Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 5 de 5
Filter
Add more filters










Database
Language
Publication year range
1.
J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn ; 43(4): 591-610, 2017 04.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28068126

ABSTRACT

Research has demonstrated a link between perspective taking and working memory. Here we used eye tracking to examine the time course with which working memory load (WML) influences perspective-taking ability in a referential communication task and how motivation to take another's perspective modulates these effects. In Experiment 1, where there was no reward or time pressure, listeners only showed evidence of incorporating perspective knowledge during integration of the target object but did not anticipate reference to this common ground object during the pretarget-noun period. WML did not affect this perspective use. In Experiment 2, where a reward for speed and accuracy was applied, listeners used perspective cues to disambiguate the target object from the competitor object from the earliest moments of processing (i.e., during the pretarget-noun period), but only under low load. Under high load, responses were comparable with the control condition, where both objects were in common ground. Furthermore, attempts to initiate perspective-relevant responses under high load led to impaired recall on the concurrent WML task, indicating that perspective-relevant responses were drawing on limited cognitive resources. These results show that when there is ambiguity, perspective cues guide rapid referential interpretation when there is sufficient motivation and sufficient cognitive resources. (PsycINFO Database Record


Subject(s)
Attention/physiology , Cognition/physiology , Comprehension/physiology , Motivation/physiology , Adolescent , Adult , Communication , Eye Movements/physiology , Female , Humans , Inhibition, Psychological , Male , Memory, Short-Term/physiology , Photic Stimulation , Reaction Time/physiology , Stroop Test , Time Factors , Young Adult
2.
Q J Exp Psychol (Hove) ; 70(8): 1646-1660, 2017 Aug.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27364567

ABSTRACT

Previous studies have shown that while people can rapidly and accurately compute their own and other people's visual perspectives, they experience difficulty ignoring the irrelevant perspective when the two perspectives differ. We used the "avatar" perspective-taking task to examine the mechanisms that underlie these egocentric (i.e., interference from their own perspective) and altercentric (i.e., interference from the other person's perspective) tendencies. Participants were eye-tracked as they verified the number of discs in a visual scene according to either their own or an on-screen avatar's perspective. Crucially in some trials the two perspectives were inconsistent (i.e., each saw a different number of discs), while in others they were consistent. To examine the effect of perspective switching, performance was compared for trials that were preceded with the same versus a different perspective cue. We found that altercentric interference can be reduced or eliminated when participants stick with their own perspective across consecutive trials. Our eye-tracking analyses revealed distinct fixation patterns for self and other perspective taking, suggesting that consistency effects in this paradigm are driven by implicit mentalizing of what others can see, and not automatic directional cues from the avatar.


Subject(s)
Attention/physiology , Eye Movements/physiology , Self Concept , Visual Perception/physiology , Adolescent , Analysis of Variance , Female , Humans , Judgment , Male , Online Systems , Photic Stimulation , Reaction Time/physiology , Young Adult
3.
Brain Res ; 1622: 252-69, 2015 Oct 05.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26119912

ABSTRACT

Recent empirical research suggests that understanding a counterfactual event (e.g. 'If Josie had revised, she would have passed her exams') activates mental representations of both the factual and counterfactual versions of events. However, it remains unclear when readers switch between these models during comprehension, and whether representing multiple 'worlds' is cognitively effortful. This paper reports two ERP studies where participants read contexts that set up a factual or counterfactual scenario, followed by a second sentence describing a consequence of this event. Critically, this sentence included a noun that was either consistent or inconsistent with the preceding context, and either included a modal verb to indicate reference to the counterfactual-world or not (thus referring to the factual-world). Experiment 2 used adapted versions of the materials used in Experiment 1 to examine the degree to which representing multiple versions of a counterfactual situation makes heavy demands on cognitive resources by measuring individuals' working memory capacity. Results showed that when reference to the counterfactual-world was maintained by the ongoing discourse, readers correctly interpreted events according to the counterfactual-world (i.e. showed larger N400 for inconsistent than consistent words). In contrast, when cues referred back to the factual-world, readers showed no difference between consistent and inconsistent critical words, suggesting that they simultaneously compared information against both possible worlds. These results support previous dual-representation accounts for counterfactuals, and provide new evidence that linguistic cues can guide the reader in selecting which world model to evaluate incoming information against. Crucially, we reveal evidence that maintaining and updating a hypothetical model over time relies upon the availability of cognitive resources.


Subject(s)
Brain/physiology , Comprehension/physiology , Language , Memory, Short-Term/physiology , Reading , Thinking/physiology , Electroencephalography , Evoked Potentials , Female , Humans , Male , Neuropsychological Tests , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Young Adult
4.
Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci ; 10(6): 848-55, 2015 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25326041

ABSTRACT

Interpreting others' actions relies on an understanding of their current mental state. Emerging research has begun to identify a number of factors that give rise to individual differences in this ability. We report an event-related brain potential study where participants (N = 28) read contexts that described a character having a true belief (TB) or false belief (FB) about an object's location. A second sentence described where that character would look for the object. Critically, this sentence included a sentence-final noun that was either consistent or inconsistent with the character's belief. Participants also completed the Empathy Quotient questionnaire. Analysis of the N400 revealed that when the character held a TB about the object's location, the N400 waveform was more negative-going for belief inconsistent vs belief consistent critical words. However, when the character held an FB about the object's location the opposite pattern was found. Intriguingly, correlations between the N400 inconsistency effect and individuals' empathy scores showed a significant correlation for FB but not TB. This suggests that people who are high in empathy can successfully interpret events according to the character's FB, while low empathizers bias their interpretation of events to their own egocentric view.


Subject(s)
Brain/physiology , Concept Formation/physiology , Empathy/physiology , Evoked Potentials/physiology , Thinking/physiology , Adolescent , Comprehension/physiology , Electroencephalography , Female , Humans , Male , Young Adult
5.
Q J Exp Psychol (Hove) ; 65(7): 1397-413, 2012.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22540847

ABSTRACT

Two experiments examined how interruptions impact reading and how interruption lags and the reader's spatial memory affect the recovery from such interruptions. Participants read paragraphs of text and were interrupted unpredictably by a spoken news story while their eye movements were monitored. Time made available for consolidation prior to responding to the interruption did not aid reading resumption. However, providing readers with a visual cue that indicated the interruption location did aid task resumption substantially in Experiment 2. Taken together, the findings show that the recovery from interruptions during reading draws on spatial memory resources and can be aided by processes that support spatial memory. Practical implications are discussed.


Subject(s)
Attention , Eye Movements/physiology , Memory/physiology , Reading , Space Perception/physiology , Acoustic Stimulation , Adolescent , Adult , Analysis of Variance , Cues , Female , Fixation, Ocular , Humans , Male , Neuropsychological Tests , Time Factors , Young Adult
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...