Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 20 de 31
Filter
1.
Behav Brain Res ; 461: 114865, 2024 Mar 12.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38220058

ABSTRACT

Animal research suggests trait-like individual variation in the degree of incentive salience attribution to reward-predictive cues, defined phenotypically as sign-tracking (high) and goal-tracking (low incentive salience attribution). While these phenotypes have been linked to addiction features in rodents, their translational validity is less clear. Here, we examined whether sign- and goal-tracking in healthy human volunteers modulates the effects of reward-paired cues on decision making. Sign-tracking was measured in a Pavlovian conditioning paradigm as the amount of eye gaze fixation on the reward-predictive cue versus the location of impending reward delivery. In Study 1 (Cherkasova et al., 2018), participants were randomly assigned to perform a binary choice task in which rewards were either accompanied (cued, n = 63) or unaccompanied (uncued, n = 68) by money images and casino jingles. In Study 2, participants (n = 58) performed cued and uncued versions of the task in a within-subjects design. Across both studies, cues promoted riskier choice. Sign-tracking was not associated with risky choice in either study. Goal-tracking rather than sign-tracking was significantly associated with greater risk-promoting effects of cues in Study 1 but not in Study 2, although the direction of findings was consistent across both studies. These findings are at odds with the notion of sign-trackers being preferentially susceptible to the influence of reward cues on behavior and point to the role of mechanisms besides incentive salience in mediating such influences.


Subject(s)
Cues , Motivation , Humans , Goals , Reward
2.
J Clin Med ; 12(17)2023 Aug 22.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37685514

ABSTRACT

The mainstay treatments for Parkinson's Disease (PD) have been limited to pharmacotherapy and deep brain stimulation. While these interventions are helpful, a new wave of research is investigating noninvasive neuromodulation methods as potential treatments. Some promising avenues have included transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS), transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS), electroconvulsive therapy (ECT), and focused ultrasound (FUS). While these methods are being tested in PD patients, investigations in animal models of PD have sought to elucidate their therapeutic mechanisms. In this rapid review, we assess the available animal literature on these noninvasive techniques and discuss the possible mechanisms mediating their therapeutic effects based on these findings.

3.
PLoS One ; 17(7): e0272070, 2022.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35877672

ABSTRACT

Modern slot machines are among the more harmful forms of gambling. Psychophysiological measures may provide a window into mental processes that underpin these harms. Here we investigated pupil dilation derived from eye tracking as a means of capturing changes in sympathetic nervous system arousal following outcomes on a real slot machine. We hypothesized that positively reinforcing slot machine outcomes would be associated with increases in arousal, reflected in larger pupil diameter. We further examined the contribution of game luminance fluctuations on pupil diameter. In Experiment 1A, experienced slot machine gamblers (N = 53) played a commercially-available slot machine in a laboratory for 20 minutes while wearing mobile eye tracking glasses. Analyses differentiated loss outcomes, wins, losses-disguised-as-wins, and (free-spin) bonus features. Bonus features were associated with rapid increases in pupil diameter following the onset of outcome-related audiovisual feedback, relative to losses. In Experiment 1B, luminance data were extracted from captured screen videos (derived from Experiment 1A) to characterize on-screen luminance changes that could modulate pupil diameter. Bonus features and wins were associated with pronounced and complex fluctuations in screen luminance (≈50 L and ≈25L, respectively). However, the pupil dilation that was observed to bonus features in Experiment 1A coincided temporally with only negligible changes in screen luminance, providing partial evidence that the pupil dilation to bonus features may be due to arousal. In Experiment 2, 12 participants viewed pairs of stimuli (scrambled slot machine images) at luminance difference thresholds of ≈25L, ≈50L, and ≈100L. Scrambled images presented at luminance differences of ≈25L and greater were sufficient to cause pupillary responses. Overall, pupillometry may detect event-related changes in sympathetic nervous system arousal following gambling outcomes, but researchers must pay careful attention to substantial in-game luminance changes that may confound arousal-based interpretations.


Subject(s)
Gambling , Arousal/physiology , Humans , Pupil
4.
Sci Rep ; 12(1): 732, 2022 01 14.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35031632

ABSTRACT

Despite significant insights into the neural mechanisms of acute placebo responses, less is known about longer-term placebo responses, such as those seen in clinical trials, or their interactions with brain disease. We examined brain correlates of placebo responses in a randomized trial of a then controversial and now disproved endovascular treatment for multiple sclerosis. Patients received either balloon or sham extracranial venoplasty and were followed for 48 weeks. Venoplasty had no therapeutic effect, but a subset of both venoplasty- and sham-treated patients reported a transient improvement in health-related quality of life, suggesting a placebo response. Placebo responders did not differ from non-responders in total MRI T2 lesion load, count or location, nor were there differences in normalized brain volume, regional grey or white matter volume or cortical thickness (CT). However, responders had higher lesion activity. Graph theoretical analysis of CT covariance showed that non-responders had a more small-world-like CT architecture. In non-responders, lesion load was inversely associated with CT in somatosensory, motor and association areas, precuneus, and insula, primarily in the right hemisphere. In responders, lesion load was unrelated to CT. The neuropathological process in MS may produce in some a cortical configuration less capable of generating sustained placebo responses.


Subject(s)
Cerebral Cortex/pathology , Multiple Sclerosis/pathology , Multiple Sclerosis/psychology , Placebo Effect , Adolescent , Adult , Aged , Cerebral Cortex/diagnostic imaging , Diffusion Tensor Imaging , Endovascular Procedures/methods , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Multiple Sclerosis/surgery , Organ Size , Quality of Life , Randomized Controlled Trials as Topic , Young Adult
5.
J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry ; 61(3): 378-391, 2022 03.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34116167

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVE: To describe adult outcome of people with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) diagnosed in childhood and its several key predictors via a review of 7 North American controlled prospective follow-up studies: Montreal, New York, Milwaukee, Pittsburgh, Massachusetts General Hospital (MGH), Berkeley, and 7-site Multimodal Treatment Study of Children With ADHD (MTA). METHOD: All studies were prospective and followed children with a diagnosis of ADHD and an age- and gender-matched control group at regular intervals from childhood (6-12 years of age) through adolescence into adulthood (20-40 years of age), evaluating symptom and syndrome persistence, functional outcomes, and predictors of these outcomes. RESULTS: The rates of ADHD syndrome persistence ranged from 5.7% to 77%, likely owing to varying diagnostic criteria and the source of information (self-report vs informant report) across the studies. However, all studies observed high rates of symptomatic persistence ranging from 60% to 86%. The 7 studies were largely consistent in finding that relative to control groups, research participants with childhood-diagnosed ADHD had significant impairments in the areas of educational functioning, occupational functioning, mental health, and physical health as well as higher rates of substance misuse, antisocial behavior, and unsafe driving. The most consistently observed predictors of functional outcomes included ADHD persistence and comorbidity, especially with disruptive behavior disorders. CONCLUSION: Childhood ADHD has high rates of symptomatic persistence, which is associated with negative functional outcomes. Characteristics that predict these negative outcomes, such as comorbid disruptive behavior disorders, may be important targets for intervention.


Subject(s)
Attention Deficit Disorder with Hyperactivity , Adolescent , Adult , Attention Deficit and Disruptive Behavior Disorders , Child , Comorbidity , Follow-Up Studies , Humans , Prospective Studies , Young Adult
6.
J Cereb Blood Flow Metab ; 41(1): 116-131, 2021 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32050828

ABSTRACT

Current methods using a single PET scan to detect voxel-level transient dopamine release-using F-test (significance) and cluster size thresholding-have limited detection sensitivity for clusters of release small in size and/or having low release levels. Specifically, simulations show that voxels with release near the peripheries of such clusters are often rejected-becoming false negatives and ultimately distorting the F-distribution of rejected voxels. We suggest a Monte Carlo method that incorporates these two observations into a cost function, allowing erroneously rejected voxels to be accepted under specified criteria. In simulations, the proposed method improves detection sensitivity by up to 50% while preserving the cluster size threshold, or up to 180% when optimizing for sensitivity. A further parametric-based voxelwise thresholding is then suggested to better estimate the release dynamics in detected clusters. We apply the Monte Carlo method to a pilot scan from a human gambling study, where additional parametrically unique clusters are detected as compared to the current best methods-results consistent with our simulations.


Subject(s)
Dopamine/metabolism , Monte Carlo Method , Positron-Emission Tomography/methods , Humans
7.
J Behav Addict ; 9(4): 1044-1055, 2020 Nov 26.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33275122

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND AND AIMS: Individuals with gambling disorder display increased levels of risk-taking, but it is not known if it is associated with an altered subjective valuation of gains and/or losses, perception of their probabilities, or integration of these sources of information into expected value. METHODS: Participants with gambling disorder (n = 48) were compared with a healthy comparison group (n = 35) on a two-choice lottery task that involved either gains-only or losses-only gambles. On each trial, two lotteries were displayed, showing the associated probability and magnitude of the possible outcome for each. On each trial, participants chose one of the two lotteries, and the outcome was revealed. RESULTS: Choice behaviour was highly sensitive to the expected value of the two gambles in both the gain and loss domains. This sensitivity to expected value was attenuated in the group with gambling disorder. The group with gambling disorder used both probability and magnitude information less, and this impairment was greater for probability information. By contrast, they used prior feedback (win vs loss) to inform their next choice, despite the independence of each trial. Within the gambling disorder group, problem gambling severity and trait gambling-related cognitions independently predicted reduced sensitivity to expected value. The majority of observed effects were consistent across both gain and loss domains. DISCUSSION AND CONCLUSIONS: Our results provide a thorough characterization of decision processes in gain and loss domains in gambling disorder, and place these problems in the context of theoretical constructs from behavioural economics.


Subject(s)
Gambling , Choice Behavior , Cognition , Decision Making , Humans , Probability , Risk-Taking
8.
J Atten Disord ; 24(6): 889-903, 2020 04.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28413900

ABSTRACT

Objective: Recent trials have demonstrated efficacy of cognitive behavioral therapy (CBT) in medicated adults with ADHD. Efficacy of CBT in unmedicated versus medicated adults remains mostly unknown. We evaluated the effects of group CBT alone versus combined with medication on ADHD symptoms and functional outcomes in adult patients. Method: Eighty-eight adults with ADHD received 12 manualized group CBT sessions, accompanied by individual coaching, either without (n = 46) or with (n = 42) medication. Treatment effects were evaluated following treatment and 3-month and 6-month follow-up using un-blinded self-report and observer ratings. Results: CBT + medication resulted in greater improvements than CBT alone in ADHD symptoms, organizational skills, and self-esteem. Group differences diminished over follow-up, as the CBT alone group continued improving, while the combined group maintained the gains. Conclusion: CBT + medication outperformed CBT alone for ADHD symptoms, organizational skills, and self-esteem, although its superiority tended to decrease over follow-up.


Subject(s)
Attention Deficit Disorder with Hyperactivity , Cognitive Behavioral Therapy , Adult , Humans , Self Concept , Self Report , Treatment Outcome
9.
Addiction ; 115(6): 1127-1138, 2020 06.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31746072

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND AND AIMS: Immersion during slot machine gambling has been linked to disordered gambling. Current conceptualizations of immersion (namely dissociation, flow and the machine zone) make contrasting predictions as to whether gamblers are captivated by the game per se ('zoned in') or motivated by the escape that immersion provides ('zoned out'). We examined whether selected eye-movement metrics can distinguish between these predictions. DESIGN AND SETTING: Pre-registered, correlational analysis in a laboratory setting. Participants gambled on a genuine slot machine for 20 minutes while wearing eye-tracking glasses. PARTICIPANTS: Fifty-three adult slot machine gamblers who were not high-risk problem gamblers. MEASUREMENTS: We examined self-reported immersion during the gambling session and eye movements at different areas of the slot machine screen (the reels, the credit window, etc.). We further explored these variables' relationships with saccade count and amplitude. FINDINGS: The ratio of dwell time on the game's credit window relative to the game's reels was positively associated with immersion (t(51)  = 1.68, P = 0.049 one-tailed, R2  = 0.05). Follow-up analyses described event-related changes in these patterns following different spin outcomes. CONCLUSIONS: Immersion while gambling on a slot machine appears to be associated with active scanning of the game and a focus on the game's credit window. These results are more consistent with a 'zoned in' account of immersion aligned with flow theory than a 'zoned out' account based on escape.


Subject(s)
Eye-Tracking Technology/psychology , Gambling/psychology , Adult , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Motivation , Reinforcement, Psychology , Reward , Young Adult
10.
Parkinsonism Relat Disord ; 66: 189-194, 2019 09.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31473085

ABSTRACT

INTRODUCTION: Clinical evidence suggests that Parkinson's Disease (PD) patients are risk averse. Dopaminergic therapy has been reported to increase risk tolerance, but the underlying mechanisms are unclear. Some studies have suggested an amplification of subjective reward value, consistent with the role of dopamine in reward value coding. Others have reported value-independent risk enhancement. We evaluated the value-dependence of the effects of PD and its therapy on risk using tasks designed to sensitively measure risk over a wide range of expected values. METHOD: 36 patients with idiopathic PD receiving levodopa monotherapy and 36 healthy matched controls performed two behavioural economic tasks aimed at quantifying 1) risk tolerance/aversion in the gain frame and 2) valuation of potential gains relative to losses. PD patients performed the tasks on and off their usual dose of levodopa in randomized order; controls performed the same tasks twice. RESULTS: Relative to the controls, unmedicated PD patients showed significant value-independent risk aversion in the gain frame, which was normalized by levodopa. PD patients did not differ from controls in their valuation of gains relative to losses. However, across both tasks and regardless of medication, choices of the patients were more determined by expected values of the prospects than those of controls. CONCLUSION: Dopamine deficiency in PD was associated with risk aversion, and levodopa promoted riskier choice in a value-independent manner. PD patients also showed an increased sensitivity to expected value, which was independent of levodopa and does not appear to result directly from dopamine deficiency.


Subject(s)
Decision Making/drug effects , Dopamine Agents/pharmacology , Levodopa/pharmacology , Parkinson Disease/drug therapy , Parkinson Disease/physiopathology , Risk-Taking , Aged , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged
11.
Mov Disord ; 33(12): 1945-1950, 2018 12.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30376184

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: The benefits of exercise in PD have been linked to enhanced dopamine (DA) transmission in the striatum. OBJECTIVE: To examine differences in DA release, reward signaling, and clinical features between habitual exercisers and sedentary subjects with PD. METHODS: Eight habitual exercisers and 9 sedentary subjects completed [11 C]raclopride PET scans before and after stationary cycling to determine exercise-induced release of endogenous DA in the dorsal striatum. Additionally, functional MRI assessed ventral striatum activation during reward anticipation. All participants completed motor (UPDRS III; finger tapping; and timed-up-and-go) and nonmotor (Beck Depression Inventory; Starkstein Apathy Scale) assessments. RESULTS: [11 C]Raclopride analysis before and after stationary cycling demonstrated greater DA release in the caudate nuclei of habitual exercisers compared to sedentary subjects (P < 0.05). Habitual exercisers revealed greater activation of ventral striatum during the functional MRI reward task (P < 0.05) and lower apathy (P < 0.05) and bradykinesia (P < 0.05) scores versus sedentary subjects. CONCLUSIONS: Habitual exercise is associated with preservation of motor and nonmotor function, possibly mediated by increased DA release. This study formulates a foundation for prospective, randomized controlled studies. © 2018 International Parkinson and Movement Disorder Society.


Subject(s)
Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Parkinson Disease/diagnostic imaging , Aged , Caudate Nucleus/pathology , Caudate Nucleus/physiopathology , Dopamine/metabolism , Exercise , Female , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging/methods , Male , Middle Aged , Multimodal Imaging/methods , Parkinson Disease/complications , Parkinson Disease/pathology , Parkinson Disease/physiopathology , Positron-Emission Tomography , Raclopride , Reward , Ventral Striatum/pathology , Ventral Striatum/physiopathology
12.
J Neurosci ; 38(48): 10362-10370, 2018 11 28.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30373765

ABSTRACT

Reward-related stimuli can potently influence behavior; for example, exposure to drug-paired cues can trigger drug use and relapse in people with addictions. Psychological mechanisms that generate such outcomes likely include cue-induced cravings and attentional biases. Recent animal data suggest another candidate mechanism: reward-paired cues can enhance risky decision making, yet whether this translates to humans is unknown. Here, we examined whether sensory reward-paired cues alter decision making under uncertainty and risk, as measured respectively by the Iowa Gambling Task and a two-choice lottery task. In the cued versions of both tasks, gain feedback was augmented with reward-concurrent audiovisual stimuli. Healthy human volunteers (53 males, 78 females) performed each task once, one with and the other without cues (cued Iowa Gambling Task/uncued Vancouver Gambling Task: n = 63; uncued Iowa Gambling Task/cued Vancouver Gambling Task: n = 68), with concurrent eye-tracking. Reward-paired cues did not affect choice on the Iowa Gambling Task. On the two-choice lottery task, the cued group displayed riskier choice and reduced sensitivity to probability information. The cued condition was associated with reduced eye fixations on probability information shown on the screen and greater pupil dilation related to decision and reward anticipation. This pupil effect was unrelated to the risk-promoting effects of cues: the degree of pupil dilation for risky versus risk-averse choices did not differ as a function of cues. Together, our data show that sensory reward cues can promote riskier decisions and have additional and distinct effects on arousal.SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT Animal data suggest that reward-paired cues can promote maladaptive reward-seeking by biasing cost-benefit decision making. Whether this finding translates to humans is unknown. We examined the effects of salient reward-paired audiovisual cues on decision making under risk and uncertainty in human volunteers. Cues had risk-promoting effects on a risky choice task and independently increased task-related arousal as measured by pupil dilation. By demonstrating risk-promoting effects of cues in human participants, our data identify a mechanism whereby cue reactivity could translate into maladaptive behavioral outcomes in people with addictions.


Subject(s)
Choice Behavior/physiology , Cues , Fixation, Ocular/physiology , Risk-Taking , Adolescent , Adult , Decision Making/physiology , Eye Movements/physiology , Female , Gambling/psychology , Humans , Male , Random Allocation , Young Adult
13.
Front Hum Neurosci ; 11: 421, 2017.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28878639

ABSTRACT

Alterations in catecholamine signaling and cortical morphology have both been implicated in the pathophysiology of attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). However, possible links between the two remain unstudied. Here, we report exploratory analyses of cortical thickness and its relation to striatal dopamine transmission in treatment-naïve adults with ADHD and matched healthy controls. All participants had one magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) and two [11C]raclopride positron emission tomography scans. Associations between frontal cortical thickness and the magnitude of d-amphetamine-induced [11C]raclopride binding changes were observed that were divergent in the two groups. In the healthy controls, a thicker cortex was associated with less dopamine release; in the ADHD participants the converse was seen. The same divergence was seen for baseline D2/3 receptor availability. In healthy volunteers, lower D2/3 receptor availability was associated with a thicker cortex, while in the ADHD group lower baseline D2/3 receptor availability was associated with a thinner cortex. Individual differences in cortical thickness in these regions correlated with ADHD symptom severity. Together, these findings add to the evidence of associations between dopamine transmission and cortical morphology, and suggest that these relationships are altered in treatment-naïve adults with ADHD.

14.
J Clin Invest ; 124(8): 3285-8, 2014 Aug.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25036701

ABSTRACT

Evaluation of potential therapies for neurological disease has been challenging due to beneficial responses in patients receiving the sham/placebo treatment. Placebo effects are especially prominent in Parkinson's disease (PD), which has become a useful model for studying the neurobiology of placebo responses. In this issue of the JCI, Ko and colleagues identify a neural circuit associated with the placebo response in a PD patient cohort. The observed placebo effect-associated pattern involved metabolic activity increases that corresponded with long-term motor improvements after sham surgery. Presurgery activity in this network was inversely related to sham response, suggesting that this network has potential for identifying sham responders and thus reducing placebo-related variance in surgical trials.


Subject(s)
Brain/metabolism , Brain/surgery , Parkinson Disease/metabolism , Parkinson Disease/surgery , Placebo Effect , Female , Humans , Male
15.
Neuropsychopharmacology ; 39(6): 1498-507, 2014 May.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24378745

ABSTRACT

Converging evidence from clinical, preclinical, neuroimaging, and genetic research implicates dopamine neurotransmission in the pathophysiology of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). The in vivo neuroreceptor imaging evidence also suggests alterations in the dopamine system in ADHD; however, the nature and behavioral significance of those have not yet been established. Here, we investigated striatal dopaminergic function in ADHD using [(11)C]raclopride PET with a d-amphetamine challenge. We also examined the relationship of striatal dopamine responses to ADHD symptoms and neurocognitive function. A total of 15 treatment-free, noncomorbid adult males with ADHD (age: 29.87 ± 8.65) and 18 healthy male controls (age: 25.44 ± 6.77) underwent two PET scans: one following a lactose placebo and the other following d-amphetamine (0.3 mg/kg, p.o.), administered double blind and in random order counterbalanced across groups. In a separate session without a drug, participants performed a battery of neurocognitive tests. Relative to the healthy controls, the ADHD patients, as a group, showed greater d-amphetamine-induced decreases in striatal [(11)C]raclopride binding and performed more poorly on measures of response inhibition. Across groups, a greater magnitude of d-amphetamine-induced change in [(11)C]raclopride binding potential was associated with poorer performance on measures of response inhibition and ADHD symptoms. Our findings suggest an augmented striatal dopaminergic response in treatment-naive ADHD. Though in contrast to results of a previous study, this finding appears consistent with a model proposing exaggerated phasic dopamine release in ADHD. A susceptibility to increased phasic dopamine responsivity may contribute to such characteristics of ADHD as poor inhibition and impulsivity.


Subject(s)
Attention Deficit Disorder with Hyperactivity/drug therapy , Attention Deficit Disorder with Hyperactivity/metabolism , Corpus Striatum/metabolism , Dextroamphetamine/pharmacology , Dopamine Uptake Inhibitors/pharmacology , Dopamine/metabolism , Adult , Attention Deficit Disorder with Hyperactivity/psychology , Brain Mapping , Corpus Striatum/diagnostic imaging , Dextroamphetamine/blood , Dopamine Antagonists/pharmacokinetics , Dopamine Uptake Inhibitors/blood , Executive Function/physiology , Humans , Inhibition, Psychological , Male , Neuropsychological Tests , Psychiatric Status Rating Scales , Raclopride/pharmacokinetics , Radionuclide Imaging , Task Performance and Analysis
16.
Biol Psychiatry ; 76(1): 23-30, 2014 Jul 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24138922

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: Not everyone who tries addictive drugs develops a substance use disorder. One of the best predictors of risk is a family history (FH) of substance use problems. In part, this might reflect perturbed mesolimbic dopamine responses. METHODS: We measured amphetamine-induced changes in [(11)C]raclopride binding in 1) high-risk young adults with a multigenerational FH of substance use disorders (n = 16); 2) stimulant drug-naïve healthy control subjects with no known risk factors for addiction (n = 17); and 3) subjects matched to the high-risk group on personal drug use but without a FH of substance use problems (n = 15). RESULTS: Compared with either control group, the high-risk young adults with a multigenerational FH of substance use disorders exhibited smaller [(11)C]raclopride responses, particularly within the right ventral striatum. Past drug use predicted the dopamine response also, but including it as a covariate increased the group differences. CONCLUSIONS: Together, the results suggest that young people at familial high risk for substance use disorders have decreased dopamine responses to an amphetamine challenge, an effect that predates the onset of addiction.


Subject(s)
Amphetamine/pharmacology , Dopamine/metabolism , Family Health , Substance-Related Disorders/diagnosis , Case-Control Studies , Central Nervous System Stimulants/pharmacology , Corpus Striatum/diagnostic imaging , Corpus Striatum/metabolism , Dopamine Antagonists , Female , Functional Neuroimaging , Humans , Male , Positron-Emission Tomography , Prodromal Symptoms , Raclopride , Substance-Related Disorders/diagnostic imaging , Young Adult
17.
J Neurosci ; 33(38): 15285-94, 2013 Sep 18.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24048857

ABSTRACT

The meso-striatal dopamine system influences responses to rewards and the motivation to seek them out. Marked individual differences in these responses are seen in laboratory animals, related in part to input from the prefrontal cortex. Here we measured the relation between cortical morphology and drug-induced striatal dopamine release in healthy young people. Participants were 24 (17 male, 7 female; age 23.0 ± 6.2 years) stimulant drug-naive subjects who underwent PET [(11)C]raclopride scans with 0.3 mg/kg d-amphetamine orally and placebo, and an anatomical MRI scan for measuring cortical thickness. As expected, d-amphetamine produced significant reductions in [(11)C]raclopride binding potential in the striatum as a percentage of the value in the placebo condition. There was substantial individual variability in this response, which was correlated with cortical thickness in the frontal lobe as a whole. The association was strongest in the anterior part of the right lateral prefrontal cortex and bilateral supplementary motor area. A thicker cortex was correlated with a smaller dopamine response. Together, this work demonstrates in humans an association between cortical thickness and the striatal dopamine response to drugs of abuse. Although prefrontal regulation of striatal function has been well studied, it was unclear whether the thickness of the prefrontal cortex was an acceptable proxy to the function of that region. These results suggest it is.


Subject(s)
Corpus Striatum/drug effects , Dextroamphetamine/pharmacology , Dopamine Uptake Inhibitors/pharmacology , Dopamine/metabolism , Individuality , Prefrontal Cortex/anatomy & histology , Adolescent , Adult , Blood Pressure/drug effects , Brain Mapping , Carbon Isotopes/pharmacokinetics , Corpus Striatum/diagnostic imaging , Dopamine Antagonists/pharmacokinetics , Dose-Response Relationship, Drug , Double-Blind Method , Female , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Male , Models, Neurological , Positron-Emission Tomography , Prefrontal Cortex/diagnostic imaging , Raclopride/pharmacokinetics , Statistics as Topic , Young Adult
18.
Can J Psychiatry ; 54(10): 651-64, 2009 Oct.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19835672

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVES: To review the findings of structural and functional neuroimaging studies in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), with a focus on abnormalities reported in brain regions that lie outside the frontostriatal circuitry, which is currently believed to play a central role in the pathophysiology of ADHD. METHODS: Relevant publications were found primarily by searching the MEDLINE and PubMed databases using the keywords ADHD and the abbreviations of magnetic resonance imaging (MRI), functional MRI, positron emission tomography, and single photon emission computed tomography. The reference lists of the articles found through the databases were then reviewed for the purpose of finding additional articles. RESULTS: There is now substantial evidence of structural and functional alterations in regions outside the frontostriatal circuitry in ADHD, most notably in the cerebellum and the parietal lobes. CONCLUSIONS: Although there is compelling evidence suggesting that frontostriatal dysfunction may be central to the pathophysiology of ADHD, the neuroimaging findings point to distributed neural substrates rather than a single one. More research is needed to elucidate the nature of contributions of nonfrontostriatal regions to the pathophysiology of ADHD.


Subject(s)
Attention Deficit Disorder with Hyperactivity/physiopathology , Brain/physiopathology , Corpus Striatum/physiopathology , Frontal Lobe/physiopathology , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Nerve Net/physiopathology , Positron-Emission Tomography , Tomography, Emission-Computed, Single-Photon , Adolescent , Attention/physiology , Attention Deficit Disorder with Hyperactivity/diagnosis , Attention Deficit Disorder with Hyperactivity/pathology , Attention Deficit Disorder with Hyperactivity/psychology , Brain/pathology , Brain Mapping , Cerebellum/pathology , Cerebellum/physiopathology , Child , Corpus Striatum/pathology , Female , Frontal Lobe/pathology , Humans , Luria-Nebraska Neuropsychological Battery , Male , Nerve Net/pathology , Parietal Lobe/pathology , Parietal Lobe/physiopathology , Temporal Lobe/pathology , Temporal Lobe/physiopathology
19.
Neurology ; 69(9): 860-70, 2007 Aug 28.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17724288

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: Patients with social developmental disorders (SDD), also known as autism spectrum disorders, may have impaired recognition of facial identity or facial expressions. OBJECTIVE: Our goal was to determine whether SDDs were characterized by loss of a perceptual mechanism responsible for face expertise, as current theories suggest that such a loss should be selective for upright faces, disproportionately affect the perception of facial configuration, and possibly be more severe in the eye region. METHOD: We tested a group of 24 adult patients with SDD with an oddity paradigm that required them to detect changes in facial configuration or feature color, in either the eyes or the mouth, in both upright and inverted faces. RESULTS: One group of subjects with SDD with normal famous face recognition had only a mild reduction in accuracy and a normal pattern of inversion effects. A second group of subjects with SDD with impaired famous face recognition had a severe reduction of accuracy. This deficit was not limited to upright faces. It affected the perception of feature configuration and feature color to a similar degree and both eye and mouth changes were discriminated poorly in upright faces. CONCLUSION: The impaired face recognition that is present in a subset of patients with social developmental disorders is accompanied by impaired face perception, and this impairment is not exclusive to upright faces, facial configuration, or the eye region. The reduced face processing skills in these subjects may be more consistent with recent computational models of face expertise than with classic dual-route hypotheses.


Subject(s)
Autistic Disorder/physiopathology , Face , Pattern Recognition, Visual , Perceptual Disorders/physiopathology , Adolescent , Adult , Autistic Disorder/diagnosis , Autistic Disorder/psychology , Brain/physiopathology , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Neuropsychological Tests , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Perceptual Disorders/diagnosis , Perceptual Disorders/psychology , Photic Stimulation , Predictive Value of Tests
20.
Exp Brain Res ; 181(2): 199-211, 2007 Aug.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17361425

ABSTRACT

The scan patterns of ocular fixations made by prosopagnosic patients while they attempt to identify faces may provide insights into how they process the information in faces. Contrasts between their scanning of upright versus inverted faces may index the presence of a hypothesized orientation-dependent expert mechanism for processing faces, while contrasts between their scanning of familiar versus novel faces may index the influence of residual facial memories on their search for meaningful facial information. We recorded the eye movements of two prosopagnosics while they viewed faces. One patient, with acquired prosopagnosia from a right occipitotemporal lesion, showed degraded orientation effects but still with a normal distribution of fixations to more salient facial features. However, the dynamics of his global scan patterns were more chaotic for novel faces, suggesting degradation of an internal facial schema, and consistent with other evidence of impaired face configuration perception in this patient. His global scan patterns for famous faces differed from novel faces, suggesting the influence of residual facial memories, as indexed previously by his relatively good imagery for famous faces. The other patient, with a developmental prosopagnosia, showed anomalous orientation effects, abnormal distribution of fixations to less salient regions, and chaotic global scan patterns, in keeping with a more severe loss of face-expert mechanisms. The effects of fame on her scanning were weaker than those in the first subject and non-existent in her global scan patterns. We conclude that scan patterns in prosopagnosia can both reflect the loss of orientation-dependent expert mechanisms and index the covert influence of residual facial memories. In these two subjects the scanning data were consistent with other results from tests of configuration perception, imagery, and covert recognition.


Subject(s)
Prosopagnosia/physiopathology , Prosopagnosia/psychology , Recognition, Psychology/physiology , Adult , Brain/pathology , Brain Neoplasms/complications , Brain Neoplasms/surgery , Cerebral Hemorrhage/etiology , Cerebral Hemorrhage/psychology , Data Interpretation, Statistical , Epilepsy, Generalized/psychology , Eye Movements/physiology , Face , Female , Fixation, Ocular , Functional Laterality/physiology , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Male , Markov Chains , Memory/physiology , Middle Aged , Neuropsychological Tests , Oligodendroglioma/complications , Oligodendroglioma/surgery , Orientation/physiology , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Visual Perception/physiology
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...