Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 16 de 16
Filter
Add more filters










Publication year range
1.
Vision Res ; 45(22): 2877-84, 2005 Oct.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16087210

ABSTRACT

We compared sensitivity to first-order versus second-order local motion in patients treated for dense central congenital cataracts in one or both eyes. Amplitude modulation thresholds were measured for discriminating the direction of motion of luminance-modulated (first-order) and contrast modulated (second-order) horizontal sine-wave gratings. Early visual deprivation, whether monocular or binocular, caused losses in sensitivity to both first- and second-order motion, with greater losses for second-order motion than for first-order motion. These findings are consistent with the hypothesis that the two types of motion are processed by different mechanisms and suggest that those mechanisms are differentially sensitive to early visual input.


Subject(s)
Cataract/physiopathology , Motion Perception/physiology , Adolescent , Adult , Child , Contrast Sensitivity/physiology , Female , Humans , Male , Photic Stimulation/methods , Sensory Thresholds/physiology , Visual Acuity
2.
Dev Neuropsychol ; 19(1): 53-81, 2001.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11411422

ABSTRACT

This article examines the effects of visual input on the development of attention by comparing normal children to children, all more than 8 years old, who had been treated for bilateral congenital cataracts during infancy. In Experiment 1, patients pushed a button as soon as they detected a target that appeared 100, 400, or 800 msec after a central cue. The cue either validly cued the upcoming location or invalidly cued the wrong location. Patients (n = 16) performed normally at the 100 msec and 400 msec stimulus onset asynchrony (SOA). However, when the cue preceded the target by the 800 msec SOA, patients' reaction times were not affected by the validity of the cue, especially when deprivation had extended past 4 months of age. In Experiment 2, patients indicated which of two shapes appeared in the periphery 400 msec after a central cue, with those shapes surrounded by compatible or incompatible distractors. Patients (n = 15) differed from age-matched controls in (a) being slowed more by incompatible distractors on invalid trials, and (b) tending to show a larger than normal effect of the validity of the cue preceding targets in the upper visual field. Together, these findings suggest that the normal development of attention is influenced by early visual experience.


Subject(s)
Attention , Cataract Extraction , Pattern Recognition, Visual , Signal Detection, Psychological , Visual Perception , Adolescent , Adult , Case-Control Studies , Cataract/congenital , Child , Child Development , Critical Period, Psychological , Cues , Female , Humans , Infant , Male , Models, Neurological , Reaction Time , Recovery of Function
4.
Vision Res ; 40(23): 3283-95, 2000.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11008144

ABSTRACT

Using the method of limits, we measured spatial and temporal vision in 15 patients, aged 4-28 years, who had been monocularly deprived of patterned visual input during infancy by a dense cataract. All patients showed losses in both spatial and temporal vision, with greater losses in spatial than in temporal vision. Losses were smaller when there had been more patching of the non-deprived eye. The results indicate that visual deprivation has smaller effects on the neural mechanisms mediating temporal vision than on those mediating spatial vision.


Subject(s)
Cataract/congenital , Child Development/physiology , Sensory Deprivation/physiology , Vision, Monocular/physiology , Visual Perception/physiology , Adolescent , Adult , Case-Control Studies , Cataract/physiopathology , Child , Child, Preschool , Contrast Sensitivity/physiology , Female , Flicker Fusion/physiology , Humans , Male , Visual Cortex/physiology
5.
Science ; 286(5437): 108-10, 1999 Oct 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10506555

ABSTRACT

Visual acuity was assessed in 28 human infants who had been deprived of all patterned visual input by cataracts in one or both eyes until they were treated at 1 week to 9 months of age. Immediately after treatment, acuity was no better than that of normal newborns. Acuity improved significantly over the next month, with some improvement apparent after as little as 1 hour of visual input. Unlike findings at older ages, the pattern of results was the same for eyes treated for monocular and for binocular deprivation. The results indicate that patterned visual input is necessary for the postnatal improvement of human visual acuity and that the onset of such input initiates rapid functional development.


Subject(s)
Photic Stimulation , Visual Acuity , Cataract/congenital , Cataract Extraction , Contact Lenses , Humans , Infant , Infant, Newborn , Matched-Pair Analysis , Pattern Recognition, Visual , Time Factors , Visual Cortex/physiology
6.
Vision Res ; 39(20): 3480-9, 1999 Oct.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10615511

ABSTRACT

Using the method of limits, we measured spatial and temporal vision in 13 children who had been deprived of patterned visual input during infancy until they were treated for dense central cataracts in both eyes. Spatial vision was assessed with vertical sine-wave gratings, and temporal vision was assessed with an unpatterned luminance field sinusoidally modulated over time. Under these testing conditions, spatial contrast sensitivity at low and medium spatial frequencies (0.33-2 c deg-1) was within normal limits, but sensitivity at higher spatial frequencies and grating acuity were reduced on average by 1.3 and 0.5 log units, respectively. Temporal vision was affected less severely, with losses in sensitivity only for low temporal frequencies (5 and 10 Hz), which averaged 0.4 log units. Thus, spatial and temporal vision are likely mediated by different neural mechanisms, that are differentially affected by deprivation.


Subject(s)
Cataract/congenital , Cataract/psychology , Sensory Deprivation , Visual Perception , Adolescent , Case-Control Studies , Cataract/physiopathology , Child , Child, Preschool , Contrast Sensitivity , Eye Injuries/physiopathology , Eye Injuries/psychology , Flicker Fusion , Humans , Sensory Thresholds , Visual Acuity , Visual Cortex/physiopathology
7.
J Pediatr Ophthalmol Strabismus ; 34(6): 347-56, 1997.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9430061

ABSTRACT

PURPOSE: We measured the extent of the monocular visual fields of children deprived of normal visual experience and examined the influence of the timing and duration of deprivation, of whether deprivation was monocular or binocular, of having patched the fellow eye, and of optical factors. METHODS: The Goldmann perimeter and a 6.4' stimulus of either 31.8 cd/m2 (target 12e) or of 318 cd/m2 (target 14e) were used to test 44 children treated for a dense and central cataract in one (n = 25) or both (n = 31) eyes that developed before 6 years of age. Then, the influence of optical factors was assessed in two adults treated for a late-onset, unilateral cataract and in two children treated for unilateral congenital cataract. RESULTS: Compared with age norms or the normal fellow eye, all children treated for cataract showed a restricted field, especially temporally, even when deprivation began as late as 6 years of age and lasted less than 6 months. The restrictions were larger than those shown by the adults who developed cataracts. The restrictions were larger after longer deprivation and monocular deprivation than after binocular deprivation. However, children who regularly patched the fellow nondeprived eye and, therefore, experienced less interocular competition, exhibited smaller restrictions temporally. Neither visual acuity nor optical factors could account for all of the restrictions in the deprived children. CONCLUSIONS: The development of the visual field is vulnerable to the effects of deprivation, especially to unilateral deprivation and to long deprivation. The losses likely reflect alterations in the visual pathways subserving peripheral vision.


Subject(s)
Sensory Deprivation/physiology , Vision, Low/physiopathology , Visual Fields/physiology , Adult , Cataract/complications , Cataract/physiopathology , Child, Preschool , Humans , Infant , Photic Stimulation , Time Factors , Vision, Binocular/physiology , Vision, Low/etiology , Vision, Monocular/physiology , Visual Acuity , Visual Field Tests
8.
Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci ; 36(10): 2080-95, 1995 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-7657547

ABSTRACT

PURPOSE: To study the development of grating acuity in children treated for dense congenital unilateral or bilateral cataract and to examine how variations in treatment affect grating acuity during early childhood. METHODS: The authors used optokinetic nystagmus (OKN), preferential looking (PL), or both to measure the grating acuity of children treated for congenital cataract in one eye (n = 63) or both eyes (n = 77) whenever possible from the time of treatment until 3 years of age. At each age, the authors compared patients' monocular acuity to that of children with no history of eye disorders. RESULTS: The OKN acuity of treated eyes did not improve with age and was abnormal by 12 months of age. In contrast, PL acuity improved with age, and acuity of most treated eyes was not outside normal limits until 24 to 30 months of age. Nonetheless, at 12 months and at 3 years of age, PL acuity correlated significantly with age at treatment in children who had bilateral cataract. In children who had unilateral cataract, PL acuity correlated significantly with the number of hours per day the good eye had been patched since treatment. Children whose good eye was patched fewer than 3 hours per day did significantly worse than children treated at a comparable age for bilateral congenital cataract. However, children whose good eye was patched at least 3 hours per day had PL acuities similar to those of children treated at a comparable age for bilateral congenital cataract. CONCLUSIONS: Children treated for congenital cataract show deficits in grating acuity, with the deficit apparent earlier in OKN acuity than in PL acuity. At least by 1 year of age, visual development has begun to be influenced by the age at treatment and, in children treated for unilateral cataract, by patching of the good eye.


Subject(s)
Cataract Extraction , Cataract/congenital , Cataract/physiopathology , Visual Acuity/physiology , Aging/physiology , Aphakia, Postcataract/physiopathology , Child, Preschool , Contact Lenses , Humans , Infant , Nystagmus, Optokinetic/physiology , Sensory Deprivation , Vision, Ocular/physiology
9.
Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci ; 34(13): 3501-9, 1993 Dec.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8258506

ABSTRACT

PURPOSE: To determine if form-deprived aphakes, like normal infants, show especially poor sensitivity in the nasal visual field. The purpose of this article was also to examine the influence on their peripheral sensitivity of the timing and duration of deprivation, of whether deprivation was monocular or binocular, and of having patched the fellow nondeprived eye. METHODS: Static perimetry was used to measure intensity thresholds at 20 degrees in the nasal visual field and at 30 degrees in the temporal visual field in normal subjects (n = 20 7-year-old children, 20 8-year-old children, 12 9-year-old children, and 20 adults) and in 46 children treated for a dense and central cataract in one (n = 21) or both (n = 25) eyes. The deprivation began either at birth or after a normal early history, and the duration of deprivation varied widely among patients. Also tested were two adults who had been treated promptly for unilateral cataracts that had developed after the age of 40 years. RESULTS: In normal subjects, sensitivity was slightly higher at 20 degrees nasally than at 30 degrees temporally, with no developmental changes in sensitivity at either location. The deprived eyes of the children had losses in sensitivity at both locations but only children treated for unilateral congenital cataract had larger losses at 20 degrees nasally than at 30 degrees temporally. There were no significant effects on their sensitivity of the duration of deprivation or, in children treated for unilateral cataract, of patching of the nondeprived eye. In contrast, the two patients in whom cataracts did not develop until adulthood had normal sensitivity. CONCLUSIONS: Pattern deprivation interferes with the development of peripheral sensitivity at both 30 degrees temporally and 20 degrees nasally. Nasal sensitivity, which is slow to mature, is affected more by early monocular deprivation than by early binocular deprivation. The results are consistent with the hypothesis that unfair interocular competition during early infancy especially affects visual functions that are slow to mature.


Subject(s)
Aphakia, Postcataract/physiopathology , Cataract/physiopathology , Cataract/therapy , Visual Fields/physiology , Adolescent , Adult , Cataract/congenital , Child , Contact Lenses , Humans , Middle Aged , Reference Values , Sensory Deprivation/physiology , Sensory Thresholds , Vision, Monocular/physiology , Visual Field Tests
10.
Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci ; 34(5): 1767-73, 1993 Apr.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8473116

ABSTRACT

PURPOSE: The authors determined whether stereopsis can be demonstrated in children treated for congenital cataract after compensating for their amblyopia and strabismus. METHODS: A custom-made set of large stereograms was used to compensate for amblyopia and presented in a synoptophore to facilitate motor fusion. Each pair of stereograms contained five wide vertical bars of high contrast, of which two or three were in crossed disparity. The authors aligned the stimuli in the synoptophore, then decreased the disparity until the child could no longer identify which bars appeared to be "in front." RESULTS: In normal children (n = 25), stereo acuity on this test (Tytla-Lewis-Maurer-Brent [TLMB] test) correlated well with the Titmus and Randot results within the ranges measured by those tests. Seven children in whom a traumatic cataract had developed after 6 yr of age (four with no clinical stereopsis) had TLMB test stereo acuities ranging from 225 to 28 arc-sec. Of 30 children treated for congenital cataract with no clinical stereopsis, 5 regularly achieved a TLMB stereo acuity of up to 225 arc-sec, and 2 with Titmus stereo acuities up to 200 arc-sec, each had a TLMB stereo acuity of 112 arc-sec. These seven congenital cases (two unilateral and five bilateral) with measurable TLMB acuities are among those with the shortest deprivation, the highest minimum resolvable acuity, and the highest contrast sensitivity. CONCLUSIONS: By compensating for amblyopia and strabismus, stereopsis can be demonstrated in some form-deprived amblyopic patients.


Subject(s)
Cataract/congenital , Cataract/physiopathology , Depth Perception/physiology , Adolescent , Amblyopia/therapy , Cataract/etiology , Cataract Extraction , Child , Child, Preschool , Eye Injuries/complications , Humans , Lens, Crystalline/injuries , Sensory Deprivation , Strabismus/therapy , Visual Acuity
11.
Ophthalmology ; 99(7): 1013-7, 1992 Jul.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-1495777

ABSTRACT

PURPOSE: The authors examined linear acuity and contrast sensitivity in the "good" (fellow) eye of children treated for unilateral congenital cataract to determine (1) whether there were subtle deficits like those described for the good eye of patients with strabismic amblyopia and (2) whether any deficits were related to the degree to which the good eye had been patched. METHODS: The authors tested patients treated for unilateral congenital cataract who had a well-documented history of patching. The good eye was physically normal with minimal refractive error. The measures were linear acuity at far (n = 15) and contrast sensitivity (n = 9). RESULTS: Compared with age-matched control subjects, the good eyes of patients had subtle deficits in linear acuity and in contrast sensitivity at high spatial frequencies. These deficits occurred even in eyes that had received minimal patching, and their severity was not related systematically to the duration of patching, which varied widely across the group. CONCLUSION: The visual sensitivity of the good eye of children treated for unilateral congenital cataract is, on average, slightly reduced, even in cases of minimal patching. Consequently, any deficits discovered after aggressive patching may not have been caused by occlusion amblyopia.


Subject(s)
Cataract Extraction , Cataract/congenital , Ocular Physiological Phenomena , Visual Acuity/physiology , Adolescent , Amblyopia/physiopathology , Amblyopia/therapy , Aphakia, Postcataract/physiopathology , Aphakia, Postcataract/therapy , Child , Child, Preschool , Contact Lenses , Contrast Sensitivity/physiology , Humans
12.
J Pediatr Ophthalmol Strabismus ; 27(5): 229-32, 1990.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-2246733

ABSTRACT

Fifty-two cases of developmental cataracts extracted using the pars plicata lensectomy/vitrectomy technique were reviewed. Eyes with additional ocular anomaly other than microphthalmos were not included. No early complications were detected in a postoperative period ranging from 1.5 to 7 years with a median of 4 years, and late complications were limited to secondary membranes occurring in small eyes. Since secondary membranes occurred only in eyes in which the axial length was less than or equal to 17.4 mm and the corneal diameter was less than or equal to 9.5 mm, we suggest that eyes that are small by the absolute dimensions cited here are at greater risk of developing secondary membranes postoperatively. Small eyes by absolute dimensions should be distinguished from microphthalmic eyes, since the relative term microphthalmic is less predictive of risk of complication than are absolute dimensions. Linear Snellen acuity in 15 patients capable of response ranged as follows: 20/20-20/80 with a median of 20/40 in eyes with partial bilateral cataracts; 20/25-20/80 with a median of 20/50 in eyes with complete bilateral cataracts; 20/30-20/400 with a median of 20/200 in eyes with unilateral partial cataracts; and 20/60-CF with a median of 20/400 in eyes with complete unilateral cataracts. Mean patient ages at surgery were 3 months for those with bilateral complete cataracts, 5 months for those with unilateral complete cataracts, 18 months for those with bilateral partial cataracts, and 25 months for those with unilateral partial cataracts. The earliest possible removal of visually significant opacities must be combined with aggressive postoperative visual rehabilitation to obtain the best possible visual outcome.


Subject(s)
Cataract Extraction/methods , Lens, Crystalline/surgery , Vitrectomy/methods , Cataract Extraction/adverse effects , Child, Preschool , Follow-Up Studies , Humans , Infant , Infant, Newborn , Prognosis , Visual Acuity
13.
Can J Psychol ; 43(2): 121-40, 1989 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-2486492

ABSTRACT

Ten years ago we proposed that the limitations on young infants' vision might be caused by an immature Y-pathway through the cortex, while their abilities might be mediated by an X-pathway to the cortex and by Y- and/or W-pathways to the superior colliculus and pretectum (Maurer & Lewis, 1979). Although that explanation was too simple overall, it fits well with what is known about asymmetrical optokinetic nystagmus, viz. the difficulty in eliciting OKN to patterns moving from the nasal field toward the temporal field. In this paper, we describe the development of symmetrical OKN, its alteration by early deprivation from cataract, and its physiological basis. We then suggest that, for primates, an explanation based on projections through the magnocellular versus parvocellular layers of the lateral geniculate nucleus may be more appropriate than one based on X-, Y-, and W-cells.


Subject(s)
Cataract/physiopathology , Nystagmus, Physiologic/physiology , Sensory Deprivation/physiology , Visual Cortex/physiopathology , Animals , Cataract/therapy , Cats , Child, Preschool , Humans , Infant , Visual Pathways/physiology
14.
Br J Ophthalmol ; 70(3): 214-20, 1986 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-3954980

ABSTRACT

We measured three aspects of vision in children treated for unilateral congenital cataract: visual resolution, the symmetry of optokinetic nystagmus (OKN), and peripheral vision. Good visual resolution was achieved by children who had had the earliest treatment and who had had the normal eye patched close to 50% of the waking time throughout early childhood. All children treated for unilateral congenital cataract showed a marked asymmetry of OKN regardless of the age of treatment. One child with early treatment who could be tested with the Goldmann perimeter also showed especially poor sensitivity in the nasal visual field of her aphakic eye. We found no such deficits in the vision of children who had had normal visual experience during early infancy and then later developed cataracts in one or both eyes. The limitations observed in children treated for congenital cataract are similar to those reported in normal human infants, in normal kittens, and in cats which were visually deprived early in life.


Subject(s)
Sensory Deprivation/physiology , Visual Perception/physiology , Adolescent , Aphakia, Postcataract/physiopathology , Cataract/congenital , Child , Female , Humans , Infant , Nystagmus, Physiologic , Sensory Thresholds/physiology , Time Factors , Visual Acuity , Visual Fields
15.
Behav Brain Res ; 10(1): 151-61, 1983 Oct.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-6639723

ABSTRACT

The vision of cats which were monocularly deprived during early infancy, of kittens, and of young human infants shares two limitations: detection in the nasal visual field is far poorer than detection in the temporal visual field, and optokinetic nystagmus (OKN) is difficult to elicit when a pattern moves nasally to temporally. Here we report similar limitations on the vision of children who had a dense central cataract in one eye during early infancy. Extensive static perimetry with one of these children whose visual acuity was good in both eyes revealed that her threshold for detection all along the horizontal meridian was higher in her aphakic than in her normal eye, with this difference much more pronounced in the nasal visual field than in the temporal visual field. Three children who developed cataracts after 6 months of age showed no such discrepancy between thresholds in the temporal and nasal fields. We tested the symmetry of OKN in 12 children treated for unilateral congenital cataract. In every test of an aphakic (n = 4) or normal eye (n = 12), OKN occurred significantly more often when stripes moved temporally to nasally than when they moved nasally to temporally. In contrast, no asymmetry was observed in any of 13 children treated for traumatic cataracts incurred after 3 years of age. We conclude that children treated for unilateral congenital cataract, like young human infants and monocularly deprived cats, show asymmetric OKN and relatively poor detection in the nasal visual field.


Subject(s)
Cataract/congenital , Nystagmus, Physiologic , Vision, Ocular/physiology , Animals , Aphakia, Postcataract/physiopathology , Cataract/etiology , Cataract/physiopathology , Cats , Humans , Infant , Lens, Crystalline/injuries , Sensory Deprivation/physiology , Visual Fields
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...