Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 17 de 17
Filter
1.
Psychol Aging ; 39(1): 102-112, 2024 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38059928

ABSTRACT

Developmental literature suggests that susceptibility to social conformity pressure peaks in adolescence and disappears with maturity into early adulthood. Predictions about these behaviors are less clear for middle-aged and older adults. On the one hand, while age-related increases in prioritization of socioemotional goals might predict greater susceptibility to social conformity pressures, aging is also associated with enhanced emotion regulation that could support resistance to conformity pressures. In this exploratory research study, we used mobile experience sampling surveys to naturalistically track how 157 healthy adults between the ages of 18 and 80 practice self-control over spontaneous desires in daily life. Many of these desires were experienced in the presence of others enacting that desire. Results showed that middle-aged and older adults were better at controlling their desires than younger adults when desires were experienced in the presence of others enacting that desire. Consistent with the literature on improved emotion regulation with age, these results provide evidence that the ability to resist social conformity pressure is enhanced across the adult life span. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).


Subject(s)
Aging , Emotional Regulation , Humans , Middle Aged , Aged , Adult , Aged, 80 and over , Aging/physiology , Social Conformity , Emotions/physiology , Longevity , Emotional Regulation/physiology
2.
Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci ; 18(1)2023 02 23.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36264228

ABSTRACT

Jury decisions are among the most consequential social decisions in which bias plays a notable role. While courts take measures to reduce the influence of non-evidentiary factors, jurors may still incorporate biases into their decisions. One common bias, crime-type bias, is the extent to which the perceived strength of a prosecutor's case depends on the severity of the crime. Moral judgment, affect and social cognition have been proposed as core processes underlying this and other biases. Behavioral evidence alone has been insufficient to distinguish these explanations. To identify the mechanism underlying crime-type bias, we collected functional magnetic resonance imaging patterns of brain activation from mock jurors reading criminal scenarios. Brain patterns from crime-type bias were most similar to those associated with social cognition (mentalizing and racial bias) but not affect or moral judgment. Our results support a central role for social cognition in juror decisions and suggest that crime-type bias and cultural bias may arise from similar mechanisms.


Subject(s)
Decision Making , Judgment , Humans , Morals , Bias , Cognition , Criminal Law
3.
J Neurosci ; 42(40): 7624-7633, 2022 10 05.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36658459

ABSTRACT

Efforts to explain complex human decisions have focused on competing theories emphasizing utility and narrative mechanisms. These are difficult to distinguish using behavior alone. Both narrative and utility theories have been proposed to explain juror decisions, which are among the most consequential complex decisions made in a modern society. Here, we asked jury-eligible male and female subjects to rate the strength of a series of criminal cases while recording the resulting patterns of brain activation. We compared patterns of brain activation associated with evidence accumulation to patterns of brain activation derived from a large neuroimaging database to look for signatures of the cognitive processes associated with different models of juror decision-making. Evidence accumulation correlated with multiple narrative processes, including reading and recall. Of the cognitive processes traditionally viewed as components of utility, activation patterns associated with uncertainty, but not value, were more active with stronger evidence. Independent of utility and narrative, activations linked to reasoning and relational logic also correlated with increasing evidence. Hierarchical modeling of cognitive processes associated with evidence accumulation supported a more prominent role for narrative in weighing evidence in complex decisions. However, utility processes were also associated with evidence accumulation. These complementary findings support an emerging view that integrates utility and narrative processes in complex decisions.SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT The last decade has seen a sharply increased interest in narrative as a central cognitive process in human decision-making and as an important factor in the evolution of human societies. However, the roles of narrative versus utility models of decision-making remain hotly debated. While available models frequently produce similar behavioral predictions, they rely on different cognitive processes and so their roles can be separated using the right neural tests. Here, we use brain imaging during mock juror decisions to show that cognitive processes associated with narrative, and to a lesser extent utility, were engaged while subjects evaluated evidence. These results are consistent with interactions between narrative and utility processes during complex decision-making.


Subject(s)
Brain , Decision Making , Humans , Male , Female , Decision Making/physiology , Uncertainty , Brain/diagnostic imaging , Brain/physiology , Problem Solving , Mental Recall
4.
Emotion ; 21(3): 453-464, 2021 Apr.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32191090

ABSTRACT

Older adults report experiencing improved emotional health, such as more intense positive affect and less intense negative affect. However, there are mixed findings on whether older adults are better at regulating emotion-a hallmark feature of emotional health-and most research is based on laboratory studies that may not capture how people regulate their emotions in everyday life. We used experience sampling to examine how multiple measures of emotional health, including mean affect, dynamic fluctuations between affective states and the ability to resist desires-a common form of emotion regulation-differ in daily life across adulthood. Participants (N = 122, ages 20-80) reported how they were feeling and responding to desire temptations for 10 days. Older adults experienced more intense positive affect, less intense negative affect, and were more emotionally stable, even after controlling for individual differences in global life satisfaction. Older adults were more successful at regulating desires, even though they experienced more intense desires than younger adults. In addition, adults in general experiencing more intense affect were less successful at resisting desires. These results demonstrate how emotional experience is related to more successful desire regulation in everyday life and provide unique evidence that emotional health and regulation improve with age. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).


Subject(s)
Emotional Regulation/physiology , Emotions/physiology , Adult , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Young Adult
5.
Psychopharmacology (Berl) ; 238(3): 711-723, 2021 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33215269

ABSTRACT

RATIONALE: Although numerous studies have suggested that pharmacological alteration of the dopamine (DA) system modulates reward discounting, these studies have produced inconsistent findings. OBJECTIVES: Here, we conducted a systematic review and pre-registered meta-analysis to evaluate DA drug-mediated effects on reward discounting of time, probability, and effort costs in studies of healthy rats. This produced a total of 1343 articles to screen for inclusion/exclusion. From the literature, we identified 117 effects from approximately 1549 individual rats. METHODS: Using random effects with maximum-likelihood estimation, we meta-analyzed placebo-controlled drug effects for (1) DA D1-like receptor agonists and (2) antagonists, (3) D2-like agonists and (4) antagonists, and (5) DA transporter-modulating drugs. RESULTS: Meta-analytic effects showed that DAT-modulating drugs decreased reward discounting. While D1-like and D2-like antagonists both increased discounting, agonist drugs for those receptors had no significant effect on discounting behavior. A number of these effects appear contingent on study design features like cost type, rat strain, and microinfusion location. CONCLUSIONS: These findings suggest a nuanced relationship between DA and discounting behavior and urge caution when drawing generalizations about the effects of pharmacologically manipulating dopamine on reward-based decision-making.


Subject(s)
Decision Making/drug effects , Delay Discounting/drug effects , Dopamine Agonists/pharmacology , Dopamine Antagonists/pharmacology , Receptors, Dopamine D1/metabolism , Receptors, Dopamine D2/metabolism , Reward , Animals , Biological Transport , Brain/drug effects , Brain/metabolism , Dose-Response Relationship, Drug , Male , Rats
6.
Nature ; 582(7810): 84-88, 2020 06.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32483374

ABSTRACT

Data analysis workflows in many scientific domains have become increasingly complex and flexible. Here we assess the effect of this flexibility on the results of functional magnetic resonance imaging by asking 70 independent teams to analyse the same dataset, testing the same 9 ex-ante hypotheses1. The flexibility of analytical approaches is exemplified by the fact that no two teams chose identical workflows to analyse the data. This flexibility resulted in sizeable variation in the results of hypothesis tests, even for teams whose statistical maps were highly correlated at intermediate stages of the analysis pipeline. Variation in reported results was related to several aspects of analysis methodology. Notably, a meta-analytical approach that aggregated information across teams yielded a significant consensus in activated regions. Furthermore, prediction markets of researchers in the field revealed an overestimation of the likelihood of significant findings, even by researchers with direct knowledge of the dataset2-5. Our findings show that analytical flexibility can have substantial effects on scientific conclusions, and identify factors that may be related to variability in the analysis of functional magnetic resonance imaging. The results emphasize the importance of validating and sharing complex analysis workflows, and demonstrate the need for performing and reporting multiple analyses of the same data. Potential approaches that could be used to mitigate issues related to analytical variability are discussed.


Subject(s)
Data Analysis , Data Science/methods , Data Science/standards , Datasets as Topic , Functional Neuroimaging , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Research Personnel/organization & administration , Brain/diagnostic imaging , Brain/physiology , Datasets as Topic/statistics & numerical data , Female , Humans , Logistic Models , Male , Meta-Analysis as Topic , Models, Neurological , Reproducibility of Results , Research Personnel/standards , Software
7.
Sci Rep ; 9(1): 20229, 2019 12 27.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31882947

ABSTRACT

The process by which the value of delayed rewards is discounted varies from person to person. It has been suggested that these individual differences in subjective valuation of delayed rewards are supported by mesolimbic dopamine D2-like receptors (D2Rs) in the ventral striatum. However, no study to date has documented an association between direct measures of dopamine receptors and neural representations of subjective value in humans. Here, we examined whether individual differences in D2R availability were related to neural subjective value signals during decision making. Human participants completed a monetary delay discounting task during an fMRI scan and on a separate visit completed a PET scan with the high affinity D2R tracer [18 F]fallypride. Region-of-interest analyses revealed that D2R availability in the ventral striatum was positively correlated with subjective value-related activity in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex and midbrain but not with choice behavior. Whole-brain analyses revealed a positive correlation between ventral striatum D2R availability and subjective value-related activity in the left inferior frontal gyrus and superior insula. These findings identify a link between a direct measure of mesolimbic dopamine function and subjective value representation in humans and suggest a mechanism by which individuals vary in neural representation of discounted subjective value.


Subject(s)
Cerebral Cortex/metabolism , Delay Discounting , Prefrontal Cortex/metabolism , Receptors, Dopamine D2/metabolism , Reward , Ventral Striatum/metabolism , Benzamides/chemistry , Brain Mapping , Cerebral Cortex/diagnostic imaging , Choice Behavior , Decision Making , Female , Fluorine Radioisotopes/chemistry , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging/methods , Male , Positron-Emission Tomography/methods , Prefrontal Cortex/diagnostic imaging , Ventral Striatum/diagnostic imaging , Young Adult
8.
Psychol Aging ; 34(7): 921-932, 2019 Nov.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31589058

ABSTRACT

The evidence that dopamine function mediates the association between aging and cognition is one of the most cited findings in the cognitive neuroscience of aging. However, few and relatively small studies have directly examined these associations. Here we examined correlations among adult age, dopamine D2-like receptor (D2R) availability, and cognition in two cross-sectional studies of healthy human adults. Participants completed a short cognitive test battery and, on a separate day, a PET scan with either the high-affinity D2R tracer [18F]Fallypride (Study 1) or [11C]FLB457 (Study 2). Digit span, a measure of short-term memory maintenance and working memory, was the only cognitive test for which dopamine D2R availability partially mediated the age effect on cognition. In Study 1, age was negatively correlated with digit span. Striatal D2R availability was positively correlated with digit span controlling for age. The age effect on digit span was smaller when controlling for striatal D2R availability. Although other cognitive measures used here have individually been associated with age and D2R availability in prior studies, we found no consistent evidence for significant associations between low D2R availability and low cognitive performance on these measures. These results at best only partially supported the correlative triad of age, dopamine D2R availability, and cognition. While a wealth of other research in human and nonhuman animals demonstrates that dopamine makes critical contributions to cognition, the present studies suggest caution in interpreting PET findings as evidence that dopamine D2R loss is a primary cause of broad age-related declines in fluid cognition. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2019 APA, all rights reserved).


Subject(s)
Aging/physiology , Cognition/physiology , Receptors, Dopamine D2/metabolism , Adult , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Animals , Cross-Sectional Studies , Female , Healthy Volunteers , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Reproducibility of Results , Young Adult
9.
Hum Brain Mapp ; 40(10): 3125-3138, 2019 07.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30932295

ABSTRACT

Theories of adult brain development, based on neuropsychological test results and structural neuroimaging, suggest differential rates of age-related change in function across cortical and subcortical sub-regions. However, it remains unclear if these trends also extend to the aging dopamine system. Here we examined cross-sectional adult age differences in estimates of D2-like receptor binding potential across several cortical and subcortical brain regions using PET imaging and the radiotracer [18 F]Fallypride in two samples of healthy human adults (combined N = 132). After accounting for regional differences in overall radioligand binding, estimated percent difference in receptor binding potential by decade (linear effects) were highest in most temporal and frontal cortical regions (~6-16% per decade), moderate in parahippocampal gyrus, pregenual frontal cortex, fusiform gyrus, caudate, putamen, thalamus, and amygdala (~3-5%), and weakest in subcallosal frontal cortex, ventral striatum, pallidum, and hippocampus (~0-2%). Some regions showed linear effects of age while many showed curvilinear effects such that binding potential declined from young adulthood to middle age and then was relatively stable until old age. Overall, these data indicate that the rate and pattern of decline in D2 receptor availability is regionally heterogeneous. However, the differences across regions were challenging to organize within existing theories of brain development and did not show the same pattern of regional change that has been observed in gray matter volume, white matter integrity, or cognitive performance. This variation suggests that existing theories of adult brain development may need to be modified to better account for the spatial dynamics of dopaminergic system aging.


Subject(s)
Aging/metabolism , Brain/metabolism , Receptors, Dopamine D2/metabolism , Receptors, Dopamine D3/metabolism , Adult , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Cross-Sectional Studies , Female , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Male , Middle Aged , Models, Statistical , Positron-Emission Tomography , Young Adult
10.
J Neurosci ; 39(2): 321-332, 2019 01 09.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30446530

ABSTRACT

Some people are more willing to make immediate, risky, or costly reward-focused choices than others, which has been hypothesized to be associated with individual differences in dopamine (DA) function. In two studies using PET imaging, one empirical (Study 1: N = 144 males and females across 3 samples) and one meta-analytic (Study 2: N = 307 across 12 samples), we sought to characterize associations between individual differences in DA and time, probability, and physical effort discounting in human adults. Study 1 demonstrated that individual differences in DA D2-like receptors were not associated with time or probability discounting of monetary rewards in healthy humans, and associations with physical effort discounting were inconsistent across adults of different ages. Meta-analytic results for temporal discounting corroborated our empirical finding for minimal effect of DA measures on discounting in healthy individuals but suggested that associations between individual differences in DA and reward discounting depend on clinical features. Addictions were characterized by negative correlations between DA and discounting, but other clinical conditions, such as Parkinson's disease, obesity, and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder, were characterized by positive correlations between DA and discounting. Together, the results suggest that trait differences in discounting in healthy adults do not appear to be strongly associated with individual differences in D2-like receptors. The difference in meta-analytic correlation effects between healthy controls and individuals with psychopathology suggests that individual difference findings related to DA and reward discounting in clinical samples may not be reliably generalized to healthy controls, and vice versa.SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT Decisions to forgo large rewards for smaller ones due to increasing time delays, uncertainty, or physical effort have been linked to differences in dopamine (DA) function, which is disrupted in some forms of psychopathology. It remains unclear whether alterations in DA function associated with psychopathology also extend to explaining associations between DA function and decision making in healthy individuals. We show that individual differences in DA D2 receptor availability are not consistently related to monetary discounting of time, probability, or physical effort in healthy individuals across a broad age range. By contrast, we suggest that psychopathology accounts for observed inconsistencies in the relationship between measures of DA function and reward discounting behavior.


Subject(s)
Brain/diagnostic imaging , Brain/physiology , Delay Discounting , Dopamine/metabolism , Mental Disorders/psychology , Reward , Adolescent , Adult , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Aging/psychology , Behavior, Addictive/diagnostic imaging , Behavior, Addictive/psychology , Brain Mapping , Female , Humans , Individuality , Male , Mental Disorders/diagnostic imaging , Middle Aged , Positron-Emission Tomography , Receptors, Dopamine D2/genetics , Receptors, Dopamine D2/metabolism , Young Adult
11.
Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci ; 18(4): 739-747, 2018 08.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29725947

ABSTRACT

Reward valuation, which underlies all value-based decision-making, has been associated with dopamine function in many studies of nonhuman animals, but there is relatively less direct evidence for an association in humans. Here, we measured dopamine D2 receptor (DRD2) availability in vivo in humans to examine relations between individual differences in dopamine receptor availability and neural activity associated with a measure of reward valuation, expected value (i.e., the product of reward magnitude and the probability of obtaining the reward). Fourteen healthy adult subjects underwent PET with [18F]fallypride, a radiotracer with strong affinity for DRD2, and fMRI (on a separate day) while performing a reward valuation task. [18F]fallypride binding potential, reflecting DRD2 availability, in the midbrain correlated positively with neural activity associated with expected value, specifically in the left ventral striatum/caudate. The present results provide in vivo evidence from humans showing midbrain dopamine characteristics are associated with reward valuation.


Subject(s)
Brain/metabolism , Individuality , Receptors, Dopamine D2/metabolism , Reward , Adult , Anticipation, Psychological/physiology , Benzamides , Brain/diagnostic imaging , Brain Mapping , Cerebrovascular Circulation , Female , Fluorine Radioisotopes , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Male , Oxygen/blood , Positron-Emission Tomography , Radiopharmaceuticals
12.
Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci ; 13(5): 449-459, 2018 05 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29618082

ABSTRACT

Every day, humans make countless decisions that require the integration of information about potential benefits (i.e. rewards) with other decision features (i.e. effort required, probability of an outcome or time delays). Here, we examine the overlap and dissociation of behavioral preferences and neural representations of subjective value in the context of three different decision features (physical effort, probability and time delays) in a healthy adult life span sample. While undergoing functional neuroimaging, participants (N = 75) made incentive compatible choices between a smaller monetary reward with lower physical effort, higher probability, or a shorter time delay versus a larger monetary reward with higher physical effort, lower probability, or a longer time delay. Behavioral preferences were estimated from observed choices, and subjective values were computed using individual hyperbolic discount functions. We found that discount rates were uncorrelated across tasks. Despite this apparent behavioral dissociation between preferences, we found overlapping subjective value-related activity in the medial prefrontal cortex across all three tasks. We found no consistent evidence for age differences in either preferences or the neural representations of subjective value across adulthood. These results suggest that while the tolerance of decision features is behaviorally dissociable, subjective value signals share a common representation across adulthood.


Subject(s)
Aging/psychology , Decision Making/physiology , Delay Discounting/physiology , Adult , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Brain/diagnostic imaging , Brain/physiology , Choice Behavior/physiology , Cognition , Female , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Male , Middle Aged , Motivation , Neuropsychological Tests , Physical Exertion , Probability , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Reward , Young Adult
13.
Physiol Behav ; 192: 188-193, 2018 08 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29233619

ABSTRACT

The fat mass and obesity associated gene (FTO) was the first gene identified by genome-wide association studies to correlate with higher body mass index (BMI) and increased odds of obesity. FTO remains the locus with the largest and most replicated effect on body weight, but the mechanism whereby FTO affects body weight and the development of obesity is not fully understood. Here we tested whether FTO is associated with differences in food cravings and a key aspect of dopamine function that has been hypothesized to influence food reward mechanisms. Moreover, as food cravings and dopamine function are known to decline with age, we explored effects of age on relations between FTO and food cravings and dopamine function. Seven-eight healthy subjects between 22 and 83years old completed the Food Cravings Questionnaire and underwent genotyping for FTO rs9939609, the first FTO single nucleotide polymorphism associated with obesity. Compared to TT homozygotes, individuals carrying the obesity-susceptible A allele had higher total food cravings, which correlated with higher BMI. Additionally, food cravings declined with age, but this age effect differed across variants of FTO rs9939609: while TT homozygotes showed the typical age-related decline in food cravings, there was no such decline among A carriers. All subjects were scanned with [18F]fallypride PET to assess a recent proposal that at the neurochemical level FTO alters dopamine D2-like receptor (DRD2) function to influence food reward related mechanisms. However, we observed no evidence of FTO effects on DRD2 availability.


Subject(s)
Aging/genetics , Alpha-Ketoglutarate-Dependent Dioxygenase FTO/genetics , Craving , Feeding Behavior , Food , Adult , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Aging/psychology , Benzamides , Body Mass Index , Brain/diagnostic imaging , Brain/metabolism , Craving/physiology , Feeding Behavior/physiology , Feeding Behavior/psychology , Female , Genetic Association Studies , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Polymorphism, Single Nucleotide , Positron-Emission Tomography , Pyrrolidines , Radiopharmaceuticals , Receptors, Dopamine D2/metabolism , Young Adult
14.
eNeuro ; 4(5)2017.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28929131

ABSTRACT

Spontaneous eye blink rate (EBR) has been proposed as a noninvasive, inexpensive marker of dopamine functioning. Support for a relation between EBR and dopamine function comes from observations that EBR is altered in populations with dopamine dysfunction and EBR changes under a dopaminergic manipulation. However, the evidence across the literature is inconsistent and incomplete. A direct correlation between EBR and dopamine function has so far been observed only in nonhuman animals. Given significant interest in using EBR as a proxy for dopamine function, this study aimed to verify a direct association in healthy, human adults. Here we measured EBR in healthy human subjects whose dopamine D2 receptor (DRD2) availability was assessed with positron emission tomography (PET)-[18F]fallypride to examine the predictive power of EBR for DRD2 availability. Effects of the dopamine agonist bromocriptine on EBR also were examined to determine the responsiveness of EBR to dopaminergic stimulation and, in light of the hypothesized inverted-U profile of dopamine effects, the role of DRD2 availability in EBR responsivity to bromocriptine. Results from 20 subjects (age 33.6 ± 7.6 years, 9F) showed no relation between EBR and DRD2 availability. EBR also was not responsive to dopaminergic stimulation by bromocriptine, and individual differences in DRD2 availability did not modulate EBR responsivity to bromocriptine. Given that EBR is hypothesized to be particularly sensitive to DRD2 function, these findings suggest caution in using EBR as a proxy for dopamine function in healthy humans.


Subject(s)
Blinking/physiology , Brain/drug effects , Bromocriptine/pharmacology , Dopamine Agonists/pharmacology , Receptors, Dopamine D2/metabolism , Adult , Benzamides/pharmacology , Blinking/drug effects , Brain/diagnostic imaging , Dopamine D2 Receptor Antagonists/pharmacology , Double-Blind Method , Female , Healthy Volunteers , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Male , Middle Aged , Positron-Emission Tomography , Pyrrolidines/pharmacology , Young Adult
15.
Mem Cognit ; 45(5): 716-730, 2017 07.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28224452

ABSTRACT

Although a group of people working together recalls more items than any one individual, they recall fewer unique items than the same number of people working apart whose responses are combined. This is known as collaborative inhibition, and it is a robust effect that occurs for both younger and older adults. However, almost all previous studies documenting collaborative inhibition have used stimuli that were neutral in emotional valence, low in arousal, and studied by all group members. In the current experiments, we tested the impact of picture-stimuli valence, picture-stimuli arousal, and information distribution in modulating the magnitude of collaborative inhibition. We included both younger and older adults because there are age differences in how people remember emotional pictures that could modulate any effects of emotion on collaborative inhibition. Results revealed that when information was shared (i.e., studied by all group members), there were robust collaborative inhibition effects for both neutral and emotional stimuli for both younger and older adults. However, when information was unshared (i.e., studied by only a single group member), these effects were attenuated. Together, these results provide mixed support for the retrieval strategy disruption account of collaborative inhibition. Supporting the retrieval strategy disruption account, unshared study information was less susceptible to collaborative inhibition than shared study information. Contradicting the retrieval strategy disruption account, emotional valence and arousal did not modulate the magnitude of collaborative inhibition despite the fact that participants clustered the emotional, but not neutral, information together in memory.


Subject(s)
Aging/physiology , Cooperative Behavior , Emotions/physiology , Mental Recall/physiology , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Adolescent , Adult , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Young Adult
16.
Neuroimage ; 148: 123-129, 2017 03 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28089678

ABSTRACT

Physical activity has been shown to ameliorate dopaminergic degeneration in non-human animal models. However, the effects of regular physical activity on normal age-related changes in dopamine function in humans are unknown. Here we present cross-sectional data from forty-four healthy human subjects between 23 and 80 years old, showing that typical age-related dopamine D2 receptor loss, assessed with PET [18F]fallypride, was significantly reduced in physically active adults compared to less active adults.


Subject(s)
Aging/metabolism , Exercise/physiology , Receptors, Dopamine D2/physiology , Adult , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Benzamides , Brain/diagnostic imaging , Brain/growth & development , Brain/physiology , Cross-Sectional Studies , Female , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Male , Middle Aged , Positron-Emission Tomography , Radiopharmaceuticals , Receptors, Dopamine D2/metabolism , Receptors, Dopamine D3/metabolism , Young Adult
17.
Neuroimage ; 138: 176-183, 2016 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27208860

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVE: The dopamine D2/3 receptor subtypes (DRD2/3) are the most widely studied neurotransmitter biomarker in research on obesity, but results to date have been inconsistent, have typically involved small samples, and have rarely accounted for subjects' ages despite the large impact of age on DRD2/3 levels. We aimed to clarify the relation between DRD2/3 availability and BMI by examining this association in a large sample of subjects with BMI spanning the continuum from underweight to extremely obese. SUBJECTS: 130 healthy subjects between 18 and 81years old underwent PET with [18F]fallypride, a high affinity DRD2/3 ligand. RESULTS: As expected, DRD2/3 availability declined with age. Critically, age significantly interacted with DRD2/3 availability in predicting BMI in the midbrain and striatal regions (caudate, putamen, and ventral striatum). Among subjects under 30years old, BMI was not associated with DRD2/3 availability. By contrast, among subjects over 30years old, BMI was positively associated with DRD2/3 availability in the midbrain, putamen, and ventral striatum. CONCLUSION: The present results are incompatible with the prominent dopaminergic hypofunction hypothesis that proposes that a reduction in DRD2/3 availability is associated with increased BMI, and highlights the importance of age in assessing correlates of DRD2/3 function.


Subject(s)
Aging/metabolism , Benzamides/pharmacokinetics , Body Mass Index , Brain/metabolism , Pyrrolidines/pharmacokinetics , Receptors, Dopamine D2/metabolism , Receptors, Dopamine D3/metabolism , Adolescent , Adult , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Biological Availability , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Molecular Imaging/methods , Positron-Emission Tomography/methods , Radiopharmaceuticals/pharmacokinetics , Reproducibility of Results , Sensitivity and Specificity , Statistics as Topic , Tissue Distribution , Young Adult
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...