Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 5 de 5
Filter
Add more filters










Database
Language
Publication year range
1.
J Food Drug Anal ; 24(2): 343-349, 2016 04.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28911588

ABSTRACT

Many species of the genus Phellinus possess beneficial properties, including antioxidant, immune-enhancing, and antimutagenic effects. Phenolic compounds and polysaccharides are two kinds of bioactive compounds; however, few studies have compared the differences between Phellinus igniarius and Phellinus linteus in their functional components, functional activities, and nutrient compositions. Herein, the proximate compositions and microelements of the fruiting body of P. igniarius and P. linteus were determined. The fruiting body of P. igniarius and P. linteus were extracted by boiling water [water extract of P. igniarius (WEPI) and P. linteus (WEPL)]. The contents of total phenolics and polysaccharides, as well as superoxide dismutase (SOD)-like and antimutagenic activities of WEPI and WEPL, were compared. We found that WEPI was rich in phenolics and polysaccharides and had higher SOD-like activity than WEPL. Nutrient compositions were mainly different in minerals, whereas anitmutagenicity was similar. All of these results suggested that P. igniarius has greater potential for the development of antioxidant and immunomodulating food products than P. linteus.


Subject(s)
Agaricales , Basidiomycota , Antimutagenic Agents , Antioxidants , Fruiting Bodies, Fungal , Phenols , Polysaccharides , Superoxides
2.
Food Chem Toxicol ; 50(11): 3902-11, 2012 Nov.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22921351

ABSTRACT

ß-Amyloid peptides (Aß) are neurotoxic and contribute to the development of Alzheimer's disease (AD). Ergothioneine (EGT) has been shown to protect against loss of memory and learning abilities in mice. In this study, mice were orally fed EGT (0.5 or 2 mg/kg body weight) for 16 days before treatment (i.c.v) with a single dose of Aß1-40 in the hippocampus. After resting for 12 days to restore the body weight, the mice were again fed EGT for additional 39 days. Active avoidance tests were conducted on days 37-39 (short-memory avoidance) and on days 37, 44 and 51 (long-memory avoidance). Water maze task was used to evaluate learning and memory abilities by acquisition test and retention test. In both long-memory avoidance and water maze tests, EGT significantly decreased the escape latency and increased the frequency of successful avoidance. Furthermore, EGT significantly prevented Aß accumulation in the hippocampus and brain lipid peroxidation, restored acetylcholinesterase (AChE) activity, maintained glutathione/glutathione disulfide ratio and superoxide dismutase activity in brain tissues of Aß1-40-teated mice. Thus, EGT can protect against Aß-induced loss of memory and learning abilities in mice. Further studies are required to confirm the protective effects of EGT on the development or progression of AD.


Subject(s)
Amyloid beta-Peptides/toxicity , Brain/drug effects , Ergothioneine/pharmacology , Neurons/drug effects , Peptide Fragments/toxicity , Acetylcholinesterase/metabolism , Administration, Oral , Alzheimer Disease/drug therapy , Amyloid beta-Peptides/metabolism , Animals , Avoidance Learning/drug effects , Brain/metabolism , Ergothioneine/administration & dosage , Glutathione/metabolism , Glutathione Disulfide/metabolism , Hippocampus/drug effects , Hippocampus/metabolism , Lipid Peroxidation/drug effects , Maze Learning/drug effects , Memory Disorders/prevention & control , Mice , Mice, Inbred C57BL , Neurons/metabolism , Neuroprotective Agents/pharmacology , Peptide Fragments/metabolism , Superoxide Dismutase/metabolism
3.
J Vis ; 10(4): 25.1-15, 2010 Apr 29.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20465343

ABSTRACT

Horizontal binocular disparity has long been the conventional predictor of stereo depth. Surprisingly, an alternative predictor fairs just as well. This alternative predicts the relative depth of two stimuli from the relation between their disparity vectors, without regard to horizontal disparities. These predictions can differ; horizontal disparities accurately predict the perceived depth of a grating and a plaid only when the grating is vertical, while the vector calculation accurately predicts it at all except near-horizontal grating orientations. For spatially two-dimensional stimulus pairs, such as plaids, dots, and textures, the predictions cannot be distinguished when the stimuli have the same disparity direction or when the disparity direction of one of the stimuli is horizontal or has a magnitude of zero. These are the conditions that have prevailed in earlier studies. We tested whether the perceived depth of two-dimensional stimuli depends on relative horizontal disparity magnitudes or on relative disparity magnitudes along a disparity axis. On both measures tested-depth matches and depth-interval matches-the perceived depth of plaids varied with their horizontal disparities and not with disparity direction differences as observed for grating-plaid pairs. Differences in disparity directions as great as 120 degrees did not affect depth judgments. This result, though opposite the grating-plaid data, is consistent with them and provides a view into the construction of orientation-invariant disparity representations.


Subject(s)
Depth Perception/physiology , Orientation/physiology , Photic Stimulation/methods , Female , Humans , Lighting
4.
J Vis ; 9(10): 3.1-19, 2009 Sep 04.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19810784

ABSTRACT

Even though binocular disparity is a very well-studied cue to depth, the function relating disparity and perceived depth has been characterized only for the case of horizontal disparities. We sought to determine the general relationship between disparity and depth for a particular set of stimuli. The horizontal disparity direction is a special case, albeit an especially important one. Non-horizontal disparities arise from a number of sources under natural viewing condition. Moreover, they are implicit in patterns that are one-dimensional, such as gratings, lines, and edges, and in one-dimensional components of two-dimensional patterns, where a stereo matching direction is not well-defined. What function describes perceived depth in these cases? To find out, we measured the phase disparities that produced depth matches between a reference stimulus and a test stimulus. The reference stimulus was two-dimensional, a plaid; the test stimulus was one-dimensional, a grating. We find that horizontal disparity is no more important than other disparity directions in determining depth matches between these two stimuli. As a result, a grating and a plaid appear equal in depth when their horizontal disparities are, in general, unequal. Depth matches are well predicted by a simple disparity vector calculation; they survive changes in component parameters that conserve these vector quantities. The disparity vector rule also describes how the disparities of 1-D components might contribute to the perceived depth of 2-D stimuli.


Subject(s)
Depth Perception , Photic Stimulation/methods , Vision Disparity , Humans , Orientation , Psychophysics
5.
Vision Res ; 49(17): 2209-16, 2009 Aug.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19540869

ABSTRACT

Binocular disparities have a straightforward geometric relation to object depth, but the computation that humans use to turn disparity signals into depth percepts is neither straightforward nor well understood. One seemingly solid result, which came out of Wheatstone's work in the 1830s, is that the sign and magnitude of horizontal disparity predict the perceived depth of an object: 'positive' horizontal disparities yield the perception of 'far' depth, 'negative' horizontal disparities yield the perception of 'near' depth, and variations in the magnitude of horizontal disparity monotonically increase or decrease the perceived extent of depth. Here we show that this classic link between horizontal disparity and the perception of 'near' versus 'far' breaks down when the stimuli are one-dimensional. For these stimuli, horizontal is not a privileged disparity direction. Instead of relying on horizontal disparities to determine their depth relative to that of two-dimensional stimuli, the visual system uses a disparity calculation that is non-veridical yet well suited to deal with the joint coding of disparity and orientation.


Subject(s)
Depth Perception/physiology , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Vision Disparity/physiology , Humans , Photic Stimulation/methods , Psychometrics , Psychophysics , Vision, Binocular/physiology
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...