Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 10 de 10
Filter
Add more filters










Publication year range
1.
J Int Neuropsychol Soc ; 22(3): 293-302, 2016 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26786245

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVES: Decrements in cognitive function may already be evident in young children with type 1 diabetes (T1D). Here we report prospectively acquired cognitive results over 18 months in a large cohort of young children with and without T1D. METHODS: A total of 144 children with T1D (mean HbA1c: 7.9%) and 70 age-matched healthy controls (mean age both groups 8.5 years; median diabetes duration 3.9 years; mean age of onset 4.1 years) underwent neuropsychological testing at baseline and after 18-months of follow-up. We hypothesized that group differences observed at baseline would be more pronounced after 18 months, particularly in those T1D patients with greatest exposure to glycemic extremes. RESULTS: Cognitive domain scores did not differ between groups at the 18 month testing session and did not change differently between groups over the follow-up period. However, within the T1D group, a history of diabetic ketoacidosis (DKA) was correlated with lower Verbal IQ and greater hyperglycemia exposure (HbA1c area under the curve) was inversely correlated to executive functions test performance. In addition, those with a history of both types of exposure performed most poorly on measures of executive function. CONCLUSIONS: The subtle cognitive differences between T1D children and nondiabetic controls observed at baseline were not observed 18 months later. Within the T1D group, as at baseline, relationships between cognition (Verbal IQ and executive functions) and glycemic variables (chronic hyperglycemia and DKA history) were evident. Continued longitudinal study of this T1D cohort and their carefully matched healthy comparison group is planned.


Subject(s)
Cognition Disorders/diagnosis , Cognition Disorders/etiology , Developmental Disabilities/etiology , Diabetes Mellitus, Type 1/complications , Neuropsychological Tests , Blood Glucose , Case-Control Studies , Child , Developmental Disabilities/diagnosis , Diabetic Ketoacidosis/blood , Executive Function/physiology , Female , Humans , Hyperglycemia/physiopathology , Longitudinal Studies , Male , Statistics, Nonparametric , Verbal Behavior/physiology
2.
J Int Neuropsychol Soc ; 20(2): 238-47, 2014 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24512675

ABSTRACT

The aim of this study was to assess cognitive functioning in children with type 1 diabetes (T1D) and examine whether glycemic history influences cognitive function. Neuropsychological evaluation of 216 children (healthy controls, n = 72; T1D, n = 144) ages 4-10 years across five DirecNet sites. Cognitive domains included IQ, Executive Functions, Learning and Memory, and Processing Speed. Behavioral, mood, parental IQ data, and T1D glycemic history since diagnosis were collected. The cohorts did not differ in age, gender or parent IQ. Median T1D duration was 2.5 years and average onset age was 4 years. After covarying age, gender, and parental IQ, the IQ and the Executive Functions domain scores trended lower (both p = .02, not statistically significant adjusting for multiple comparisons) with T1D relative to controls. Children with T1D were rated by parents as having more depressive and somatic symptoms (p < .001). Learning and memory (p = .46) and processing speed (p = .25) were similar. Trends in the data supported that the degree of hyperglycemia was associated with Executive Functions, and to a lesser extent, Child IQ and Learning and Memory. Differences in cognition are subtle in young children with T1D within 2 years of onset. Longitudinal evaluations will help determine whether these findings change or become more pronounced with time.


Subject(s)
Cognition Disorders/etiology , Diabetes Mellitus, Type 1/complications , Affect , Child , Child, Preschool , Executive Function/physiology , Female , Glycated Hemoglobin/metabolism , Glycemic Index , Humans , Male , Neuropsychological Tests
3.
J Int Neuropsychol Soc ; 15(2): 169-81, 2009 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19232155

ABSTRACT

Recent findings suggest that neural representations of semantic knowledge contain information about category, modality, and attributes. Although an object's category is defined according to shared attributes that uniquely distinguish it from other category members, a clear dissociation between visual attribute and category representation has not yet been reported. We investigated the contribution of category (living and nonliving) and visual attribute (global form and local details) to semantic representation in the fusiform gyrus. During functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), 40 adults named pictures of animals, tools, and vehicles. In a preliminary study, identification of objects in these categories was differentially dependent on global versus local visual feature processing. fMRI findings indicate that activation in the lateral and medial regions of the fusiform gyrus distinguished stimuli according to category, that is, living versus nonliving, respectively. In contrast, visual attributes of global form (animals) were associated with higher activity in the right fusiform gyrus, while local details (tools) were associated with higher activity in the left fusiform gyrus. When both global and local attributes were relevant to processing (vehicles), cortex in both left and right medial fusiform gyri was more active than for other categories. Taken together, results support distinctions in the role of visual attributes and category in semantic representation.


Subject(s)
Brain Mapping , Brain/blood supply , Brain/physiology , Knowledge , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Semantics , Adult , Analysis of Variance , Female , Functional Laterality , Humans , Image Processing, Computer-Assisted/methods , Magnetic Resonance Imaging/methods , Male , Oxygen/blood , Reaction Time , Young Adult
4.
Neurobiol Aging ; 29(3): 436-51, 2008 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17147975

ABSTRACT

Healthy older adults frequently report word-finding difficulties, yet the underlying cause of these problems is not well understood. This study examined whether age-related changes in word retrieval are related to changes in areas of the frontal lobes thought to subserve word retrieval or changes in areas of the inferior temporal lobes thought to be involved in semantic knowledge. Twenty younger and 20 older healthy adults named aloud photographs during event-related fMRI. Results showed that in the face of equivalent naming accuracy, older adults activated a larger frontal network than younger adults during word retrieval, but there were no activity differences between groups in the fusiform gyrus, suggesting that the substrates for word retrieval but not for semantic knowledge change with aging. Additionally, correlations between BOLD response and naming accuracy and response latency were found in several frontal and subcortical regions in older adults. Findings are discussed in the context of possible compensatory mechanisms invoked to maintain performance in healthy aging, and suggest that increased involvement of the right hemisphere is not universally beneficial to performance.


Subject(s)
Aging/physiology , Brain Mapping , Cerebral Cortex/physiology , Mental Recall/physiology , Verbal Learning/physiology , Adult , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Cerebral Cortex/blood supply , Female , Humans , Image Processing, Computer-Assisted/methods , Magnetic Resonance Imaging/methods , Male , Neuropsychological Tests , Oxygen/blood , Photic Stimulation/methods , Reaction Time/physiology
5.
J Int Neuropsychol Soc ; 13(4): 582-94, 2007 Jul.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17521480

ABSTRACT

Twenty-three chronic nonfluent aphasia patients with moderate or severe word-finding impairments and 11 with profound word-finding impairments received two novel picture-naming treatments. The intention treatment initiated picture-naming trials with a complex left-hand movement and was designed to enhance right frontal participation during word retrieval. The attention treatment required patients to view visual stimuli for picture-naming trials in their left hemispace and was designed to enhance right posterior perisylvian participation during word retrieval. Because the intention treatment addressed action mechanisms and nonfluent aphasia reflects difficulty initiating or maintaining action (i.e., language output), it was hypothesized that intention component of the treatment would enhance re-acquisition of picture naming more than the attention component. Patients with moderate and severe word-finding impairment showed gains with both treatments but greater incremental improvement from one treatment phase to the next with the intention than the attention treatment. Thus, the hypothesis that intention component would be a more active constituent than the attention component was confirmed for these patients. Patients with profound word-finding impairment showed some improvement with both treatments but no differential effects for the intention treatment. Almost all patients who showed treatment gains on either treatment also demonstrated generalization from trained to untrained items.


Subject(s)
Aphasia, Broca/rehabilitation , Attention , Intention , Names , Physical Therapy Modalities , Aged , Analysis of Variance , Female , Functional Laterality , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Pattern Recognition, Visual , Photic Stimulation/methods , Reaction Time/physiology , Time Factors
6.
J Int Neuropsychol Soc ; 10(3): 453-65, 2004 May.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15147602

ABSTRACT

Cognitive deficits following ventromedial prefrontal damage (VM-PFD) have been elusive, with most studies reporting primarily emotional and behavioral changes. The present case illustrates the utility of a process approach to assessing cognitive deficits following VM-PFD. At age 26, C.D. acquired bilateral VM-PFD, more so in the left frontal region, following a penetrating head injury. Despite exemplary premorbid academic and military performances, his subsequent history suggests dramatic occupational and social changes, reminiscent of Phineas Gage. In fact, lesion analysis revealed similar structural damage to that estimated of Gage. C.D.'s scores on the vast majority of neuropsychological measures were average to superior (e.g., Verbal IQ = 119). However, on several new process measures, particularly those that quantify error rates on multilevel executive function and memory tasks, C.D. exhibited marked impairments. From his pattern of deficits, C.D. appeared to sacrifice accuracy for speed and to adopt liberal response strategies, implicating problems with cognitive inflexibility, impulsivity, and disinhibition. The current findings suggest that VM-PFD may be associated with a wider spectrum of cognitive deficits than previously characterized.


Subject(s)
Cognition Disorders/complications , Cognition/physiology , Head Injuries, Penetrating/complications , Mental Disorders/complications , Prefrontal Cortex/physiopathology , Adult , Decision Making/physiology , Humans , Imaging, Three-Dimensional/methods , Magnetic Resonance Imaging/methods , Male , Mental Recall/physiology , Neuropsychological Tests/statistics & numerical data , Prefrontal Cortex/injuries , Verbal Learning/physiology
7.
J Cogn Neurosci ; 16(2): 167-77, 2004 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15068589

ABSTRACT

Responses of rostral frontal and retrosplenial cortices to the emotional significance of words were measured using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). Twenty-six strongly right-handed participants engaged in a language task that alternated between silent word generation to categories with positive, negative, or neutral emotional connotation and a baseline task of silent repetition of emotionally neutral words. Activation uniquely associated with word generation to categories with positive or negative versus neutral emotional connotation occurred bilaterally in rostral frontal and retrosplenial cortices. Furthermore, the time courses of activity in these areas differed, indicating that they subserve different functions in processing the emotional connotation of words. Namely, the retrosplenial cortex appears to be involved in evaluating the emotional salience of information from external sources, whereas the rostral frontal cortex also plays a role in internal generation of words with emotional connotation. In both areas, activity associated with positive or negative emotional connotation was more extensive in the left hemisphere than the right, regardless of valence, presumably due to the language demands of word generation. The present findings localize specific areas in the brain that are involved in processing emotional meaning of words within the brain's distributed semantic system. In addition, time course analysis reveals diverging mechanisms in anterior and posterior cortical areas during processing of words with emotional significance.


Subject(s)
Cerebral Cortex/physiology , Emotions/physiology , Frontal Lobe/physiology , Functional Laterality/physiology , Mental Processes/physiology , Adult , Cerebrovascular Circulation/physiology , Cues , Female , Humans , Kinetics , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Male
8.
J Int Neuropsychol Soc ; 9(7): 1061-77, 2003 Nov.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-14738287

ABSTRACT

fMRI was used to determine the frontal, basal ganglia, and thalamic structures engaged by three facets of language generation: lexical status of generated items, the use of semantic vs. phonological information during language generation, and rate of generation. During fMRI, 21 neurologically normal subjects performed four tasks: generation of nonsense syllables given beginning and ending consonant blends, generation of words given a rhyming word, generation of words given a semantic category at a fast rate (matched to the rate of nonsense syllable generation), and generation of words given a semantic category at a slow rate (matched to the rate of generating of rhyming words). Components of a left pre-SMA-dorsal caudate nucleus-ventral anterior thalamic loop were active during word generation from rhyming or category cues but not during nonsense syllable generation. Findings indicate that this loop is involved in retrieving words from pre-existing lexical stores. Relatively diffuse activity in the right basal ganglia (caudate nucleus and putamen) also was found during word-generation tasks but not during nonsense syllable generation. Given the relative absence of right frontal activity during the word generation tasks, we suggest that the right basal ganglia activity serves to suppress right frontal activity, preventing right frontal structures from interfering with language production. Current findings establish roles for the left and the right basal ganglia in word generation. Hypotheses are discussed for future research to help refine our understanding of basal ganglia functions in language generation.


Subject(s)
Basal Ganglia/physiology , Frontal Lobe/physiology , Functional Laterality/physiology , Language , Linguistics , Adolescent , Adult , Basal Ganglia/anatomy & histology , Brain Mapping , Female , Frontal Lobe/anatomy & histology , Humans , Language Tests , Magnetic Resonance Imaging/methods , Male , Memory/physiology , Paired-Associate Learning , Time Factors , Tomography, Emission-Computed
9.
J Int Neuropsychol Soc ; 8(5): 607-22, 2002 Jul.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-12164671

ABSTRACT

Previous studies showed that cortex in the anterior portions of the left frontal and temporal lobes participates in generating words with emotional connotations and processing pictures with emotional content. If these cortices process the semantic attribute of emotional connotation, they should be active whenever processing emotional connotation, without respect to modality of input or mode of output. Thus, we hypothesized that they would activate during monitoring of words with emotional connotations. Sixteen normal subjects performed semantic monitoring of words with emotional connotations, animal names, and implement names during fMRI. Cortex in the anterior left frontal lobe demonstrated significant activity for monitoring words with emotional connotations compared to monitoring tone sequences, animal names, or implement names. Together, the current and previous results implicate cortex in the anterior left frontal lobe in semantic processing of emotional connotation, consistent with connections of this cortex to paralimbic association areas. Current findings also indicate that neural substrates for processing emotional connotation are independent of substrates for processing the categories of living and nonliving things.


Subject(s)
Emotions/physiology , Prefrontal Cortex/physiology , Acoustic Stimulation , Adult , Auditory Perception/physiology , Female , Functional Laterality/physiology , Humans , Image Processing, Computer-Assisted , Imagination/physiology , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Male , Reaction Time/physiology , Semantics
10.
Clin Neuropsychol ; 16(4): 524-35, 2002 Dec.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-12822060

ABSTRACT

This study assessed the ability of normal controls to simulate mild traumatic brain injury with or without the aid of general simulation strategies. An additional purpose was to evaluate the relative ability of four tests of performance motivation or malingering to discriminate among the five groups in this study. Twenty-one patients with documented mild traumatic brain injury (TBI) and 112 undergraduate students were administered the measures of symptom validity in randomized order with instructions either to perform to the best of their ability or to fake believable deficits. Students asked to malinger were either given instructions to do so with no guidance (No Strategies group or NS), a minimal level of guidance (Only Strategies group or OS) or a moderate level of guidance (Strategies and Example or SE). Students given simulation strategies (OS and SE groups) were able to match performance of the TBI group in only those instances when TBI performance was similar to the normal comparison group. When TBI performance fell considerably below the normal comparison group, naïve simulators (NS group) best approximated TBI performance. The degree of variability in the classification success of the four tests underscored the necessity of combining detection methods, as well as the need to develop new tests more resistant to attempts to feign brain injury.


Subject(s)
Brain Injuries/psychology , Malingering/diagnosis , Malingering/psychology , Neuropsychological Tests/standards , Adult , Case-Control Studies , Female , Humans , Male , Patient Simulation , Psychometrics , Severity of Illness Index , Task Performance and Analysis
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...