Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 5 de 5
Filter
Add more filters










Database
Language
Publication year range
1.
J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform ; 37(1): 38-47, 2011 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21038993

ABSTRACT

Recent evidence suggests that the recognition of an object's presence and its explicit recognition are temporally closely related. Here we re-examined the time course (using a fine and a coarse temporal resolution) and the sensitivity of three possible component processes of visual object recognition. In particular, participants saw briefly presented (Experiment I to III) or noise masked (Experiment IV) static images of objects and non-object textures. Participants reported the presence of an object, its basic level category, and its subordinate category while we measured recognition performance by means of accuracy and reaction times. All three recognition tasks were clearly separable in terms of their time course and sensitivity. Finally, the use of a coarser temporal sampling of presentation times decreased performance differences between the detection and basic level categorization task suggesting that a fine temporal sampling for the dissociation of recognition performances is important. Overall the three probed recognition processes were associated with different time courses and sensitivities.


Subject(s)
Recognition, Psychology/physiology , Visual Perception/physiology , Adolescent , Adult , Female , Humans , Judgment/physiology , Male , Photic Stimulation , Reaction Time , Time Factors , Young Adult
2.
PLoS One ; 5(11): e15395, 2010 Nov 15.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21085590

ABSTRACT

The understanding of individual differences in response to threat (e.g., attentional bias) is important to better understand the development of anxiety disorders. Previous studies revealed only a small attentional bias in high-anxious (HA) subjects. One explanation for this finding may be the assumption that all HA-subjects show a constant attentional bias. Current models distinguish HA-subjects depending on their level of tolerance for uncertainty and for arousal. These models assume that only HA-subjects with intolerance for uncertainty but tolerance for arousal ("sensitizers") show an attentional bias, compared to HA-subjects with intolerance for uncertainty and intolerance for arousal ("fluctuating subjects"). Further, it is assumed that repressors (defined as intolerance for arousal but tolerance for uncertainty) would react with avoidance behavior when confronted with threatening stimuli. The present study investigated the influence of coping styles on attentional bias. After an extensive recruiting phase, 36 subjects were classified into three groups (sensitizers, fluctuating, and repressors). All subjects were exposed to presentations of happy and threatening faces, while recording gaze durations with an eye-tracker. The results showed that only sensitizer showed an attentional bias: they gazed longer at the threatening face rather than at the happy face during the first 500 ms. The results support the findings of the relationship between anxiety and attention and extend these by showing variations according to coping styles. The differentiation of subjects according to a multifaceted coping style allows a better prediction of the attentional bias and contributes to an insight into the complex interplay of personality, coping, and behavior.


Subject(s)
Adaptation, Psychological/physiology , Anxiety/physiopathology , Attention/physiology , Face , Adult , Anxiety/psychology , Anxiety Disorders/physiopathology , Anxiety Disorders/psychology , Arousal/physiology , Facial Expression , Female , Humans , Male , Reaction Time/physiology , Task Performance and Analysis , Young Adult
3.
J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform ; 33(5): 1127-44, 2007 Oct.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17924812

ABSTRACT

This article investigates vehicle steering control, focusing on the task of lane changing and the role of different sources of sensory feedback. Participants carried out 2 experiments in a fully instrumented, motion-based simulator. Despite the high level of realism afforded by the simulator, participants were unable to complete a lane change in the absence of visual feedback. When asked to produce the steering movements required to change lanes and turn a corner, participants produced remarkably similar behavior in each case, revealing a misconception of how a lane-change maneuver is normally executed. Finally, participants were asked to change lanes in a fixed-based simulator, in the presence of intermittent visual information. Normal steering behavior could be restored using brief but suitably timed exposure to visual information. The data suggest that vehicle steering control can be characterized as a series of unidirectional, open-loop steering movements, each punctuated by a brief visual update.


Subject(s)
Automobile Driving , Feedback , Motor Vehicles , Visual Perception , Adult , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged
4.
Curr Biol ; 12(4): 295-9, 2002 Feb 19.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11864569

ABSTRACT

Some motor tasks can be completed, quite literally, with our eyes shut. Most people can touch their nose without looking or reach for an object after only a brief glance at its location. This distinction leads to one of the defining questions of movement control: is information gleaned prior to starting the movement sufficient to complete the task (open loop), or is feedback about the progress of the movement required (closed loop)? One task that has commanded considerable interest in the literature over the years is that of steering a vehicle, in particular lane-correction and lane-changing tasks. Recent work has suggested that this type of task can proceed in a fundamentally open loop manner, with feedback mainly serving to correct minor, accumulating errors. This paper reevaluates the conclusions of these studies by conducting a new set of experiments in a driving simulator. We demonstrate that, in fact, drivers rely on regular visual feedback, even during the well-practiced steering task of lane changing. Without feedback, drivers fail to initiate the return phase of the maneuver, resulting in systematic errors in final heading. The results provide new insight into the control of vehicle heading, suggesting that drivers employ a simple policy of "turn and see," with only limited understanding of the relationship between steering angle and vehicle heading.


Subject(s)
Automobile Driving , Feedback , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Vision, Ocular/physiology , Computer Simulation , Humans , Photic Stimulation , Time Factors
5.
J Exp Psychol Appl ; 8(4): 247-58, 2002 Dec.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-12570099

ABSTRACT

The effects of gaze eccentricity on the steering of an automobile were studied. Drivers performed an attention task while attempting to drive down the middle of a straight road in a simulation. Steering was biased in the direction of fixation, and deviation from the center of the road was proportional to the gaze direction until saturation at approximately 15 degrees gaze-angle from straight ahead. This effect remains when the position of the head was controlled and a reverse-steering task was used. Furthermore, the effect was not dependent on speed but reversed when the forward movement of the driver was removed from the simulation. Thus, small deviations in a driver's gaze can lead to significant impairments of the ability to drive a straight course.


Subject(s)
Fixation, Ocular , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Visual Perception/physiology , Adolescent , Adult , Attention , Female , Humans , Male , Random Allocation
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...