear response pattern. This effect was not mediated by offspring stress history (age 0-16). Analyses were corrected for smoking behaviour and adolescent depressed mood. The fact that PDP were measured by self-report questionnaires and did not reflect clinical DSM-IV diagnosis could be considered a limitation of the study.


Subject(s)
Depression/genetics , Hydrocortisone/metabolism , Parents/psychology , Stress, Psychological/physiopathology , Adolescent , Adult , Child , Diagnostic Self Evaluation , Female , Health Surveys , Humans , Mathematics , Netherlands , Parent-Child Relations , Prospective Studies , Psychological Tests , Saliva/chemistry , Smoking , Speech , Stress, Psychological/psychology , Surveys and Questionnaires , Young Adult
10.
Behav Genet ; 41(2): 253-61, 2011 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20680430

ABSTRACT

Previously, sequence variation in the glucocorticoid (GR) and mineralocorticoid (MR) receptor genes (NR3C1 and NR3C2, respectively) have been found to be associated with physiological stress responses to social stress tests in small samples of adult men and oral contraceptives (OC) using women. Associations between single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) in the GR (23EK-rs6190, 9beta-rs6198, BclI-rs4142324) and the MR gene (I180V-rs5522 and -2G/C (rs2070951) with cortisol and heart rate responses to a performance-related social stress task (public speaking and mental arithmetic) were examined in a large sample (n = 553) of adolescents (15-17 years). To make comparisons with previous findings, associations were tested in boys (n = 277), free-cycling (FC) girls (n = 183) and OC users (n = 93). None of the previously reported associations in adults could be replicated in this large adolescent sample. Explanations for non-replication are discussed.


Subject(s)
Heart Rate , Hydrocortisone/blood , Polymorphism, Single Nucleotide , Receptors, Glucocorticoid/genetics , Receptors, Mineralocorticoid/genetics , Stress, Psychological/metabolism , Adolescent , Contraceptives, Oral/adverse effects , Depression/blood , False Positive Reactions , Female , Gene Frequency , Genotype , Humans , Hydrocortisone/metabolism , Hypothalamo-Hypophyseal System , Male
12.
Biol Psychiatry ; 68(2): 209-12, 2010 Jul 15.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20553751

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: The three-way interaction between the functional polymorphism in the serotonin transporter gene linked promoter region, the val66met polymorphism in the brain-derived neurotrophic factor gene, and childhood adversity in the prediction of depression in children, reported by Kaufman and colleagues in 2006, has only been confirmed in adult samples. This study examines the gene-by-gene-by-environment interaction in an adolescent sample. METHODS: In a longitudinal population-based study, depression scores were assessed with the Youth Self Report at ages 11, 13.5, and 16. Pre- and perinatal adversities and childhood events were assessed in a parent interview at age 11. Long-term difficulties until age 11 were assessed with a parent questionnaire at age 13.5. Blood or buccal cells were collected for genotyping at age 16. The study included 1096 complete data sets. RESULTS: Depression score over the three measurements was not significantly predicted by any interaction between genotypes and childhood adversities. CONCLUSIONS: We were unable to confirm the three-way interaction in a representative, population-based sample of adolescents. The large sample resulted in adequate power, which in combination with the reliability of our measures gives confidence in our findings.


Subject(s)
Brain-Derived Neurotrophic Factor/genetics , Depressive Disorder/genetics , Life Change Events , Polymorphism, Single Nucleotide/genetics , Serotonin Plasma Membrane Transport Proteins/genetics , Adolescent , Adult , Depressive Disorder/psychology , Female , Genotype , Humans , Linear Models , Male , Prospective Studies , Stress, Psychological/genetics , Stress, Psychological/psychology
13.
Psychoneuroendocrinology ; 34(6): 884-93, 2009 Jul.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19195792

ABSTRACT

Studies on the influence of sex hormones on cortisol responses to awakening and stress have mainly been conducted in adults, while reports on adolescents are scarce. We studied the effects of gender, menstrual cycle phase and oral contraceptive (OC) use on cortisol responses in a large sample of adolescents. Data come from TRAILS (TRacking Adolescents' Individual Lives Survey), a prospective population study of Dutch adolescents. This study uses data of 644 adolescents (age 15-17 years, 54.7% boys) who participated in a laboratory session including a performance-related social stress task (public speaking and mental arithmetic). Free cortisol levels were assessed by multiple saliva samples, both after awakening and during the laboratory session. No significant effects of gender and menstrual phase on cortisol responses to awakening were found, while girls using OC displayed a slightly blunted response (F(1, 244)=5.30, p=.02). Cortisol responses to social stress were different for boys and free-cycling girls (F(3, 494)=9.73, p<.001), and OC users and free-cycling girls (F(3, 279)=15.12, p<.001). Unexpectedly, OC users showed no response at all but displayed linearly decreasing levels F(1, 279)=19.03, p<.001) of cortisol during the social stress test. We found no effect of menstrual cycle phase on cortisol responses to social stress (F(3, 157)=0.58, p=.55). The absence of a gender difference in the adolescents' cortisol awakening response found in this study is consistent with previous reports. Our results further suggest that adolescent OC users display slightly blunted cortisol responses after awakening, and that gender differences in cortisol responses to social stress during adolescence are comparable to those described for adult populations, that is, stronger responses in men than in women. Whereas previous work in adults suggested blunted stress responses in OC users compared to men and free-cycling women, adolescent OC users showed no cortisol response. Effects of type of OC could not be studied because of low numbers of OC that were only progestin based.


Subject(s)
Arousal/physiology , Contraceptives, Oral/therapeutic use , Hydrocortisone/metabolism , Menstrual Cycle/physiology , Stress, Psychological/metabolism , Adolescent , Blood Pressure/physiology , Data Collection , Depression/epidemiology , Depression/metabolism , Female , Heart Rate/physiology , Humans , Male , Menstrual Cycle/metabolism , Netherlands , Psychological Tests , Sex Factors , Social Behavior , Stress, Psychological/physiopathology
14.
J Sex Med ; 6(4): 1045-1053, 2009 Apr.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18761589

ABSTRACT

INTRODUCTION: The potential contribution of psychological and anatomical changes to sexual dysfunction in female patients following short-term preoperative radiotherapy (5 x 5 Gy) and total mesorectal excision (TME) is not clear. Aim. In this study we assessed female sexual dysfunction in patients who underwent radiotherapy and TME for rectal cancer. MAIN OUTCOME MEASURES: Genital arousal was assessed using vaginal videoplethysmography. METHODS: Sexual functioning was examined in four patients who had rectal cancer and underwent radiotherapy and TME. All investigations were done at least 15 months after treatment. The results were compared with an age-matched group of 18 healthy women. RESULTS: The patients and healthy controls showed comparable changes in vaginal vasocongestion during sexual arousal, though three out of four patients showed a lower mean spectral tension (MST) of the vaginal pulse compared with healthy controls. Subjective sexual arousal was equivalent between the two groups. CONCLUSIONS: In this study the changes of genital and subjective sexual arousal after erotic stimulus condition between patients and healthy controls were not different, though lower MST of the vaginal pulse was found in three out of four patients compared with healthy women. Additional work, however, must be performed to clarify the mechanisms of sexual dysfunction following treatment of rectal cancer.


Subject(s)
Plethysmography/instrumentation , Rectal Neoplasms/radiotherapy , Rectal Neoplasms/surgery , Sexual Behavior/psychology , Sexual Dysfunctions, Psychological , Videotape Recording , Aged , Combined Modality Therapy , Erotica , Female , Humans , Middle Aged , Pilot Projects , Rectal Neoplasms/epidemiology , Severity of Illness Index , Sexual Dysfunctions, Psychological/diagnosis , Sexual Dysfunctions, Psychological/epidemiology , Sexual Dysfunctions, Psychological/psychology , Surveys and Questionnaires , Vagina/innervation
15.
J Affect Disord ; 105(1-3): 185-93, 2008 Jan.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17574686

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: Stressful life events increase the probability of depressive problems in early adolescence. Several genetic and environmental risk factors may change individual sensitivity to the depressogenic effect of these events. We examined modification by parental depression and gender, and mediation of the former by temperament and family environment. METHODS: Data were collected as part of a longitudinal cohort study of (pre)adolescents (n = 2127). During the first assessment wave at approximately age 11, we assessed parental depression, family functioning, perceived parenting behaviours, and temperamental frustration and fearfulness. At the second wave, about two and a half years later, stressful life events between the first and second assessment were assessed. Depressive problems were measured at both waves. RESULTS: Adolescents with parents who had a (lifetime) depressive episode were more sensitive to the depressogenic effect of stressful events than adolescents without depressed parents. Furthermore, girls are more sensitive to these effects than boys. The modifying effect of parental depression was not mediated by temperament, family functioning and perceived parenting. LIMITATIONS: Life events were assessed without consideration of contextual information. Depressive problems were measured by questionnaires that did not directly represent DSM-IV criteria. The measure of parental depression was unspecific regarding severity and timing of depressive episodes. CONCLUSION: The results suggest that gender and parental depression are associated with increased sensitivity to depression after experiencing stressful life events during adolescence.


Subject(s)
Child of Impaired Parents/psychology , Child of Impaired Parents/statistics & numerical data , Depressive Disorder/epidemiology , Depressive Disorder/psychology , Family/psychology , Life Change Events , Parents/psychology , Social Environment , Temperament , Adolescent , Depressive Disorder/diagnosis , Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders , Female , Humans , Male , Parenting , Prevalence , Severity of Illness Index
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...