Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 13 de 13
Filter
1.
J Pers ; 68(3): 451-89, 2000 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10831309

ABSTRACT

The Riverside Behavioral Q-sort (RBQ) is a flexible technique for gathering a wide-ranging description of the behavior of individuals in dyadic social interaction. Ratings of RBQ items can attain adequate reliability to reflect behavioral effects of experimental manipulations and to manifest meaningful correlations with a variety of personality characteristics. The RBQ's flexibility, validity, and relative ease of use may facilitate the more frequent inclusion of behavioral data in personality and social psychology.


Subject(s)
Personality , Social Behavior , Female , Humans , Interpersonal Relations , Male , Reproducibility of Results
2.
J Pers ; 64(2): 311-37, 1996 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8656321

ABSTRACT

In this article we compare the accuracy of personality judgements by the self and by knowledgeable others. Self- and acquaintance judgements of general personality attributes were used to predict general, videotaped behavioral criteria. Results slightly favored the predictive validity of personality judgements made by single acquaintances over self-judgements, and significantly favored the aggregated personality judgements of two acquaintances over self-judgements. These findings imply that the most valid source for personality judgements that are relevant to patterns of overt behavior may not be self-reports but the consensus of the judgement of the community of one's peers.


Subject(s)
Ego , Judgment , Peer Group , Personality , Self-Assessment , Female , Humans , Male , Personality Assessment
3.
J Pers Soc Psychol ; 68(6): 1152-62, 1995 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-7608859

ABSTRACT

The relation between overly positive self-evaluations and psychological adjustment was examined. Three studies, two based on longitudinal data and another on laboratory data, contrasted self-descriptions of personality with observer ratings (trained examiners or friends) to index self-enhancement. In the longitudinal studies, self-enhancement was associated with poor social skills and psychological maladjustment 5 years before and 5 years after the assessment of self-enhancement. In the laboratory study, individuals who exhibited a tendency to self-enhance displayed behaviors, independently judged, that seemed detrimental to positive social interaction. These results indicate there are negative short-term and long-term consequences for individuals who self-enhance and, contrary to some prior formulations, imply that accurate appraisals of self and of the social environment may be essential elements of mental health.


Subject(s)
Adaptation, Psychological , Personality , Reality Testing , Self Concept , Adult , Defense Mechanisms , Female , Humans , Interpersonal Relations , Longitudinal Studies , Male , Social Environment , Social Perception
4.
Psychol Bull ; 116(1): 3-20, 1994 Jul.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8078973

ABSTRACT

The proposition recently offered by S. E. Taylor and J. D. Brown (1988) that positive illusions foster mental health has garnered considerable attention and acceptance. However, the significant theoretical and applied implications of their view for mental health require a critical evaluation of their argument. An examination of the logic and empirical evidence used to relate mental health to three key positive illusions--unrealistically positive views of the self, illusions of control, and unrealistic optimism--failed to substantiate Taylor and Brown's thesis. Further survey of more recent studies on positive illusions and mental health also failed to lend support to the Taylor and Brown generalization. Close consideration of several assumptions underlying the formulation raises further questions regarding their thesis. The present article concludes that it remains unproven that positive illusions foster mental health.


Subject(s)
Illusions , Mental Health , Acquired Immunodeficiency Syndrome , Cognition , Humans , Self Concept
5.
J Pers ; 61(4): 611-35, 1993 Dec.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8151501

ABSTRACT

Research has recently demonstrated that the personalities of some individuals are more accurately judged than others, and that these "judgable" persons possess a coherent and identifiable personality structure (Colvin, 1993). In this article, four indices of judgability, based on personality descriptions provided by trained examiners, friends, and self, were derived when subjects were 18 and 23 years of age. A reliable composite of the four indices was related to adolescent and childhood personality ratings. The results reveal (a) rank-order stability of judgability from age 18 to age 23, (b) that adolescent ego resiliency predicts judgability in early adulthood, and (c) that adolescent ego resiliency mediates the relationship between childhood personality and young-adult judgability, but only for men. Observed gender differences and similarities are discussed. Overall, the convergence between the results from this study and previous research provides evidence for the construct validity of judgability.


Subject(s)
Personality Assessment , Personality , Adolescent , Adult , Female , Humans , Male , Observer Variation , Sex Factors
6.
J Pers Soc Psychol ; 64(5): 861-73, 1993 May.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8505714

ABSTRACT

"Judgability" discriminates people who are open and knowable from those who are closed and enigmatic. The current study investigated this individual difference characteristic and its personality and behavioral correlates. Subjects (N = 164) were videotaped interacting with a partner of the opposite sex. They completed several personality measures, and each subject was described by 2 friends. Three indices--subject-peer agreement, peer-peer agreement, and peer-behavior agreement--formed a reliable composite index of judgability. The correlates of judgability included extraversion, agreeableness, conscientiousness, emotional stability, and, more generally, psychological adjustment.


Subject(s)
Emotions , Interpersonal Relations , Personality Development , Self Disclosure , Social Behavior , Adult , Female , Humans , Individuality , Male , Peer Group , Self Concept
7.
J Pers Soc Psychol ; 61(3): 492-503, 1991 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-1941521

ABSTRACT

Recent research indicates that happiness, or affective well-being, is related primarily to the frequency, not to the intensity, of positive affect (PA). The question arises as to why intense positive affect (PI) is not a larger contributor to subjective well-being. Whether processes that yield PI also produce intense negative affect was examined. Studies 1 and 2 suggested that cognitive mechanisms that amplify or dampen affect can carry over from positive to negative events. Study 3 demonstrated that, because of judgment mechanisms, an extremely positive event can make other events less positive. Study 4 revealed that naturally occurring intensely positive experiences are often preceded by negative ones. Study 5 suggested that the more persons valence success at a task, the happier they will be if they succeed, but unhappier if they fail. The 5 studies reveal that intense positive experiences may sometimes have costs that counterbalance their desirable nature.


Subject(s)
Adaptation, Psychological , Arousal , Depression/psychology , Happiness , Adult , Empathy , Female , Humans , Life Change Events
8.
J Pers Assess ; 57(1): 149-61, 1991 Aug.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-1920028

ABSTRACT

The structure of subjective well-being has been conceptualized as consisting of two major components: the emotional or affective component and the judgmental or cognitive component (Diener, 1984; Veenhoven, 1984). The judgmental component has also been conceptualized as life satisfaction (Andrews & Withey, 1976). Although the affective component of subjective well-being has received considerable attention from researchers, the judgmental component has been relatively neglected. The Satisfaction With Life Scale (SWLS; Diener, Emmons, Larsen, & Griffin, 1985) was developed as a measure of the judgmental component of subjective well-being (SWB). Two studied designed to validate further the SWLS are reported. Peer reports, a memory measure, and clinical ratings are used as external criteria for validation. Evidence for the reliability and predictive validity of the SWLS is presented, and its performance is compared to other related scales. The SWLS is shown to be a valid and reliable measure of life satisfaction, suited for use with a wide range of age groups and applications, which makes possible the savings of interview time and resources compared to many measures of life satisfaction. In addition, the high convergence of self- and peer-reported measures of subjective well-being and life satisfaction provide strong evidence that subjective well-being is a relatively global and stable phenomenon, not simply a momentary judgment based on fleeting influences.


Subject(s)
Aging/psychology , Attitude to Health , Personal Satisfaction , Personality Tests/statistics & numerical data , Adult , Affect , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Female , Humans , Life Style , Male , Middle Aged , Psychometrics , Reference Values
9.
J Pers Soc Psychol ; 60(6): 884-94, 1991 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-1865326

ABSTRACT

Recent research has shown that interjudge agreement in personality ratings increases with acquaintanceship. The present study sought to replicate and extend this finding by investigating the relation between acquaintanceship and behavioral prediction. A total of 138 undergraduate targets were videotaped while interacting with an opposite-sex partner in 3 situations. The targets also completed 5 personality measures. Results indicated that acquaintances' judgments predicted personality scores much better than did strangers' judgments, but acquaintances' and strangers' judgments did equally well at predicting behavior. Implications for research on the accuracy of personality judgments are discussed.


Subject(s)
Interpersonal Relations , Personality , Set, Psychology , Social Behavior , Adult , Female , Humans , Male , Q-Sort , Social Environment
10.
J Pers Soc Psychol ; 60(5): 773-94, 1991 May.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-2072255

ABSTRACT

In a study exploring the cross-situational consistency of behavior, 140 undergraduate Ss were video-taped in each of 3 laboratory settings, and personality descriptions of these Ss were obtained from friends and acquaintances. Analyses focused on the degree to which Ss maintained consistent patterns of behavior across laboratory settings and between these settings and daily life. The following conclusions were reached: (a) Behavior can exhibit impressive consistency at the level of psychological meaning, (b) psychological properties of situations can be detected from their behavioral effects, (c) cross-situational consistency and discriminativeness are independent, and (d) some behaviors are more consistent than others.


Subject(s)
Individuality , Personality Assessment , Social Behavior , Social Environment , Adaptation, Psychological , Adult , Female , Humans , Male , Q-Sort
11.
J Pers Soc Psychol ; 55(1): 149-58, 1988 Jul.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-3418488

ABSTRACT

We examined the effect of acquaintanceship on interjudge agreement in personality ratings. Approximately 150 undergraduates described their own personalities using the Q-sort. They were also described by two close acquaintances and by two "strangers" who knew them only via a single, spontaneous interaction viewed on videotape. The effect of acquaintanceship was powerful: Judgments by close acquaintances agreed with each other and with subjects' self-judgments much better than did judgments by strangers, even though strangers' judgments agreed with each other and with subjects' self-judgments beyond a chance level. This result implies that agreement among acquaintances' judgments must derive at least partly from experience with and observation of the person who is judged. The same traits that yielded better agreement among acquaintances also yielded better agreement among strangers and tended to be rated higher in subjective visibility, suggesting that people are intuitively knowledgeable about the traits they can judge with more and less agreement.


Subject(s)
Interpersonal Relations , Judgment , Personality , Adult , Female , Humans , Male , Q-Sort
12.
J Rehabil ; 38(3): 28-9, 1972.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-5024352
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...