Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 20 de 25
Filter
Add more filters










Publication year range
1.
Neurosci Lett ; 728: 134970, 2020 05 29.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32302700

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: Gabor patterns are defined as the product of a sinusoid function and a Gaussian envelope and are commonly used in visual and attentional research due to their ability to selectively stimulate the primary visual cortex. The aim of this study was to investigate whether Gabor patterns can be used as visual stimuli in the rodent continuous performance test (rCPT), a newly developed task to study attentional function and impulsivity. METHODS: Sixteen male C57BL/6 J mice were trained in the rCPT using Gabor patterns as visual stimuli and their performance was compared to sixteen mice that were trained using traditional high-contrast pattern stimuli. Mice were compared during training, baseline, and a variable stimulus duration probe. RESULTS: The Gabor pattern group required more training sessions to reach criteria than the group with high-contrast patterns. At baseline, the Gabor pattern group showed a higher false alarm rate and a lower discriminability index. As task difficulty increased during the variable stimulus duration probe, differences between groups became more pronounced. Specifically, the Gabor pattern group showed decreased hit rate and discriminability index, as well as increased false alarm rate and premature responses compared to the high-contrast pattern group. CONCLUSION: This feasibility study showed that it is possible to use Gabor patterns as visual stimuli in the rCPT, although it increases task demands. We discuss the differences between Gabor patterns and high-contrast patterns in the context of translatability of animal models in visual and cognitive research and give two examples of applicability.


Subject(s)
Attention/physiology , Central Nervous System Stimulants/pharmacology , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Photic Stimulation , Visual Perception/drug effects , Animals , Impulsive Behavior/drug effects , Male , Mice, Inbred C57BL , Photic Stimulation/methods
2.
Psychopharmacology (Berl) ; 234(5): 845-855, 2017 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28070619

ABSTRACT

RATIONALE: The 5-choice serial reaction time task (5-CSRTT) is widely used to measure rodent attentional functions. In humans, many attention studies in healthy and clinical populations have used testing based on Bundesen's Theory of Visual Attention (TVA) to estimate visual processing speeds and other parameters of attentional capacity. OBJECTIVES: We aimed to bridge these research fields by modifying the 5-CSRTT's design and by mathematically modelling data to derive attentional parameters analogous to human TVA-based measures. METHODS: C57BL/6 mice were tested in two 1-h sessions on consecutive days with a version of the 5-CSRTT where stimulus duration (SD) probe length was varied based on information from previous TVA studies. Thereafter, a scopolamine hydrobromide (HBr; 0.125 or 0.25 mg/kg) pharmacological challenge was undertaken, using a Latin square design. Mean score values were modelled using a new three-parameter version of TVA to obtain estimates of visual processing speeds, visual thresholds and motor response baselines in each mouse. RESULTS: The parameter estimates for each animal were reliable across sessions, showing that the data were stable enough to support analysis on an individual level. Scopolamine HBr dose-dependently reduced 5-CSRTT attentional performance while also increasing reward collection latency at the highest dose. Upon TVA modelling, scopolamine HBr significantly reduced visual processing speed at both doses, while having less pronounced effects on visual thresholds and motor response baselines. CONCLUSIONS: This study shows for the first time how 5-CSRTT performance in mice can be mathematically modelled to yield estimates of attentional capacity that are directly comparable to estimates from human studies.


Subject(s)
Attention/physiology , Choice Behavior/physiology , Reaction Time/physiology , Visual Perception/physiology , Animals , Attention/drug effects , Behavior, Animal , Choice Behavior/drug effects , Cholinergic Antagonists/pharmacology , Male , Mice , Mice, Inbred C57BL , Models, Theoretical , Psychological Theory , Reaction Time/drug effects , Reward , Scopolamine/pharmacology , Visual Perception/drug effects
3.
Percept Psychophys ; 63(1): 85-98, 2001 Jan.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11304019

ABSTRACT

Shiffrin and Schneider (1977, Experiment 4d) reported that after consistent training in search for particular alphanumeric characters, presentation of one of these characters (former targets) as a distractor impeded detection of simultaneously presented current targets. Even if presented in an irrelevant display location, the former target appeared to attract attention. Here, we analyze weaknesses in the design of Experiment 4d and report four follow-up experiments ranging from a fairly close replication of the original multiframe experiment to a rather conventional single-frame search study. In each experiment, presentation of former targets consistently impeded detection of simultaneously presented current targets. The results suggest that automatic attention attraction to individual alphanumeric characters develops not only in the special experimental paradigm used by Shiffrin and Schneider, but also in standard visual search tasks. The fact that attention appeared to be attracted by shapes as complex as individual letters supports the assumption that simultaneously presented visual stimuli can be compared in parallel against memory representations of alphanumeric characters.


Subject(s)
Attention , Linguistics , Visual Perception , Adult , Female , Humans , Male , Signal Detection, Psychological
4.
J Cogn Neurosci ; 12(5): 763-74, 2000 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11054919

ABSTRACT

Visual comparison between different-sized objects with respect to shape can be done by encoding one of the objects as a mental image, transforming the image to the size format of the other object, and then testing for a match (Bundesen, C., & Larsen, A. [1975]. Visual transformation of size. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 1, 214-220). To identify the brain structures implicated in mental transformation of size, we measured the distribution of regional cerebral blood flow (rCBF) by positron emission tomography (PET) in 12 normal subjects who compared random stimulus patterns with respect to shape regardless of variations in size in a one-back match-to-sample paradigm. Each subject was PET-scanned 12 times during repetitive injections of H(2)(15)O. In one condition (three scans), all stimulus patterns were small. In a second condition (three scans), all stimuli were large. In the third condition (six scans), the stimuli alternated between small and large. Mental transformation of size should occur in the alternating-size condition but not in the fixed-size conditions. As expected, behavioral measures (reaction time [RT], d', beta) were nearly the same for the two fixed-size conditions but mean RT was longer and d' smaller in the alternating-size condition. Changes in rCBF specific to mental transformation of size were estimated by contrasting the alternating-size with the fixed-size conditions by use of statistical parametric mapping (SPM96) at a threshold of p <. 05 corrected for multiple comparisons. The detected brain structures implicated in mental transformation of size were primarily located in the dorsal pathways, comprising structures in the occipital, parietal, and temporal transition zone (predominantly in the left hemisphere), posterior parietal cortex (bilaterally), area MT/V5 (left), and vermis (bilaterally). Contrasts between the two fixed-size conditions showed significant effects in only the occipital cortex.


Subject(s)
Brain/physiology , Space Perception/physiology , Adult , Behavior/physiology , Brain/diagnostic imaging , Brain Mapping , Cerebrovascular Circulation/physiology , Female , Humans , Male , Neuropsychological Tests , Reference Values , Tomography, Emission-Computed
5.
Psychol Res ; 62(2-3): 79-80, 1999.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10472195
6.
J Exp Psychol Gen ; 128(4): 450-78, 1999 Dec.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10650583

ABSTRACT

A variety of impairments in visual attention can follow damage to the brain. The authors develop systematic methods for analyzing such impairments in terms of C. Bundesen's (1990) Theory of Visual Attention and apply these in a group of 9 patients with parietal lobe lesions and variable spatial neglect. In whole report, patients report letters from brief, vertical arrays in left or right visual field. The results show substantial, largely bilateral impairments in processing capacity, implying a major nonlateralized aspect to neglect. In partial report, arrays contain 1 or 2 letters in red and/or green. The task is to report only those letters in a specified target color. In addition to the expected bias against left-sided letters, patients show striking, bilateral preservation of top-down control, or attentional priority for targets. The results show how differentiation of attentional impairments can be informed by a theory of normal function.


Subject(s)
Agnosia , Attention , Cerebral Infarction/psychology , Parietal Lobe/pathology , Visual Perception , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Cerebral Hemorrhage/psychology , Color Perception , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Models, Neurological , Visual Fields
7.
Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci ; 353(1373): 1271-81, 1998 Aug 29.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9770221

ABSTRACT

A computational theory of visual attention is presented. The basic theory (TVA) combines the biased-choice model for single-stimulus recognition with the fixed-capacity independent race model (FIRM) for selection from multi-element displays. TVA organizes a large body of experimental findings on performance in visual recognition and attention tasks. A recent development (CTVA) combines TVA with a theory of perceptual grouping by proximity. CTVA explains effects of perceptual grouping and spatial distance between items in multi-element displays. A new account of spatial focusing is proposed in this paper. The account provides a framework for understanding visual search as an interplay between serial and parallel processes.


Subject(s)
Attention/physiology , Computer Simulation , Models, Biological , Models, Theoretical , Visual Perception/physiology , Humans
8.
J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform ; 24(3): 719-31, 1998 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9627411

ABSTRACT

Effects of the spatial separation between 2 simultaneously presented random patterns on the ability to determine whether the patterns are identical up to a translational displacement across the retina were investigated by using signal-detection methods. The patterns to be compared were presented on the periphery of an imaginary circle centered on fixation. Exposures were brief and postmasked. In Experiments 1 and 2, sensitivity (d') varied with exposure duration but not with the spatial separation between the patterns. In Experiment 3, the task was changed so that members of a "same" pair could differ by both a translation and a rotation. When the rotational component was O, d' was a monotonic decreasing function of the spatial separation between the stimuli. Apparently, in the special case, performance was based on mental alignment by a process of gradual mental translation of one of the members of a stimulus pair to the location of the other one.


Subject(s)
Mental Processes/physiology , Space Perception/physiology , Visual Perception/physiology , Adolescent , Adult , Female , Humans , Male
9.
Percept Psychophys ; 59(5): 714-20, 1997 Jul.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9259638

ABSTRACT

Subjects were presented with briefly exposed visual displays of words that were common first names with a length of four to six letters. In the main experiment, each display consisted of four words: two names shown in red and two shown in white. The subject's task was to report the red names (targets), but ignore the white ones (distractors). On some trials the subject's own name appeared as a display item (target or distractor). Presentation of the subject's name as a distractor caused no more interference with report of targets than did presentation of other names as distractors. Apparently, visual attention was not automatically attracted by the subject's own name.


Subject(s)
Attention , Automatism , Names , Visual Perception , Adolescent , Adult , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Perceptual Masking
10.
Mem Cognit ; 24(2): 136-43, 1996 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8881318

ABSTRACT

Psychological data suggest that internal representations such as mental images can be used as templates in visual pattern recognition. But computational studies suggest that traditional template matching is insufficient for high-accuracy recognition of real-life patterns such as handwritten characters. Here we explore a model for visual pattern recognition that combines a template-matching and a feature-analysis approach: Character classification is based on weighted evidence from a number of analyzers (demons), each of which computes the degree of match between the input character and a stored template (a copy of a previously presented character). The template-matching pandemonium was trained to recognize totally unconstrained handwritten digits. With a mean of 37 templates per type of digit, the system has attained a recognition rate of 95.3%, which falls short of human performance by only 2%-3%.


Subject(s)
Discrimination Learning , Handwriting , Pattern Recognition, Visual , Software , Algorithms , Humans , Neural Networks, Computer , Reaction Time
12.
Percept Psychophys ; 50(1): 87-9, 1991 Jul.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-1881770

ABSTRACT

Nissen (1985) compared selection by location with selection by color or shape in partial-report experiments. Her analysis of response contingencies when a target was defined in terms of one attribute (location, color, or shape), and when the task was to report the two remaining attributes, suggested a special role for selection by location: It appeared that cross-referencing between color and shape was mediated by location. An alternative interpretation is developed here: The findings are explained by a theory of attention (Bundesen, 1990), in which selection by location is treated on a par with selection by color or shape.


Subject(s)
Attention , Color Perception , Form Perception , Discrimination Learning , Humans , Psychophysics
13.
Psychol Rev ; 97(4): 523-47, 1990 Oct.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-2247540

ABSTRACT

A unified theory of visual recognition and attentional selection is developed by integrating the biased-choice model for single-stimulus recognition (Luce, 1963; Shepard, 1957) with a choice model for selection from multielement displays (Bundesen, Pedersen, & Larsen, 1984) in a race model framework. Mathematically, the theory is tractable, and it specifies the computations necessary for selection. The theory is applied to extant findings from a broad range of experimental paradigms. The findings include effects of object integrality in selective report, number and spatial position of targets in divided-attention paradigms, selection criterion and number of distracters in focused-attention paradigms, delay of selection cue in partial report, and consistent practice in search. On the whole, the quantitative fits are encouraging.


Subject(s)
Attention , Discrimination Learning , Pattern Recognition, Visual , Humans , Models, Theoretical , Orientation , Psychophysics , Reaction Time
14.
J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform ; 14(4): 591-600, 1988 Nov.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-2974870

ABSTRACT

In a partial-report experiment, subjects reported the digits from a circular array of digits and letters terminated by a pattern mask. Individual frequency distributions of the number of correctly reported digits were analyzed as functions of number of digits (2, 4, or 6) and number of letters (0, 2, 4, 6, or 8) at nine exposure durations ranging from 10 to 200 ms. The distributions (hundreds of data points per subject) were accurately predicted by a four-parameter fixed-capacity independent race model that assumes exponentially distributed processing times, limitations in both processing capacity and storage capacity, and time-invariant selectivity. Estimated from the data, processing capacity C was 45 items/s, selectivity alpha (ratio between the amount of processing capacity devoted to a distractor and the amount devoted to a target) was 0.48, short-term storage capacity K was 3.5 items, and the longest ineffective exposure duration t0 was 18 ms.


Subject(s)
Form Perception , Models, Psychological , Pattern Recognition, Visual , Adult , Female , Humans , Male , Models, Statistical , Perceptual Masking , Time Factors
16.
J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform ; 10(3): 329-39, 1984 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-6242410

ABSTRACT

In the proposed model for partial report, performance reflects the number of targets in a short-term memory buffer. The total number of items (targets, distractors, or extraneous noise) entering the buffer is independent of the number of targets and distractors in the stimulus. Entrance is determined by selective sampling according to a Luce (1959) ratio rule. The model was tested in a variety of conditions with partial reports based on brightness, color, shape, or alphanumeric class. With three parameters, the model accounted for 99% of the variance with number of targets and number of distractors in data obtained by averaging across conditions. Parameter K (number of items entering the buffer) showed little variation with the selection criterion, and estimates for parameter epsilon (total impact of extraneous noise with impact per target as the unit) were rather small. Estimates for parameter alpha (impact per distractor with impact per target as the unit) varied widely across conditions. Parameter alpha is a measure for the efficiency of selecting targets rather than distractors.


Subject(s)
Memory, Short-Term , Visual Perception , Adult , Color Perception , Form Perception , Humans , Male , Models, Psychological , Perceptual Masking
19.
Perception ; 12(5): 549-58, 1983.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-6676705

ABSTRACT

Sequential alternation between same-shaped stimuli differing in size (size ratio s) and orientation (angular difference v) produced a visual illusion of translation in depth and concurrent rotation. The minimum stimulus-onset asynchrony required for the appearance of a rigidly moving object was approximately a linearly increasing function of (s-1)/(s+1) for simple translation in depth and a linearly increasing function of v for simple rotation. The extrapolated zero intercept was lower for translation than for rotation, but estimated transformation times were additive in combined transformations. The results suggest that (a) the processes of apparent translation in depth and apparent rotation are individually sequential-additive in structure, and (b) apparent translations and rotations are combined by fine-grained alternation of steps of apparent translation and steps of apparent rotation. Similar principles account for recent data on imagined spatial transformations of visual size and orientation.


Subject(s)
Illusions , Motion Perception , Optical Illusions , Orientation , Size Perception , Depth Perception , Discrimination Learning , Form Perception , Humans , Psychophysics , Sensory Thresholds
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...