Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 20 de 28
Filter
Add more filters










Publication year range
1.
Transl Psychiatry ; 5: e570, 2015 May 26.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26080087

ABSTRACT

The ability to recognize facial expressions of emotion in others is a cornerstone of human interaction. Selective impairments in the recognition of facial expressions of fear have frequently been reported in chronic cocaine users, but the nature of these impairments remains poorly understood. We used the multivariate method of partial least squares and structural magnetic resonance imaging to identify gray matter brain networks that underlie facial affect processing in both cocaine-dependent (n = 29) and healthy male volunteers (n = 29). We hypothesized that disruptions in neuroendocrine function in cocaine-dependent individuals would explain their impairments in fear recognition by modulating the relationship with the underlying gray matter networks. We found that cocaine-dependent individuals not only exhibited significant impairments in the recognition of fear, but also for facial expressions of anger. Although recognition accuracy of threatening expressions co-varied in all participants with distinctive gray matter networks implicated in fear and anger processing, in cocaine users it was less well predicted by these networks than in controls. The weaker brain-behavior relationships for threat processing were also mediated by distinctly different factors. Fear recognition impairments were influenced by variations in intelligence levels, whereas anger recognition impairments were associated with comorbid opiate dependence and related reduction in testosterone levels. We also observed an inverse relationship between testosterone levels and the duration of crack and opiate use. Our data provide novel insight into the neurobiological basis of abnormal threat processing in cocaine dependence, which may shed light on new opportunities facilitating the psychosocial integration of these patients.


Subject(s)
Cocaine-Related Disorders/physiopathology , Crack Cocaine , Facial Expression , Facial Recognition/physiology , Gray Matter/pathology , Testosterone/blood , Adult , Anger , Case-Control Studies , Cocaine-Related Disorders/blood , Cocaine-Related Disorders/pathology , Cocaine-Related Disorders/psychology , Fear , Humans , Hydrocortisone/blood , Intelligence , Least-Squares Analysis , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Male , Middle Aged , Neural Pathways/pathology , Opioid-Related Disorders/blood , Opioid-Related Disorders/pathology , Opioid-Related Disorders/physiopathology , Opioid-Related Disorders/psychology , Young Adult
2.
Psychol Med ; 44(15): 3215-27, 2014 Nov.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25065819

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: Mentalizing deficits are a hallmark of the autism spectrum condition (ASC) and a potential endophenotype for atypical social cognition in ASC. Differences in performance and neural activation on the 'Reading the Mind in the Eyes' task (the Eyes task) have been identified in individuals with ASC in previous studies. METHOD: Performance on the Eyes task along with the associated neural activation was examined in adolescents with ASC (n = 50), their unaffected siblings (n = 40) and typically developing controls (n = 40). Based on prior literature that males and females with ASC display different cognitive and associated neural characteristics, analyses were stratified by sex. Three strategies were applied to test for endophenotypes at the level of neural activation: (1) identifying and locating conjunctions of ASC-control and sibling-control differences; (2) examining whether the sibling group is comparable to the ASC or intermediate between the ASC and control groups; and (3) examining spatial overlaps between ASC-control and sibling-control differences across multiple thresholds. RESULTS: Impaired behavioural performance on the Eyes task was observed in males with ASC compared to controls, but only at trend level in females; and no difference in performance was identified between sibling and same-sex control groups in both sexes. Neural activation showed a substantial endophenotype effect in the female groups but this was only modest in the male groups. CONCLUSIONS: Behavioural impairment on complex emotion recognition associated with mental state attribution is a phenotypic, rather than an endophenotypic, marker of ASC. However, the neural response during the Eyes task is a potential endophenotypic marker for ASC, particularly in females.


Subject(s)
Brain/physiopathology , Child Development Disorders, Pervasive/physiopathology , Facial Expression , Siblings , Theory of Mind/physiology , Adolescent , Endophenotypes , Eye , Female , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Male , Sex Factors
3.
Biol Lett ; 9(6): 20130633, 2013.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24307526

ABSTRACT

Recently, the importance of skin colour for facial attractiveness has been recognized. In particular, dietary carotenoid-induced skin colour has been proposed as a signal of health and therefore attractiveness. While perceptual results are highly consistent, it is currently not clear whether carotenoid skin colour is preferred because it poses a cue to current health condition in humans or whether it is simply seen as a more aesthetically pleasing colour, independently of skin-specific signalling properties. Here, we tested this question by comparing attractiveness ratings of faces to corresponding ratings of meaningless scrambled face images matching the colours and contrasts found in the face. We produced sets of face and non-face stimuli with either healthy (high-carotenoid coloration) or unhealthy (low-carotenoid coloration) colour and asked participants for attractiveness ratings. Results showed that, while for faces increased carotenoid coloration significantly improved attractiveness, there was no equivalent effect on perception of scrambled images. These findings are consistent with a specific signalling system of current condition through skin coloration in humans and indicate that preferences are not caused by sensory biases in observers.


Subject(s)
Beauty , Carotenoids/chemistry , Face/physiology , Skin Pigmentation , Skin/anatomy & histology , Skin/metabolism , Adolescent , Adult , Cues , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Photic Stimulation/methods , Prejudice , Random Allocation , Recognition, Psychology , Young Adult
4.
Transl Psychiatry ; 1: e19, 2011 Jul 12.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22832521

ABSTRACT

Siblings of individuals with autism have over 20 times the population risk of autism. Evidence of comparable, but less marked, cognitive and social communication deficits in siblings suggests a role for these traits in the search for biomarkers of familial risk. However, no neuroimaging biomarkers of familial risk have been identified to date. Here we show, for the first time, that the neural response to facial expression of emotion differs between unaffected siblings and healthy controls with no family history of autism. Strikingly, the functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) response to happy versus neutral faces was significantly reduced in unaffected siblings compared with controls within a number of brain areas implicated in empathy and face processing. The response in unaffected siblings did not differ significantly from the response in autism. Furthermore, investigation of the response to faces versus fixation crosses suggested that, within the context of this study, an atypical response specifically to happy faces, rather than to faces in general, accounts for the observed sibling versus controls difference and is a clear biomarker of familial risk. Our findings suggest that an atypical implicit response to facial expression of emotion may form the basis of impaired emotional reactivity in autism and in the broader autism phenotype in relatives. These results demonstrate that the fMRI response to facial expression of emotion is a candidate neuroimaging endophenotype for autism, and may offer far-reaching insights into the etiology of autism.


Subject(s)
Autistic Disorder/physiopathology , Brain/physiopathology , Emotions/physiology , Endophenotypes , Facial Expression , Magnetic Resonance Imaging/methods , Adolescent , Autistic Disorder/genetics , Biomarkers , Child , Female , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging/instrumentation , Male , Risk , Siblings
5.
Emotion ; 10(5): 605-14, 2010 Oct.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21038944

ABSTRACT

Emotional stimuli receive high processing priority in attention and memory. This processing "advantage" is generally thought to be predominantly mediated by arousal. However, recent data suggest that ratings of an image's affective "impact" may be a better predictor of recollection than arousal or valence. One interpretation of these findings is that high-impact images may draw an individual's attention, thus facilitating subsequent processing. We investigated the allocation of visual attention to negative emotional images that differed in impact but were matched for valence, arousal, and other characteristics. Participants viewed a central image flanked by 2 neutral indoor or outdoor scenes and made speeded judgments about whether the neutral scenes matched. In Experiment 1, responses were slower on high-impact relative to low-impact or neutral trials. In Experiment 2, responses on high-arousal relative to low-arousal trials did not differ significantly. These data provide evidence for differential allocation of attention to distinct sets of negative, equally arousing images, and argue against the prevailing view that heightened attention to and processing of emotional stimuli relate simply to arousal or valence.


Subject(s)
Arousal , Attention , Emotions , Adolescent , Adult , Female , Humans , Male , Visual Perception , Young Adult
6.
Psychol Med ; 40(11): 1919-29, 2010 Nov.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20102666

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: Previous behavioural and neuroimaging studies of emotion processing in autistic spectrum disorder (ASD) have focused on the use of facial stimuli. To date, however, no studies have examined emotion processing in autism across a broad range of social signals. METHOD: This study addressed this issue by investigating emotion processing in a group of 23 adults with ASD and 23 age- and gender-matched controls. Recognition of basic emotions ('happiness', 'sadness', 'anger', disgust' and 'fear') was assessed from facial, body movement and vocal stimuli. The ability to make social judgements (such as approachability) from facial stimuli was also investigated. RESULTS: Significant deficits in emotion recognition were found in the ASD group relative to the control group across all stimulus domains (faces, body movements and voices). These deficits were seen across a range of emotions. The ASD group were also impaired in making social judgements compared to the control group and this correlated with impairments in basic emotion recognition. CONCLUSIONS: This study demonstrates that there are significant and broad-ranging deficits in emotion processing in ASD present across a range of stimulus domains and in the auditory and visual modality; they cannot therefore be accounted for simply in terms of impairments in face processing or in the visual modality alone. These results identify a core deficit affecting the processing of a wide range of emotional information in ASD, which contributes to the impairments in social function seen in people with this condition.


Subject(s)
Child Development Disorders, Pervasive/psychology , Emotional Intelligence , Adult , Case-Control Studies , Child , Child Development Disorders, Pervasive/physiopathology , Cognition , Emotions , Facial Expression , Female , Humans , Interpersonal Relations , Judgment , Male , Movement , Speech , Wechsler Scales
7.
J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry ; 80(10): 1143-5, 2009 Oct.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19762901

ABSTRACT

Progressive supranuclear palsy (PSP) is an atypical parkinsonian syndrome characterised by akinesis, rigidity, falls, supranuclear gaze palsy and cognitive, particularly executive, dysfunction. This study examined the extent to which emotion recognition is affected by PSP. Although deficits in the recognition of emotion have been reported in several diseases which share clinicopathological characteristics with PSP, it has never been studied systematically in PSP. Twenty-four patients with probable or definite PSP and matched healthy controls were studied using tests of facial identity and facial emotion recognition. Patients were not impaired in recognising famous faces, but they showed significant deficits in the recognition of emotions, particularly negative emotions. Moreover, emotion recognition was strongly correlated with the severity of other cognitive deficits in PSP, but not disease duration. Deficits in emotion recognition form an integral part of the cognitive spectrum of the disease. The findings point to the pathological involvement of key regions necessary for the processing of emotions and to a subtype of PSP with cognitive and emotion recognition impairments. The acknowledgement of deficits in emotion recognition is important for management of both patients and their carers.


Subject(s)
Emotions , Facial Expression , Perception , Recognition, Psychology , Supranuclear Palsy, Progressive/psychology , Aged , Case-Control Studies , Cohort Studies , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Neuropsychological Tests , Supranuclear Palsy, Progressive/pathology , Supranuclear Palsy, Progressive/physiopathology
8.
J Cogn Neurosci ; 19(7): 1206-17, 2007 Jul.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17583995

ABSTRACT

Patients with Huntington's disease (HD) can show disproportionate impairments in recognizing facial signals of disgust, but the neural basis of this deficit remains unclear. Functional imaging studies have implicated the anterior insula in the ability to recognize disgust, but have identified other structures as well, including the basal ganglia. In view of variable insula and basal ganglia volume changes in HD, we used voxel-based morphometry to map regional variations in gray matter (GM) volume in participants carrying the mutation for HD, and correlated this with their performance on a test of facial emotion recognition for six basic emotions (disgust, fear, anger, happiness, sadness, surprise). The volume of the anteroventral insula was strongly correlated with performance on the disgust recognition task. The amygdala volume (bilaterally) correlated with the ability to recognize happy facial expressions. There was marked specificity of the regional correlations for the emotion involved. Recognition of other emotion expressions, or more general cognitive or motor performance as measured by a standardized rating scale, did not correlate with regional brain volume in this group. Control participants showed no effect for any measure. The strong linear correlations for disgust and happiness recognition imply direct involvement of the anterior insula in disgust appreciation, and a similar role for the amygdala in recognizing happy facial expressions. The absence of a significant correlation with the basal ganglia suggests a less critical role for these structures in disgust recognition than has previously been suggested. The findings also highlight the role of neurodegenerative diseases combined with statistical imaging techniques in elucidating the brain basis of behavior and cognition.


Subject(s)
Amygdala/pathology , Cerebral Cortex/pathology , Emotions/physiology , Huntington Disease/pathology , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Recognition, Psychology/physiology , Adult , Amygdala/physiopathology , Brain Mapping , Cerebral Cortex/physiopathology , Facial Expression , Female , Humans , Huntington Disease/genetics , Huntington Disease/physiopathology , Huntington Disease/psychology , Male , Middle Aged , Neuropsychological Tests , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Severity of Illness Index , Statistics as Topic
9.
Psychol Med ; 34(5): 795-802, 2004 Jul.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15500300

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: The processing of facial emotion involves a distributed network of limbic and paralimbic brain structures. Many of these regions are also implicated in the pathophysiology of mood disorders. Behavioural data indicate that depressed subjects show a state-related positive recognition bias for faces displaying negative emotions. There are sparse data to suggest there may be an analogous, state-related negative recognition bias for negative emotions in mania. We used functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to investigate the behavioural and neurocognitive correlates of happy and sad facial affect recognition in patients with mania. METHOD: Functional MRI and an explicit facial affect recognition task were used in a case-control design to measure brain activation and associated behavioural response to variable intensity of sad and happy facial expressions in 10 patients with bipolar I mania and 12 healthy comparison subjects. RESULTS: The patients with mania had attenuated subjective rating of the intensity of sad facial expressions, and associated attenuation of activation in the subgenual anterior cingulate and bilateral amygdala, with increased activation in the posterior cingulate and posterior insula. No behavioural or neurocognitive abnormalities were found in response to presentation of happy facial expressions. CONCLUSIONS: Patients with mania showed a specific, mood-congruent, negative bias in sad facial affect recognition, which was associated with an abnormal profile of brain activation in paralimbic regions implicated in affect recognition and mood disorders. Functional imaging of facial emotion recognition may be a useful probe of cortical and subcortical abnormalities in mood disorders.


Subject(s)
Affect , Bipolar Disorder/epidemiology , Bipolar Disorder/psychology , Facial Expression , Perceptual Disorders/epidemiology , Recognition, Psychology , Social Perception , Adult , Amygdala/physiopathology , Bipolar Disorder/physiopathology , Female , Gyrus Cinguli/physiopathology , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Male , Visual Perception
10.
J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry ; 74(2): 253-4, 2003 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-12531962

ABSTRACT

It has been suggested that similar neural mechanisms may underlie the affective modulation of both recollective and perceptual experience. A case is reported of a patient who has bilateral amygdala damage and marked impairment in the perception of emotion, particularly fear. The patient DR and 10 healthy control subjects (matched for school leaving age, intelligence quotient, and non-emotional memory performance) were shown a series of slides accompanied by an emotionally arousing narrative. One week later DR and the controls were given a surprise memory test for this material. In addition, they completed a verbal memory test using emotionally arousing stimuli. Both DR and the healthy control subjects showed a normative pattern of enhanced memory for emotional material. On the basis of these results and the previously demonstrated impairment of perception of emotion in this patient, it is concluded that different neural mechanisms may underlie affective modulation of recollective and perceptual experience.


Subject(s)
Affect/physiology , Amygdala/physiopathology , Brain Damage, Chronic/physiopathology , Mental Recall/physiology , Perceptual Disorders/physiopathology , Amygdala/surgery , Brain Damage, Chronic/diagnosis , Brain Damage, Chronic/psychology , Epilepsies, Partial/surgery , Female , Follow-Up Studies , Humans , Memory, Short-Term/physiology , Middle Aged , Neuropsychological Tests , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Perceptual Disorders/diagnosis , Perceptual Disorders/psychology , Postoperative Complications/diagnosis , Postoperative Complications/physiopathology , Postoperative Complications/psychology , Retention, Psychology/physiology , Semantics , Speech Perception/physiology , Stereotaxic Techniques , Verbal Learning/physiology
11.
Nat Rev Neurosci ; 2(5): 352-63, 2001 May.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11331919

ABSTRACT

For over 60 years, ideas about emotion in neuroscience and psychology have been dominated by a debate on whether emotion can be encompassed within a single, unifying model. In neuroscience, this approach is epitomized by the limbic system theory and, in psychology, by dimensional models of emotion. Comparative research has gradually eroded the limbic model, and some scientists have proposed that certain individual emotions are represented separately in the brain. Evidence from humans consistent with this approach has recently been obtained by studies indicating that signals of fear and disgust are processed by distinct neural substrates. We review this research and its implications for theories of emotion.


Subject(s)
Amygdala/physiology , Corpus Striatum/physiology , Fear/physiology , Hate , Amygdala/cytology , Animals , Brain Mapping , Cerebral Cortex/cytology , Cerebral Cortex/physiology , Corpus Striatum/cytology , Fear/psychology , Humans , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology
12.
Vision Res ; 41(9): 1179-208, 2001 Apr.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11292507

ABSTRACT

Pictures of facial expressions from the Ekman and Friesen set (Ekman, P., Friesen, W. V., (1976). Pictures of facial affect. Palo Alto, California: Consulting Psychologists Press) were submitted to a principal component analysis (PCA) of their pixel intensities. The output of the PCA was submitted to a series of linear discriminant analyses which revealed three principal findings: (1) a PCA-based system can support facial expression recognition, (2) continuous two-dimensional models of emotion (e.g. Russell, J. A. (1980). A circumplex model of affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39, 1161-1178) are reflected in the statistical structure of the Ekman and Friesen facial expressions, and (3) components for coding facial expression information are largely different to components for facial identity information. The implications for models of face processing are discussed.


Subject(s)
Facial Expression , Image Processing, Computer-Assisted , Adolescent , Adult , Cues , Discriminant Analysis , Emotions/physiology , Female , Form Perception/physiology , Humans , Male , Memory/physiology
13.
Nat Neurosci ; 3(11): 1077-8, 2000 Nov.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11036262

ABSTRACT

Huntington's disease can particularly affect people's recognition of disgust from facial expressions, and functional neuroimaging research has demonstrated that facial expressions of disgust consistently engage different brain areas (insula and putamen) than other facial expressions. However, it is not known whether these particular brain areas process only facial signals of disgust or disgust signals from multiple modalities. Here we describe evidence, from a patient with insula and putamen damage, for a neural system for recognizing social signals of disgust from multiple modalities.


Subject(s)
Caudate Nucleus/physiology , Emotions/physiology , Facial Expression , Globus Pallidus/pathology , Nonverbal Communication/physiology , Putamen/physiology , Adult , Brain Injuries/pathology , Brain Injuries/psychology , Caudate Nucleus/pathology , Humans , Huntington Disease/pathology , Huntington Disease/psychology , Male , Nonverbal Communication/psychology , Putamen/pathology , Recognition, Psychology/physiology
14.
Cognition ; 76(2): 105-46, 2000 Aug 14.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10856740

ABSTRACT

The physical differences between facial expressions (e.g. fear) and a reference norm (e.g. a neutral expression) were altered to produce photographic-quality caricatures. In Experiment 1, participants rated caricatures of fear, happiness and sadness for their intensity of these three emotions; a second group of participants rated how 'face-like' the caricatures appeared. With increasing levels of exaggeration the caricatures were rated as more emotionally intense, but less 'face-like'. Experiment 2 demonstrated a similar relationship between emotional intensity and level of caricature for six different facial expressions. Experiments 3 and 4 compared intensity ratings of facial expression caricatures prepared relative to a selection of reference norms - a neutral expression, an average expression, or a different facial expression (e.g. anger caricatured relative to fear). Each norm produced a linear relationship between caricature and rated intensity of emotion; this finding is inconsistent with two-dimensional models of the perceptual representation of facial expression. An exemplar-based multidimensional model is proposed as an alternative account.


Subject(s)
Caricatures as Topic , Emotions , Facial Expression , Pattern Recognition, Visual , Adult , Discrimination Learning , Female , Humans , Male , Social Perception
15.
J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform ; 26(2): 527-51, 2000 Apr.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10811161

ABSTRACT

Composite facial expressions were prepared by aligning the top half of one expression (e.g., anger) with the bottom half of another (e.g., happiness). Experiment 1 shows that participants are slower to identify the expression in either half of these composite images relative to a "noncomposite" control condition in which the 2 halves are misaligned. This parallels the composite effect for facial identity (A. W. Young, D. Hellawell, & D. C. Hay, 1987), and like its identity counterpart, the effect is disrupted by inverting the stimuli (Experiment 2). Experiment 3 shows that no composite effect is found when the top and bottom sections contain different models' faces posing the same expression; this serves to exclude many nonconfigural interpretations of the composite effect (e.g., that composites are more "attention-grabbing" than noncomposites). Finally, Experiment 4 demonstrates that the composite effects for identity and expression operate independently of one another.


Subject(s)
Discrimination Learning , Facial Expression , Pattern Recognition, Visual , Adult , Attention , Female , Humans , Male , Orientation , Reaction Time
16.
Cogn Neuropsychol ; 17(1): 73-87, 2000 Feb 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20945172

ABSTRACT

We present an investigation of facial expression recognition by three people (BC, LP, and NC) with Mobius syndrome, a congenital disorder producing facial paralysis. The participants were asked to identify the emotion displayed in 10 examples of facial expressions associated with each of 6 basic emotions from the Ekman and Friesen (1976) series. None of the three people with Mobius syndrome was significantly impaired on this task. On a second test of facial expression recognition using computer-morphed facial expressions, NC showed a statistically significant impairment, BC a borderline deficit, and LP was unimpaired. However, even when impairments were found, people with Mobius syndrome still recognised many of the facial expressions shown to them. The recognition of facial expressions by people who have never been able to produce such signals on their own faces demonstrates that the ability to produce facial expressions is not a necessary prerequisite of their recognition.

17.
Neuropsychologia ; 37(10): 1111-7, 1999 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10509833

ABSTRACT

Findings from several case studies have shown that bilateral amygdala damage impairs recognition of emotions in facial expressions, especially fear. However, one study did not find such an impairment, and, in general, comparison across studies has been made difficult because of the different stimuli and tasks employed. In a collaborative study to facilitate such comparisons, we report here the recognition of emotional facial expressions in nine subjects with bilateral amygdala damage, using a sensitive and quantitative assessment. Compared to controls, the subjects as a group were significantly impaired in recognizing fear, although individual performances ranged from severely impaired to essentially normal. Most subjects were impaired on several negative emotions in addition to fear, but no subject was impaired in recognizing happy expressions. An analysis of response consistency showed that impaired recognition of fear could not be attributed simply to mistaking fear for another emotion. While it remains unclear why some subjects with amygdala damage included here are not impaired on our task, the results overall are consistent with the idea that the amygdala plays an important role in triggering knowledge related to threat and danger signaled by facial expressions.


Subject(s)
Amygdala/injuries , Brain Injury, Chronic/psychology , Facial Expression , Memory , Adult , Aged , Case-Control Studies , Fear , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged
18.
Proc Biol Sci ; 265(1408): 1809-17, 1998 Oct 07.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9802236

ABSTRACT

Neuropsychological studies report more impaired responses to facial expressions of fear than disgust in people with amygdala lesions, and vice versa in people with Huntington's disease. Experiments using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) have confirmed the role of the amygdala in the response to fearful faces and have implicated the anterior insula in the response to facial expressions of disgust. We used fMRI to extend these studies to the perception of fear and disgust from both facial and vocal expressions. Consistent with neuropsychological findings, both types of fearful stimuli activated the amygdala. Facial expressions of disgust activated the anterior insula and the caudate-putamen; vocal expressions of disgust did not significantly activate either of these regions. All four types of stimuli activated the superior temporal gyrus. Our findings therefore (i) support the differential localization of the neural substrates of fear and disgust; (ii) confirm the involvement of the amygdala in the emotion of fear, whether evoked by facial or vocal expressions; (iii) confirm the involvement of the anterior insula and the striatum in reactions to facial expressions of disgust; and (iv) suggest a possible general role for the perception of emotional expressions for the superior temporal gyrus.


Subject(s)
Affect/physiology , Brain Mapping , Facial Expression , Fear/physiology , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Adult , Cerebrovascular Circulation , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Male , Oxygen Consumption
19.
Brain ; 121 ( Pt 1): 47-57, 1998 Jan.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9549487

ABSTRACT

Localized amygdalar lesions in humans produce deficits in the recognition of fearful facial expressions. We used functional neuroimaging to test two hypotheses: (i) that the amygdala and some of its functionally connected structures mediate specific neural responses to fearful expressions; (ii) that the early visual processing of emotional faces can be influenced by amygdalar activity. Normal subjects were scanned using PET while they performed a gender discrimination task involving static grey-scale images of faces expressing varying degrees of fear or happiness. In support of the first hypothesis, enhanced activity in the left amygdala, left pulvinar, left anterior insula and bilateral anterior cingulate gyri was observed during the processing of fearful faces. Evidence consistent with the second hypothesis was obtained by a demonstration that amygdalar responses predict expression-specific neural activity in extrastriate cortex.


Subject(s)
Amygdala/physiology , Emotions/physiology , Facial Expression , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Adult , Amygdala/diagnostic imaging , Discrimination, Psychological/physiology , Female , Humans , Male , Regression Analysis , Sex , Tomography, Emission-Computed
20.
Neuropsychologia ; 36(1): 59-70, 1998 Jan.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9533388

ABSTRACT

Face processing and facial emotion recognition were investigated in five post-encephalitic people of average or above-average intelligence. Four of these people (JC, YW, RB and SE) had extensive damage in the region of the amygdala. A fifth post-encephalitic person with predominantly hippocampal damage and relative sparing of the amygdala (RS) participated, allowing us to contrast the effects of temporal lobe damage including and excluding the amygdala region. The findings showed impaired recognition of fear following bilateral temporal lobe damage when this included the amygdala. For JC, this was part of a constellation of deficits on face processing tasks, with impaired recognition of several emotions. SE, YW and RB, however, showed relatively circumscribed deficits. Although they all had some problems in recognizing or naming famous faces, and had poor memory for faces on the Warrington Recognition Memory Test, none showed a significant impairment on the Benton Test of Facial Recognition, indicating relatively good perception of the face's physical structure. In a test of recognition of basic emotions (happiness, surprise, fear, sadness, disgust and anger), SE, YW and RB achieved normal levels of performance in comparison to our control group for all emotions except fear. Their results contrast with those of RS, with relative sparing of the amygdala region and unimpaired recognition of emotion, pointing clearly toward the importance of the amygdala in the recognition of fear.


Subject(s)
Amygdala/pathology , Encephalitis/complications , Facial Expression , Fear , Memory/physiology , Aged , Emotions , Encephalitis/psychology , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Visual Perception
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...