Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 20 de 31
Filter
1.
Front Plant Sci ; 14: 1218984, 2023.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38023917

ABSTRACT

Rocket (Eruca vesicaria subsp. sativa) is a source of sulfur-containing glucosinolates (GSLs). GSLs and their breakdown hydrolysis products (GHPs) are responsible for health-related benefits, such as anti-cancer and anti-neurodegenerative properties. Understanding how phytochemical composition changes between cultivation environments is key to developing cultivars with improved nutritional quality. Two consecutive harvests (first and second regrowth) of crops, grown in both Italy and the UK, were used to determine the phytochemical and transcriptomic differences between six lines of Eruca. Samples were taken upon delivery from field sites (D0) and after five days of cold storage (D5) for each location. Leaves were analysed for sulfur content, volatile organic compounds (VOCs), GSLs, GHPs, and sugars. Transcriptome data were associated with metabolite profiles to identify differentially expressed genes between plants grown in the two environments. VOC compounds (carbon disulfide, methyl thiocyanate) were associated with growth environment and with differences in sulfur metabolism gene expression (APR2, LSU2, LSU3, SDI1, SiR), GSL biosynthesis (MYB28, FMOGS-OX2) and GHP formation (ESM1, TGG1, TGG2). The concentrations of sugars were an order of magnitude greater in UK grown samples (up to 29.9 mg g-1 dry weight; dw). Sulfur content was significantly higher in the Italy plant samples (11.4 - 20.1 mg g-1 dw), which was in turn associated with higher concentrations of GSLs (pentyl GSL, up to 15.8 µmol g-1 dw; sinigrin, up to 0.005 µmol g-1 dw; glucoraphanin, up to 5.1 µmol g-1 dw; glucorucolamine, up to 23.6 µmol g-1 dw; neoglucobrassicin, up to 5.3 µmol g-1 dw) and hydrolysis products (sativin, up to 13.5 µmol g-1 dw; erucin, up to 1 µmol g-1 dw; sulforaphane, up to 34.7 µmol g-1 dw). VOC profiles of plants cultivated in the UK were distinct from Italy grown plants, with higher relative abundances of alkanes and esters in second cut and shelf-life (D5) samples. The data indicate a significant interaction of cultivar response with environment, highlighting the difficulty of producing Eruca crops with consistent phytochemical and postharvest traits. Genes with differential expression between plants grown in Italy and the UK could be used as markers of phytochemical quality and composition.

2.
Int J Pharm Pract ; 31(5): 459-471, 2023 Sep 30.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37543960

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: Challenges to the provision of health care are occurring internationally and are expected to increase in the future, further increasing health spending. As pharmacist roles are evolving and expanding internationally to provide individualised pharmaceutical care it is important to assess the cost-effectiveness of these services. OBJECTIVES: To systematically synthesise the international literature regarding published economic evaluations of pharmacy services to assess their cost-effectiveness and clinical outcomes. METHODS: A systematic review of economic evaluations of pharmacy services was conducted in MEDLINE, EMBASE, PubMed, Scopus, Web of Science, CINAHL, IPA and online journals with search functions likely to publish economic evaluations of pharmacy services. Data were extracted regarding the interventions, the time horizon, the outcomes and the incremental cost-effectiveness ratio. Studies' quality of reporting was assessed using the Consolidated Health Economic Evaluation Reporting Standard (CHEERS) statement. RESULTS: Seventy-five studies were included in the systematic review, including 67 cost-effectiveness analyses, 6 cost-benefit analyses and 2 cost-consequence analyses. Of these, 57 were either dominant or cost-effective using a willingness-to-pay threshold of NZ$46 645 per QALY. A further 11 studies' cost-effectiveness were unable to be evaluated. Interventions considered to be most cost-effective included pharmacist medication reviews, pharmacist adherence strategies and pharmacist management of type 2 diabetes mellitus, hypertension and warfarin/INR monitoring. The quality of reporting of studies differed with no studies reporting all 28 items of the CHEERS statement. CONCLUSIONS: There is strong economic evidence to support investment in extended pharmacist services, particularly those focussed on long-term chronic health conditions.


Subject(s)
Diabetes Mellitus, Type 2 , Pharmaceutical Services , Humans , Cost-Benefit Analysis , Diabetes Mellitus, Type 2/drug therapy , Pharmacists , Cost-Effectiveness Analysis
3.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 120(18): e2213709120, 2023 05 02.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37094137

ABSTRACT

The philosopher John Rawls proposed the Veil of Ignorance (VoI) as a thought experiment to identify fair principles for governing a society. Here, we apply the VoI to an important governance domain: artificial intelligence (AI). In five incentive-compatible studies (N = 2, 508), including two preregistered protocols, participants choose principles to govern an Artificial Intelligence (AI) assistant from behind the veil: that is, without knowledge of their own relative position in the group. Compared to participants who have this information, we find a consistent preference for a principle that instructs the AI assistant to prioritize the worst-off. Neither risk attitudes nor political preferences adequately explain these choices. Instead, they appear to be driven by elevated concerns about fairness: Without prompting, participants who reason behind the VoI more frequently explain their choice in terms of fairness, compared to those in the Control condition. Moreover, we find initial support for the ability of the VoI to elicit more robust preferences: In the studies presented here, the VoI increases the likelihood of participants continuing to endorse their initial choice in a subsequent round where they know how they will be affected by the AI intervention and have a self-interested motivation to change their mind. These results emerge in both a descriptive and an immersive game. Our findings suggest that the VoI may be a suitable mechanism for selecting distributive principles to govern AI.


Subject(s)
Artificial Intelligence , Societies , Humans , Social Justice
5.
Mol Hortic ; 2(1): 23, 2022 Oct 10.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37789447

ABSTRACT

Eruca vesicaria subsp. sativa is a leafy vegetable of the Brassicaceae family known for its pungency. Variation in growing conditions, leaf age, agronomic practices, and variety choice lead to inconsistent quality, especially in content of isothiocyanates (ITCs) and their precursor glucosinolates (GSLs). We present the first linkage and Quantitative Trait Loci (QTL) map for Eruca, generated using a population of 139 F4 lines. A significant environmental effect on the abundance of primary and secondary metabolites was observed, with UK-grown plants containing significantly higher concentrations of glucoraphanin, malic acid, and total sugars. Italian-grown plants were characterized by higher concentrations of glucoerucin, indolic GSLs, and low monosaccharides. 20 QTL were identified and associated with robust SNP markers. Five genes putatively associated with the synthesis of the GSL 4-methoxyglucobrassicin (4MGB) were identified as candidate regulators underlying QTL. Analysis revealed that orthologs of MYB51, IGMT1 and IGMT4 present on LG1 are associated with 4MGB concentrations in Eruca. This research illustrates the utility of the map for identifying genes associated with nutritional composition in Eruca and its value as a genetic resource to assist breeding programs for this leafy vegetable crop.

6.
Neuron ; 109(4): 568-570, 2021 02 17.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33600753

ABSTRACT

In this issue of Neuron, Cross et. al (2021) use a deep reinforcement learning algorithm to understand human neural activation evoked by playing different video games. The results shed light on the neural principles underlying reward-guided decisions in naturalistic domains.


Subject(s)
Video Games , Algorithms , Brain , Humans , Reinforcement, Psychology , Reward
7.
J Allied Health ; 49(4): 285-289, 2020.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33259575

ABSTRACT

The term allied health is not an uncommon one within the healthcare lexicon. However, the derivation and meaning of the term sit within the murkiness of history, making relevance in the current context perplexing. This article sets out to explore the origins of the term, and in turn how the term and its associated meaning have developed internationally. To give perspective, case studies of the term within the United States, Australia, Scotland, and New Zealand are used. Each is traced within discoverable literature, and a conclusion is drawn as to how the term within these case studies developed, the impact of the term, and whether the term is seen as symbolic or associative.


Subject(s)
Allied Health Personnel , Internationality , Australia , Humans , New Zealand , United States
8.
Front Plant Sci ; 11: 525102, 2020.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33193472

ABSTRACT

Rocket (Eruca sativa) is a source of health-related metabolites called glucosinolates (GSLs) and isothiocyanates (ITCs) but little is known of the genetic and transcriptomic mechanisms responsible for regulating pre and postharvest accumulations. We present the first de novo reference genome assembly and annotation, with ontogenic and postharvest transcriptome data relating to sulfur assimilation, transport, and utilization. Diverse gene expression patterns related to sulfur metabolism, GSL biosynthesis, and glutathione biosynthesis are present between inbred lines of rocket. A clear pattern of differential expression determines GSL abundance and the formation of hydrolysis products. One breeding line sustained GSL accumulation and hydrolysis product formation throughout storage. Multiple copies of MYB28, SLIM1, SDI1, and ESM1 have increased and differential expression postharvest, and are associated with GSLs and hydrolysis product formation. Two glucosinolate transporter gene (GTR2) copies were found to be associated with increased GSL accumulations in leaves. Monosaccharides (which are essential for primary metabolism and GSL biosynthesis, and contribute to the taste of rocket) were also quantified in leaves, with glucose concentrations significantly correlated with the expression of numerous GSL-related genes. Significant negative correlations were observed between the expression of glutathione synthetase (GSH) genes and those involved in GSL metabolism. Breeding line "B" showed increased GSH gene expression and low GSL content compared to two other lines where the opposite was observed. Co-expression analysis revealed senescence (SEN1) and oxidative stress-related (OXS3) genes have higher expression in line B, suggesting that postharvest deterioration is associated with low GSL concentrations.

9.
Neuron ; 99(6): 1342-1354.e6, 2018 09 19.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30236285

ABSTRACT

Recent evidence challenges the widely held view that the hippocampus is specialized for episodic memory, by demonstrating that it also underpins the integration of information across experiences. Contemporary computational theories propose that these two contrasting functions can be accomplished by big-loop recurrence, whereby the output of the system is recirculated back into the hippocampus. We use ultra-high-resolution fMRI to provide support for this hypothesis, by showing that retrieved information is presented as a new input on the superficial entorhinal cortex-driven by functional connectivity between the deep and superficial entorhinal layers. Further, the magnitude of this laminar connectivity correlated with inferential performance, demonstrating its importance for behavior. Our findings offer a novel perspective on information processing within the hippocampus and support a unifying framework in which the hippocampus captures higher-order structure across experiences, by creating a dynamic memory space from separate episodic codes for individual experiences.


Subject(s)
Behavior/physiology , Cognition/physiology , Entorhinal Cortex/physiology , Hippocampus/physiology , Adult , Brain Mapping , Female , Humans , Male , Memory, Episodic , Temporal Lobe/physiology
10.
PLoS Biol ; 16(7): e2005479, 2018 07.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29965966

ABSTRACT

Systems-level consolidation refers to the time-dependent reorganisation of memory traces in the neocortex, a process in which the ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC) has been implicated. Capturing the precise temporal evolution of this crucial process in humans has long proved elusive. Here, we used multivariate methods and a longitudinal functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) design to detect, with high granularity, the extent to which autobiographical memories of different ages were represented in vmPFC and how this changed over time. We observed an unexpected time course of vmPFC recruitment during retrieval, rising and falling around an initial peak of 8-12 months, before reengaging for older 2- and 5-year-old memories. This pattern was replicated in 2 independent sets of memories. Moreover, it was further replicated in a follow-up study 8 months later with the same participants and memories, for which the individual memory representations had undergone their hypothesised strengthening or weakening over time. We conclude that the temporal engagement of vmPFC in memory retrieval seems to be nonmonotonic, revealing a complex relationship between systems-level consolidation and prefrontal cortex recruitment that is unaccounted for by current theories.


Subject(s)
Mental Recall/physiology , Prefrontal Cortex/physiology , Adult , Female , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Male , Young Adult
11.
Nature ; 557(7705): 429-433, 2018 05.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29743670

ABSTRACT

Deep neural networks have achieved impressive successes in fields ranging from object recognition to complex games such as Go1,2. Navigation, however, remains a substantial challenge for artificial agents, with deep neural networks trained by reinforcement learning3-5 failing to rival the proficiency of mammalian spatial behaviour, which is underpinned by grid cells in the entorhinal cortex 6 . Grid cells are thought to provide a multi-scale periodic representation that functions as a metric for coding space7,8 and is critical for integrating self-motion (path integration)6,7,9 and planning direct trajectories to goals (vector-based navigation)7,10,11. Here we set out to leverage the computational functions of grid cells to develop a deep reinforcement learning agent with mammal-like navigational abilities. We first trained a recurrent network to perform path integration, leading to the emergence of representations resembling grid cells, as well as other entorhinal cell types 12 . We then showed that this representation provided an effective basis for an agent to locate goals in challenging, unfamiliar, and changeable environments-optimizing the primary objective of navigation through deep reinforcement learning. The performance of agents endowed with grid-like representations surpassed that of an expert human and comparison agents, with the metric quantities necessary for vector-based navigation derived from grid-like units within the network. Furthermore, grid-like representations enabled agents to conduct shortcut behaviours reminiscent of those performed by mammals. Our findings show that emergent grid-like representations furnish agents with a Euclidean spatial metric and associated vector operations, providing a foundation for proficient navigation. As such, our results support neuroscientific theories that see grid cells as critical for vector-based navigation7,10,11, demonstrating that the latter can be combined with path-based strategies to support navigation in challenging environments.


Subject(s)
Biomimetics/methods , Machine Learning , Neural Networks, Computer , Spatial Navigation , Animals , Entorhinal Cortex/cytology , Entorhinal Cortex/physiology , Environment , Grid Cells/physiology , Humans
12.
Hippocampus ; 27(4): 417-424, 2017 04.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28032672

ABSTRACT

Neonatal hypoxia can lead to hippocampal atrophy, which can lead, in turn, to memory impairment. To test the generalizability of this causal sequence, we examined a cohort of 41 children aged 8-16, who, having received the arterial switch operation to correct for transposition of the great arteries, had sustained significant neonatal cyanosis but were otherwise neurodevelopmentally normal. As predicted, the cohort had significant bilateral reduction of hippocampal volumes relative to the volumes of 64 normal controls. They also had significant, yet selective, impairment of episodic memory as measured by standard tests of memory, despite relatively normal levels of intelligence, academic attainment, and verbal fluency. Across the cohort, degree of memory impairment was correlated with degree of hippocampal atrophy suggesting that even as early as neonatal life no other structure can fully compensate for hippocampal injury and its special role in serving episodic long term memory. © 2017 Wiley Periodicals, Inc.


Subject(s)
Hippocampus/pathology , Hypoxia-Ischemia, Brain/complications , Memory Disorders/diagnostic imaging , Memory Disorders/etiology , Transposition of Great Vessels/complications , Academic Success , Adolescent , Atrophy/diagnostic imaging , Atrophy/etiology , Child , Cohort Studies , Cyanosis/diagnostic imaging , Cyanosis/etiology , Cyanosis/psychology , Cyanosis/surgery , Female , Hippocampus/diagnostic imaging , Hippocampus/growth & development , Humans , Hypoxia-Ischemia, Brain/pathology , Intelligence , Language , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Male , Memory, Episodic , Neuropsychological Tests , Organ Size , Transposition of Great Vessels/diagnostic imaging , Transposition of Great Vessels/psychology , Transposition of Great Vessels/surgery
13.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 113(36): 10180-5, 2016 09 06.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27551087

ABSTRACT

Recent advances in neuroscience have given us unprecedented insight into the neural mechanisms of false memory, showing that artificial memories can be inserted into the memory cells of the hippocampus in a way that is indistinguishable from true memories. However, this alone is not enough to explain how false memories can arise naturally in the course of our daily lives. Cognitive psychology has demonstrated that many instances of false memory, both in the laboratory and the real world, can be attributed to semantic interference. Whereas previous studies have found that a diverse set of regions show some involvement in semantic false memory, none have revealed the nature of the semantic representations underpinning the phenomenon. Here we use fMRI with representational similarity analysis to search for a neural code consistent with semantic false memory. We find clear evidence that false memories emerge from a similarity-based neural code in the temporal pole, a region that has been called the "semantic hub" of the brain. We further show that each individual has a partially unique semantic code within the temporal pole, and this unique code can predict idiosyncratic patterns of memory errors. Finally, we show that the same neural code can also predict variation in true-memory performance, consistent with an adaptive perspective on false memory. Taken together, our findings reveal the underlying structure of neural representations of semantic knowledge, and how this semantic structure can both enhance and distort our memories.


Subject(s)
Mental Recall/physiology , Perceptual Distortion/physiology , Recognition, Psychology/physiology , Semantics , Temporal Lobe/physiopathology , Adolescent , Adult , Brain Mapping , Female , Hippocampus/anatomy & histology , Hippocampus/physiology , Hippocampus/physiopathology , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Male , Temporal Lobe/anatomy & histology , Temporal Lobe/physiology
14.
Brain Connect ; 6(1): 37-47, 2016 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26446207

ABSTRACT

Increasing evidence is emerging for sexual dimorphism in the trajectory of white matter development in children assessed using volumetric magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) and more recently diffusion MRI. Recent studies using diffusion MRI have examined cohorts with a wide age range (typically between 5 and 30 years) showing focal regions of differential diffusivity and fractional anisotropy (FA) and have implicated puberty as a possible contributory factor. To further investigate possible dimorphic trajectories in a young cohort, presumably closer to the expected onset of puberty, we used tract-based spatial statistics to investigate diffusion metrics. The cohort consisted of 23 males and 30 females between the ages of 8 and 16 years. Differences in diffusion metrics were corrected for age, total brain volume, and full scale IQ. In contrast to previous studies showing focal differences between males and females, widespread sexually dimorphic trajectories in structural white matter development were observed. These differences were characterized by more advanced development in females compared to males indicated by lower mean diffusivity, radial and axial diffusivity, and higher FA in females. This difference appeared to be larger at lower ages (8-9 years) with diffusion measures from males and females tending to converge between 10 and 14 years of age. Males showed a steeper slope for age-diffusion metric correlations compared to females, who either did not correlate with age or correlated in fewer regions. Further studies are now warranted to determine the role of hormones on the observed differences, particularly in 8-9-year-old children.


Subject(s)
Nerve Fibers, Myelinated/metabolism , Nerve Net/growth & development , Sexual Development , White Matter/growth & development , Adolescent , Anisotropy , Child , Diffusion Tensor Imaging/methods , Female , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Male , Sex Characteristics
15.
Front Psychol ; 6: 338, 2015.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25859236

ABSTRACT

Each of us has a rich set of autobiographical memories that provides us with a coherent story of our lives. These memories are known to be highly structured both thematically and temporally. However, it is not known how we naturally tend to explore the mental timeline of our memories. Here we developed a novel cued retrieval paradigm in order to investigate the temporal element of memory search. We found that, when asked to search for memories in the days immediately surrounding a salient cued event, participants displayed a marked set of temporal biases in their search patterns. Specifically, participants first tended to jump back in time and retrieve memories from the day prior to the cued event. Following this they then transitioned forward in time, and retrieved memories from the day after the cued event. This pattern of results replicated in a second experiment with a much larger group of participants, and a different method of cueing the memories. We argue that this set of temporal biases is consistent with memory search conforming to a temporally ordered narrative structure.

16.
Curr Biol ; 25(1): 87-92, 2015 Jan 05.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25532898

ABSTRACT

Navigating to a safe place, such as a home or nest, is a fundamental behavior for all complex animals. Determining the direction to such goals is a crucial first step in navigation. Surprisingly, little is known about how or where in the brain this "goal direction signal" is represented. In mammals, "head-direction cells" are thought to support this process, but despite 30 years of research, no evidence for a goal direction representation has been reported. Here, we used fMRI to record neural activity while participants made goal direction judgments based on a previously learned virtual environment. We applied multivoxel pattern analysis to these data and found that the human entorhinal/subicular region contains a neural representation of intended goal direction. Furthermore, the neural pattern expressed for a given goal direction matched the pattern expressed when simply facing that same direction. This suggests the existence of a shared neural representation of both goal and facing direction. We argue that this reflects a mechanism based on head-direction populations that simulate future goal directions during route planning. Our data further revealed that the strength of direction information predicts performance. Finally, we found a dissociation between this geocentric information in the entorhinal/subicular region and egocentric direction information in the precuneus.


Subject(s)
Entorhinal Cortex/physiology , Goals , Homing Behavior/physiology , Parietal Lobe/physiology , Animals , Female , Functional Neuroimaging , Healthy Volunteers , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Male
17.
Brain Struct Funct ; 220(1): 291-306, 2015 Jan.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24170375

ABSTRACT

The optic radiation (OR) is a component of the visual system known to be myelin mature very early in life. Diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) and its unique ability to reconstruct the OR in vivo were used to study structural maturation through analysis of DTI metrics in a cohort of 90 children aged 5-18 years. As the OR is at risk of damage during epilepsy surgery, we measured its position relative to characteristic anatomical landmarks. Anatomical distances, DTI metrics and volume of the OR were investigated for age, gender and hemisphere effects. We observed changes in DTI metrics with age comparable to known trajectories in other white matter tracts. Left lateralization of DTI metrics was observed that showed a gender effect in lateralization. Sexual dimorphism of DTI metrics in the right hemisphere was also found. With respect to OR dimensions, volume was shown to be right lateralised and sexual dimorphism demonstrated for the extent of the left OR. The anatomical results presented for the OR have potentially important applications for neurosurgical planning.


Subject(s)
Brain/anatomy & histology , Brain/growth & development , Diffusion Tensor Imaging , Optic Tract/anatomy & histology , Adolescent , Anisotropy , Brain Mapping , Child , Child, Preschool , Cohort Studies , Female , Humans , Image Processing, Computer-Assisted , Male , Nerve Fibers
19.
Neuroimage ; 102 Pt 2: 458-64, 2014 Nov 15.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25038440

ABSTRACT

Intelligent action entails exploiting predictions about associations between elements of ones environment. The hippocampus and mediotemporal cortex are endowed with the network topology, physiology, and neurochemistry to automatically and sparsely code sensori-cognitive associations that can be reconstructed from single or partial inputs. Whilst acquiring fMRI data and performing an attentional task, participants were incidentally presented with a sequence of cartoon images. By assigning subjects a post-scan free-association task on the same images we assayed the density of associations triggered by these stimuli. Using multivariate Bayesian decoding, we show that human hippocampal and temporal neocortical structures host sparse associative representations that are automatically triggered by visual input. Furthermore, as predicted theoretically, there was a significant increase in sparsity in the Cornu Ammonis subfields, relative to the entorhinal cortex. Remarkably, the sparsity of CA encoding correlated significantly with associative memory performance over subjects; elsewhere within the temporal lobe, entorhinal, parahippocampal, perirhinal and fusiform cortices showed the highest model evidence for the sparse encoding of associative density. In the absence of reportability or attentional confounds, this charts a distribution of visual associative representations within hippocampal populations and their temporal lobe afferent fields, and demonstrates the viability of retrospective associative sampling techniques for assessing the form of reflexive associative encoding.


Subject(s)
Association Learning/physiology , Hippocampus/physiology , Nerve Net/physiology , Temporal Lobe/physiology , Visual Perception/physiology , Adult , Attention/physiology , Brain Mapping , Female , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Male , Models, Neurological , Photic Stimulation , Young Adult
20.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 111(29): 10720-5, 2014 Jul 22.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25002463

ABSTRACT

There is enduring interest in why some of us have clearer memories than others, given the substantial individual variation that exists in retrieval ability and the precision with which we can differentiate past experiences. Here we report novel evidence showing that variation in the size of human hippocampal subfield CA3 predicted the amount of neural interference between episodic memories within CA3, which in turn predicted how much retrieval confusion occurred between past memories. This effect was not apparent in other hippocampal subfields. This shows that subtle individual differences in subjective mnemonic experience can be accurately gauged from measurable variations in the anatomy and neural coding of hippocampal region CA3. Moreover, this mechanism may be relevant for understanding memory muddles in aging and pathological states.


Subject(s)
CA3 Region, Hippocampal/anatomy & histology , CA3 Region, Hippocampal/physiology , Mental Recall/physiology , Adolescent , Adult , Female , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Male , Models, Neurological , Organ Size , Photic Stimulation , Young Adult
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...