Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 17 de 17
Filter
Add more filters










Publication year range
1.
J Opt Soc Am A Opt Image Sci Vis ; 35(7): 1124-1130, 2018 Jul 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30110304

ABSTRACT

The visual performance of a reference group (RG) using diffuser filters was compared to a cataract-diagnosed group (CatG). Measurements of straylight (SL) parameter, photopic contrast sensitivity (CS), and visual acuity (VA) were carried out in both groups. Before the analysis, the performance of the instruments used for this purpose was tested. The RG was comprised of three healthy, young eyes (25-30 years old) while 59 subjects (aged 50-80 years old) with lens opacities were recruited for the CatG. Six diffuser conditions were tested in the RG. To discriminate between light scattering levels, SL measurements proved to be most sensitive, VA did not discriminate at all, while CS showed intermediate sensitivity. VA was not correlated with SL, while the correlation between CS and SL was significant (p<0.05) in both groups. Since the correlation in the RG was particularly strong, parameters of a linear regression model are presented. The behavior of CS as a function of SL was comparable to some extent between RG and CatG.


Subject(s)
Contrast Sensitivity , Vision Tests/methods , Adult , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Case-Control Studies , Cataract/physiopathology , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Scattering, Radiation , Vision Tests/instrumentation , Visual Acuity
2.
BMC Res Notes ; 11(1): 424, 2018 Jul 03.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29970151

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVE: Determination of color and evaluating its variation form the basis for a broad range of research questions. For studies on taxonomy, systematics, etc., resorting to mammal specimens in museum collections has a number of advantages over using field specimens. However, if museum specimens are to be for studying color, they should accurately represent the color of live animals, or we should understand how they differ. Basically, this study addresses this question: How does coat color vary when dealing with specimens of Akodon budini (Budin's grass mouse, Thomas 1918), stored in one museum collection for different periods of time? RESULTS: We measured color values through a spectroradiometer and a diffuse illumination cabin and used the reflectance values in the form of CIELab tri-stimulus values, considering CIE standard illuminant A. We observed that there is a relationship between specimen storage antiquity and pelage color and it seems that it is general for at least a number of small mammals and this could indicate a universal phenomenon across several mammal species and across several storage conditions. Our results, as others, emphasize the importance of considering storage time, among other circumstances, in research studies using mammal skins and where color is of importance.


Subject(s)
Mammals , Museums , Adult , Animals , Color , Humans , Lighting , Mice
3.
J Opt Soc Am A Opt Image Sci Vis ; 33(8): 1546-52, 2016 Aug 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27505653

ABSTRACT

The purpose of this study is to determine the pupillary dynamics with periodical flashes from a peripheral glare source, in similar conditions to night driving, while focusing on dependence with age. We measured two groups of people: youth and adults. Maximum pupil size decreases due to periodic flashes. Latency does not present significant differences. The reduction of pupil size is greater for older adults. The presence of a peripheral and periodic glare source modifies the pupil size. This leads to a reduction of retinal illuminance, which is greater for older adults.


Subject(s)
Adaptation, Physiological/physiology , Aging/physiology , Light , Mesopic Vision/physiology , Pupil/physiology , Adaptation, Physiological/radiation effects , Humans , Mesopic Vision/radiation effects , Middle Aged , Young Adult
4.
An Acad Bras Cienc ; 88 Suppl 1: 489-502, 2016.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27142543

ABSTRACT

The role of color in taxonomic and systematic studies of several taxa is central. Color data are more valuable if they permit comparisons. We aimed to provide pelage objective color measurements of a series of Akodon budini and to use those color data to test and to quantitatively analyze the influence of both the lighting source and the measuring point, in the determination of pelage color of museum skins. We used a spectroradiometer to measure the pelage color at five points over the dorsal midline of 54 Akodon budini museum skins. PCAs and ANOVAs were conducted over the color data. The characteristics of the studied series also allowed us to include and assess the potential effects of some main sources of intra-specific variation. Determinations of fur color strongly depend on the lighting source, and therefore it is essential that lighting conditions are controlled during color measurements and then made explicit when communicating the color characterization of particular taxonomic units. Furthermore, color determinations strongly depend on the measuring point, even on the same body area, and therefore it is critical that the color characterization of a taxon includes the variation between different points of the different body parts of the studied specimens.


Subject(s)
Animal Fur , Arvicolinae , Color , Animals , Arvicolinae/classification , Lighting
5.
Perception ; 45(8): 931-945, 2016 Aug.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27125640

ABSTRACT

We analyzed different contrast metrics to scale the stimulus strength for suprathreshold reaction times (RTs) when it is modulated along an achromatic channel (L + M) and both chromatic channels L/M and S/(L + M) considering increments and decrements along these axes. RTs were examined as a function of the Weber luminance contrast; spatial luminance ratio (SRL) and, in terms of threshold units. The results show that when there is only luminance decreasing or increasing, RTs cluster around a single RT/luminance contrast function regardless the stimulus sign and our results indicate that both SRL, Weber luminance contrast or threshold units, equate RT values. While, if the stimulus is modulated along an isoluminant plane, the appropriate contrast is Weber (RMS) or SRL for stimuli modulated along L/M axis and for stimuli modulated along S/L + M, showing an asymmetry between S-cone decrements and increments in L/M cone pathway. Threshold units are not appropriate, showing inconsistencies: The stimulus with chromatic direction equal to 90° appears as the most informative with a maximum gain. Even more so, the shared contrast gain grows as the size of the stimulus decreases.


Subject(s)
Contrast Sensitivity/physiology , Psychophysics/methods , Reaction Time/physiology , Sensory Thresholds/physiology , Humans
6.
Curr Eye Res ; 41(9): 1209-15, 2016 09.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26766561

ABSTRACT

PURPOSE: To measure log(s) and OSI parameters, both related to forward light scattering in the eye, in subjects with different kinds of early cataracts-nuclear or posterior subcapsular-and corrected visual acuity (CVA). METHODS: 34 eyes of 19 patients ranged between 50 and 75 years old with diagnosed nuclear (14 eyes) or posterior subcapsular cataract (20 eyes) were recruited. Only NO1, NO2, P1, and P2 opacity scores according to LOCS III were included. Observer examination included visual acuity, contrast threshold (Ct), and measurements performed by straylightmeter (straylight parameter log(s)) and double-pass instrument (objective scatter index (OSI)). RESULTS: OSI and log(s) were correlated with LOCSIII in nuclear opacities (p = 0.015 and 0.004, respectively) and in the whole data (p = 0.027 and 0.019, respectively) but did not for posterior subcapsular opacities alone. OSI was strongly correlated with log(s) in nuclear (r = 0.885 and p < 0.001) but not in posterior subcapsular cases (r = 0.382 and p = 0.097). Ct was correlated with log(s) for both cataract types (p = 0.043 for nuclear and p= 0.005 for posterior subcapsular cataract) but not with OSI (p = 0.093 for nuclear and p = 0.064 for posterior subcapsular cataract). CONCLUSIONS: OSI and log(s) discriminate early stages of nuclear cataracts when taking LOCS III as reference, so these opacities could be graded by any of those parameters. LOCSIII does not represent the visual condition for posterior subcapsular cataract. Straylightmeter measurements express the loss in contrast sensitivity caused by nuclear and posterior subcapsular opacities. Studies of lens opacities must be separated according to the type of opacity present in eyes.


Subject(s)
Cataract/diagnosis , Contrast Sensitivity/physiology , Light , Visual Acuity/physiology , Aged , Cataract/physiopathology , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Prospective Studies , Scattering, Radiation , Vision Tests
7.
Curr Eye Res ; 39(12): 1187-93, 2014 Dec.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24750121

ABSTRACT

PURPOSE: The aim of this article is to propose a quantitative methodology for determining a criterion to discriminate the nonsurgical nuclear cataract from the surgical one taking into account objective measures of intraocular scattering in patients with good visual acuity (>0.6). METHODS: Two groups of subjects were taken into account: a control group and a group with nuclear cataracts. At a first stage, eyes belonging to the cataract group were classified into "nonsurgical" and "surgical" cataracts by ophthalmologists at their clinical settings. At a second stage a double-pass instrument was also used to determine the objective scatter index (OSI) at the laboratory. Receiver operating characteristic (ROC) curves were used to analyze OSI values to determine a value able to separate between nonsurgical and surgical cataracts. RESULTS: We obtained statistically significant differences among the control and both nuclear cataract groups (p < 0.05). ROC curves determined an OSI criterion level (of 2.1) to suggest surgery in nuclear cataracts with an area under curve of 0.83, i.e. with 80% of sensitivity and 80% of specificity. CONCLUSIONS: ROC analysis allows separating both groups of nuclear cataract, and we determined a value of OSI in nuclear cataract quantification for surgery.


Subject(s)
Cataract Extraction , Cataract/diagnosis , Lens Nucleus, Crystalline/pathology , ROC Curve , Scattering, Radiation , Adult , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Cataract/classification , Humans , Lens Nucleus, Crystalline/radiation effects , Light , Middle Aged , Visual Acuity , Young Adult
8.
Psychol. neurosci. (Impr.) ; 6(2): 219-226, jul.-dez. 2013. ilus
Article in English | LILACS | ID: lil-699238

ABSTRACT

In this work we introduce a new category of barriers that we call "functional vision barriers." This expression refers to lighting and visual elements that may complicate or hinder functional vision and may make life even more difficult for people with visual defects. These barriers appear as a consequence of certain negative effects caused by the poor design of the visual stimulus or visual environment that surrounds it in which lighting is one of the main factors. We use the term "functional vision" because this expression refers to the ability of the visual system to perform everyday tasks. We analyzed some of our previous results with regard to situations that can be considered "functional vision barriers": (1) stimuli with low luminance contrast information in which the addition of chromatic contrast improves visual performance and (2) tasks that are performed in the presence of a glare source in the visual field, diminishing visual performance and reducing brightness perception...


Subject(s)
Humans , Lighting , Photic Stimulation , Vision Disorders , Visual Perception
9.
Psychol. neurosci. (Impr.) ; 6(2): 219-226, 2013. ilus
Article in English | Index Psychology - journals | ID: psi-61336

ABSTRACT

In this work we introduce a new category of barriers that we call "functional vision barriers." This expression refers to lighting and visual elements that may complicate or hinder functional vision and may make life even more difficult for people with visual defects. These barriers appear as a consequence of certain negative effects caused by the poor design of the visual stimulus or visual environment that surrounds it in which lighting is one of the main factors. We use the term "functional vision" because this expression refers to the ability of the visual system to perform everyday tasks. We analyzed some of our previous results with regard to situations that can be considered "functional vision barriers": (1) stimuli with low luminance contrast information in which the addition of chromatic contrast improves visual performance and (2) tasks that are performed in the presence of a glare source in the visual field, diminishing visual performance and reducing brightness perception.(AU)


Subject(s)
Lighting , Visual Perception , Photic Stimulation , Vision Disorders
10.
J Opt Soc Am A Opt Image Sci Vis ; 29(10): 2230-6, 2012 Oct 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23201673

ABSTRACT

The presence of a glare source in the visual field produces a veiling luminance (L(v)), which generates a brightness reduction that can be expressed in terms of the glare index (V). The relation between the veiling luminance caused by glare and the apparent brightness reduction of a reference target has already been established for steady conditions. In this paper, the relationship is derived for transient glare. First, the relation is tested empirically, and then previous results concerning the effect of transient glare on brightness are summarized and analyzed. From this analysis, a power function relation between L(v) and V is encountered.

11.
J Opt Soc Am A Opt Image Sci Vis ; 28(10): 2187-91, 2011 Oct 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21979526

ABSTRACT

When a bright light is present in the field of view, visibility is dramatically reduced. Many studies have investigated the effect of glare on visibility considering foveal vision. However, the effects on peripheral vision have received little attention. In a previous work [J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 25, 1790 (2008)], we showed that the effect of glare on reaction time (RT) for foveal vision at mesopic adaptation depends on the stimulus spatial frequency. In this work, we extend this study to peripheral vision. We measured the RT of achromatic sinusoidal gratings as a function of contrast for a range of spatial frequency, and eccentricity, and for two glare levels, in addition to the no-glare condition. Data were fitted with Piéron's law, following a linear relationship. We found that glare increases the slope of these lines for all conditions. These slopes seem to depend critically on eccentricity for 4 cycles/degree (c/deg), but not for 1 and 2 c/deg. We explain our results in terms of the contrast sensitivity (gain) of the underlying detection mechanisms.

12.
Perception ; 40(1): 50-62, 2011.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21513184

ABSTRACT

We measured at the fovea the chromatic contrast threshold for stimuli modulated along different chromatic directions in the isoluminant plane of MBDKL colour space, considering the two cardinal axes (L/M) and S/(L + M) and other intermediate non-cardinal directions. This psychophysical determination was conducted as a function of stimulus size. The test stimulus was a foveal isoluminant Gaussian patch with a raised cosinusoidal temporal profile superimposed on a neutral background. The task was performed binocularly. The increment threshold was measured for three observers by a Bayesian adaptive psychometric method (QUEST). The Ricco area of complete spatial summation was estimated from the threshold-versus-area curves. The perceptive fields are smaller for the L/M-cone opponent direction than the S/(L + M)-cone opponent. The perceptive field sizes for the stimuli in non-cardinal chromatic directions and stimuli modulated at the (L/M)-cone opponent direction present similar values. Measurements were made at two luminance levels, 5 and 40 cd m(-2), but the differences found were small. The perceptive field sizes found could be associated with LGN area.


Subject(s)
Color Perception/physiology , Fovea Centralis/physiology , Retinal Cone Photoreceptor Cells/physiology , Adult , Analysis of Variance , Bayes Theorem , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Photic Stimulation/methods , Psychophysics/methods , Sensory Thresholds/physiology
13.
Vis Neurosci ; 27(3-4): 119-29, 2010 Jul.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20594382

ABSTRACT

We present a series of experiments exploring the effect of chromaticity on reaction time (RT) for a variety of stimulus conditions, including chromatic and luminance contrast, luminance, and size. The chromaticity of these stimuli was varied along a series of vectors in color space that included the two chromatic-opponent-cone axes, a red-green (L-M) axis and a blue-yellow [S - (L + M)] axis, and intermediate noncardinal orientations, as well as the luminance axis (L + M). For Weber luminance contrasts above 10-20%, RTs tend to the same asymptote, irrespective of chromatic direction. At lower luminance contrast, the addition of chromatic information shortens the RT. RTs are strongly influenced by stimulus size when the chromatic stimulus is modulated along the [S - (L + M)] pathway and by stimulus size and adaptation luminance for the (L-M) pathway. RTs are independent of stimulus size for stimuli larger than 0.5 deg. Data are modeled with a modified version of Pieron's formula with an exponent close to 2, in which the stimulus intensity term is replaced by a factor that considers the relative effects of chromatic and achromatic information, as indexed by the RMS (square-root of the cone contrast) value at isoluminance and the Weber luminance contrast, respectively. The parameters of the model reveal how RT is linked to stimulus size, chromatic channels, and adaptation luminance and how they can be interpreted in terms of two chromatic mechanisms. This equation predicts that, for isoluminance, RTs for a stimulus lying on the S-cone pathway are higher than those for a stimulus lying on the L-M-cone pathway, for a given RMS cone contrast. The equation also predicts an asymptotic trend to the RT for an achromatic stimulus when the luminance contrast is sufficiently large.


Subject(s)
Color Perception/physiology , Color , Contrast Sensitivity/physiology , Reaction Time/physiology , Adaptation, Physiological/physiology , Adult , Analysis of Variance , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Models, Statistical , Photic Stimulation/methods , Psychophysics/methods
14.
Vision Res ; 49(13): 1613-9, 2009 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19336241

ABSTRACT

We present a series of experiments exploring the effect of the stimulus spatial configuration on speed discrimination and two different types of segmentation, for random dot patterns. In the first experiment, we find that parsing the image produces a decrease of speed discrimination thresholds such as was first shown by Verghese and Stone [Verghese, P., & Stone, L. (1997). Spatial layout affects speed discrimination threshold. Vision Research, 37(4), 397-406; Verghese, P., & Stone, L. S. (1996). Perceived visual speed constrained by image segmentation. Nature, 381, 161-163] for sinusoidal gratings. In the second experiment, we study how the spatial configuration affects the ability of a subject in localizing an illusory contour defined by two surfaces with different speeds. Results show that the speed difference necessary to localize the contour decreases as the stimulus patches are separated. The third experiment involves transparency. Our results show a little or null effect for this condition. We explain the first and second experiment in the framework of the model of Bravo and Watamaniuk [Bravo, M., & Watamaniuk, S. (1995). Evidence for two speed signals: a coarse local signal for segregation and a precise global signal for discrimination. Vision Research, 35(12), 1691-1697] who proposed that motion computation consists in, at least, two stages: a first computation of coarse local speeds followed by an integration stage. We propose that the more precise estimate of speed obtained from the integration stage is used to produce a new refined segmentation of the image perhaps, through a feedback loop. Our data suggest that this third stage would not apply to the processing of transparency.


Subject(s)
Motion Perception/physiology , Space Perception/physiology , Discrimination, Psychological/physiology , Humans , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Photic Stimulation/methods , Psychophysics , Sensory Thresholds/physiology
15.
J Opt Soc Am A Opt Image Sci Vis ; 25(7): 1790-8, 2008 Jul.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18594637

ABSTRACT

We systematized the study of the effect of glare on reaction time (RT), for visual conditions similar to the ones found during night driving: Mesopic range of adaptation (0.14 cd/m2), glare levels of the order of those produced by car headlights (E(G)=15, 60 lx), suprathreshold luminance contrasts, and a variety of spatial frequencies covering the selected range of visibility (1, 2, 4, and 8 c/deg). We found that for the no-glare situation, RT increases with decreasing contrast and increasing spatial frequency, which agrees with previous findings. When data are plotted as a function of the inverse of contrast, RT varies linearly, with k--the RT-contrast factor of Pieron's law--representing the slope of the lines. The effect of glare on RT is an increase in the slope of these lines. This effect is different for each spatial frequency, which cannot be accounted for in the classic approach considering that glare can be replaced by a single veiling luminance. We show that the effect of glare on RT must be modeled by an equivalent glare luminance that depends on spatial frequency.

16.
Percept Psychophys ; 68(4): 702-9, 2006 May.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16933432

ABSTRACT

We measured the effect of a transient glare source on t heperceived brightness of astandard luminance (L(STD)) patch (0.5 cd/m2) as a function of the surround luminance (Ls). In the experiment, both increment and decrement stimuli were dependent on the value of the Ls (0.01, 0.2, 0.4, 0.6, 0.8, 1.0, 1.5, or 2.0 cd/m2). We adopted a magnitude comparison paradigm using constant stimuli to determine the test matching luminance (L(M)). When L(S) was lower than the luminance of the patch, which corresponds to increments, LM was lower than L(STD), and this effect was highest for the lowest L(S). There was a small but noticeable cusp as increments shifted to decrements. As L(S) increased further (i.e., as the decrement grew), L(M) flattened out below L(STF). The overall pattern of results could be interpreted in terms of the concept of contrast brightness, with consideration of the intrinsic differences in brightness evaluations between decrements and increments.


Subject(s)
Light , Visual Perception , Adult , Humans , Male , Models, Psychological , Vision, Binocular
17.
J Opt Soc Am A Opt Image Sci Vis ; 23(2): 233-8, 2006 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16477827

ABSTRACT

It was shown that a peripheral glare source reduces the brightness of a foveal stimulus. We hypothesized that this brightness reduction is governed by an inhibitory effect of the glare source on the test. We reported the results of an investigation of the dynamic of brightness reduction of an incremental stimulus immediately after the onset of a glare source in the field of view. A magnitude comparison paradigm using constant stimuli was adopted to determine the luminance that appeared equal in brightness to the standard patch. The luminance of the standard stimulus was in the mesopic range (0.5 cd/m2), and the levels of glare were 15 and 60 lx. Results showed that the time course of brightness reduction followed the typical shape attributed to the Broca-Sulzer effect. Data were fitted with a model that first considers the response of a peripheral ganglion cell to glare and then its inhibitory effect on the test signals. We discussed the plausibility of a postretinal stage of processing.


Subject(s)
Contrast Sensitivity/physiology , Fovea Centralis/physiology , Glare , Lighting , Models, Neurological , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Retinal Ganglion Cells/physiology , Adult , Computer Simulation , Female , Fovea Centralis/radiation effects , Humans , Light , Male , Photic Stimulation/methods , Retinal Ganglion Cells/radiation effects , Sensory Thresholds/physiology , Time Factors
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...