Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 20 de 23
Filter
1.
PLoS One ; 16(11): e0258586, 2021.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34731203

ABSTRACT

Despite several attempts to provide a definite pattern regarding the effects of personality traits on performance in higher education, the debate over the nature of the relationship is far from being conclusive. The use of different subject pools and sample sizes, as well as the use of identification strategies that either do not adequately account for selection bias or are unable to establish causality between measures of academic performance and noncognitive skills, are possible sources of heterogeneity. This paper investigates the impact of the Big Five traits, as measured before the beginning of the academic year, on the grade point average achieved in the first year after the enrolment, taking advantage of a unique and large dataset from a cohort of Italian students in all undergraduate programs containing detailed information on student and parental characteristics. Relying on a robust strategy to credibly satisfy the conditional independence assumption, we find that higher levels of conscientiousness and openness to experience positively affect student score.


Subject(s)
Academic Performance , Emotions , Personality/genetics , Students, Medical/psychology , Adult , Education/standards , Female , Humans , Italy/epidemiology , Male , Students/psychology , Universities/standards , Young Adult
2.
Front Psychol ; 12: 732248, 2021.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34690888

ABSTRACT

Whistleblowing is a powerful and rather inexpensive instrument to deter tax evasion. Despite the deterrent effects on tax evasion, whistleblowing can reduce trust and undermine agents' attitude to cooperate with group members. Yet, no study has investigated the potential spillover effects of whistleblowing on ingroup cooperation. This paper reports results of a laboratory experiment in which subjects participate in two consecutive phases in unchanging groups: a tax evasion game, followed by a generalized gift exchange game. Two dimensions are manipulated in our experiment: the inclusion of a whistleblowing stage in which, after observing others' declared incomes, subjects can signal other group members to the tax authority, and the provision of information about the content of the second phase before the tax evasion game is played. Our results show that whistleblowing is effective in both curbing tax evasion and improving the precision of tax auditing. Moreover, we detect no statistically significant spillover effects of whistleblowing on ingroup cooperation in the subsequent generalized gift exchange game, with this result being unaffected by the provision of information about the experimental task in the second phase. Finally, the provision of information does not significantly alter subjects' (tax and whistleblowing) choices in the tax evasion game: thus, knowledge about perspective ingroup cooperation did not alter attitude toward whistleblowing.

3.
Front Psychol ; 12: 675684, 2021.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34220644

ABSTRACT

Social scientists have devoted considerable research effort to investigate the determinants of the Partisan Gender Gap (PGG), whereby US women (men) tend to exhibit more liberal (conservative) political preferences over time. Results of a survey experiment run during the COVID-19 emergency and involving 3,086 US residents show that exposing subjects to alternative narratives on the causes of the pandemic increases the PGG: relative to a baseline treatment in which no narrative manipulation is implemented, exposing subjects to either the Lab narrative (claiming that COVID-19 was caused by a lab accident in Wuhan) or the Nature narrative (according to which COVID-19 originated in the wildlife) makes women more liberal. The polarization effect documented in our experiment is magnified by the political orientation of participants' state of residence: the largest PGG effect is between men residing in Republican-leaning states and women living in Democratic-leaning states. JEL Classification: J16, D83, C83, C99, P16, D72.

4.
Front Psychol ; 12: 643758, 2021.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34305713

ABSTRACT

COVID-19 continues to spread across the globe at an exponential speed, infecting millions and overwhelming even the most prepared healthcare systems. Concerns are looming that the healthcare systems in low- and middle-income countries (LMICs) are mostly unprepared to combat the virus because of limited resources. The problems in LMICs are exacerbated by the fact that citizens in these countries generally exhibit low trust in the healthcare system because of its low quality, which could trigger a number of uncooperative behaviors. In this paper, we focus on one such behavior and investigate the relationship between trust in the healthcare system and the probability of potential treatment-seeking behavior upon the appearance of the first symptoms of COVID-19. First, we provide motivating evidence from a unique national online survey administered in Armenia-a post-Soviet LMIC country. We then present results from a large-scale survey experiment in Armenia that provides causal evidence supporting the investigated relationship. Our main finding is that a more trustworthy healthcare system enhances the probability of potential treatment-seeking behavior when observing the initial symptoms.

5.
Soc Sci Med ; 273: 113739, 2021 03.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33609965

ABSTRACT

Roughly 90 percent of cervical cancer deaths occur in low- and middle-income countries (LMICs), where the lack of adequate infrastructures hampers screening, while informational, cultural, and socio-economic barriers limit participation in the few programs that do exist. We conducted a field experiment with the Armenian cervical cancer screening program to determine whether, despite these barriers, the simple, economical invitation strategies adopted in high-income countries could enhance screening take-up in LMICs. We find that letters of invitation increase screening take-up, especially when there are follow-up reminders. Different ways of framing messages appear to have no impact. Finally, women in rural areas are more likely to respond to invitation by letter, helping to narrow the urban-rural screening gap.


Subject(s)
Uterine Cervical Neoplasms , Armenia/epidemiology , Developing Countries , Early Detection of Cancer , Female , Humans , Mass Screening , Uterine Cervical Neoplasms/diagnosis
6.
Heliyon ; 6(3): e03576, 2020 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32258457

ABSTRACT

We report results of a survey experiment aimed at testing whether eliciting taxpayer preferences on how to allocate the collected taxes over national public goods as well as providing information about the composition of the public expenditure influence the tax rate that taxpayers consider adequate to pay. We find that information exerts no effects on the level of the adequate tax rate. However, taxpayers are willing to accept a higher tax burden when they express their preferences on how to use tax revenues to finance public goods and services.

7.
PLoS One ; 13(3): e0192941, 2018.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29513685

ABSTRACT

AIMS: Social scientists have postulated that the discrepancy between achievements and expectations affects individuals' subjective well-being. Still, little has been done to qualify and quantify such a psychological effect. Our empirical analysis assesses the consequences of positive and negative affective forecasting errors-the difference between realized and expected subjective well-being-on the subsequent level of subjective well-being. DATA: We use longitudinal data on a representative sample of 13,431 individuals from the German Socio-Economic Panel. In our sample, 52% of individuals are females, average age is 43 years, average years of education is 11.4 and 27% of our sample lives in East Germany. Subjective well-being (measured by self-reported life satisfaction) is assessed on a 0-10 discrete scale and its sample average is equal to 6.75 points. METHODS: We develop a simple theoretical framework to assess the consequences of positive and negative affective forecasting errors-the difference between realized and expected subjective well-being-on the subsequent level of subjective well-being, properly accounting for the endogenous adjustment of expectations to positive and negative affective forecasting errors, and use it to derive testable predictions. Given the theoretical framework, we estimate two panel-data equations, the first depicting the association between positive and negative affective forecasting errors and the successive level of subjective well-being and the second describing the correlation between subjective well-being expectations for the future and hedonic failures and successes. Our models control for individual fixed effects and a large battery of time-varying demographic characteristics, health and socio-economic status. RESULTS AND CONCLUSIONS: While surpassing expectations is uncorrelated with subjective well-being, failing to match expectations is negatively associated with subsequent realizations of subjective well-being. Expectations are positively (negatively) correlated to positive (negative) forecasting errors. We speculate that in the first case the positive adjustment in expectations is strong enough to cancel out the potential positive effects on subjective well-being of beaten expectations, while in the second case it is not, and individuals persistently bear the negative emotional consequences of not achieving expectations.


Subject(s)
Achievement , Affect , Forecasting , Quality of Life , Adult , Algorithms , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Models, Theoretical , Personal Satisfaction
9.
PLoS One ; 10(4): e0121530, 2015.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25853520

ABSTRACT

We report results from an incentivized laboratory experiment undertaken with the purpose of providing controlled evidence on the causal effects of alcohol consumption on risk-taking, time preferences and altruism. Our design disentangles the pharmacological effects of alcohol intoxication from those mediated by expectations, as we compare the behavior of three groups of subjects: those who participated in an experiment with no reference to alcohol, those who were exposed to the possibility of consuming alcohol but were given a placebo and those who effectively consumed alcohol. All subjects participated in a series of economic tasks administered in the same sequence across treatments. After controlling for both the willingness to pay for an object and the potential misperception of probabilities as elicited in the experiment, we detect no effect of alcohol in depleting subjects' risk tolerance. However, we find that alcohol intoxication increases impatience and makes subjects less altruistic.


Subject(s)
Alcohol Drinking , Altruism , Economics , Risk-Taking , Adult , Blood Alcohol Content , Humans , Motivation , Optimism , Perception , Young Adult
10.
Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci ; 8(2): 165-70, 2013 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22133561

ABSTRACT

Humans are able to mentally adopt the spatial perspective of others and represent the visual world from their point of view. Here, we present neuropsychological evidence that information inaccessible from an egocentric perspective can be accessed from the perspective of another person. Patients affected by left neglect were asked to describe arrays of objects from their own egocentric perspective, from an opposite perspective (disembodied perspective taking), and from the point of view of another person actually seated in front of them (embodied perspective taking). Although disembodied perspective-taking ameliorated neglect severity, there was an even stronger positive effect of embodied perspective-taking: items presented on the left and neglected when reported from the egocentric perspective were instead recovered when patients assumed the perspective of the other. These findings suggest that perspective-taking entails an altercentric remapping of space, i.e. remapping of objects and locations coded with reference to the other person's body.


Subject(s)
Brain/physiopathology , Perceptual Disorders/physiopathology , Adult , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Brain/pathology , Female , Humans , Imagination/physiology , Male , Middle Aged , Neuropsychological Tests , Severity of Illness Index , Space Perception/physiology , Visual Perception/physiology
11.
Soc Indic Res ; 105(2): 293-308, 2012 Jan.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22207782

ABSTRACT

In this paper we investigate how age affects the self-reported level of life satisfaction among the elderly in Europe. By using a vignette approach, we find evidence that age influences life satisfaction through two counterbalancing channels. On the one hand, controlling for the effects of all other variables, the own perceived level of life satisfaction increases with age. On the other hand, given the same true level of life satisfaction, older respondents are more likely to rank themselves as "dissatisfied" with their life than younger individuals. Detrimental health conditions and physical limitations play a crucial role in explaining scale biases in the reporting style of older individuals.

12.
Neurosciences (Riyadh) ; 16(1): 60-5, 2011 Jan.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21206447

ABSTRACT

We describe a patient who presented with a pure topographical disorientation after a stroke involving the right mesial occipito-temporal cortex. He could not point to external unseen landmarks or draw a map of his city, while he could recognize landmarks, and judge the distance, and describe the route between pairs of landmarks of the same city. He underwent standardized cognitive tests, and 6 tasks were used to assess a topographical orientation route-survey. This study provides evidence that topographical disorientation can be subdivided into very specific components. The results suggest that one of these components might refer to the processing of an allocentric map separable from the representation of route knowledge.


Subject(s)
Brain Injuries/pathology , Brain Mapping , Occipital Lobe/pathology , Temporal Lobe/pathology , Aged , Brain Injuries/complications , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging/methods , Male , Memory Disorders/etiology , Neuropsychological Tests , Statistics as Topic , Tomography, X-Ray Computed/methods , Verbal Learning/physiology , Visual Perception/physiology
13.
J Neurol ; 257(8): 1356-61, 2010 Aug.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20352253

ABSTRACT

Alterations in temporal estimation have been observed in patients with Parkinson's disease (PD) and have been associated to dopaminergic dysfunction. Nevertheless, levodopa treatment and deep brain stimulation of the subthalamic nucleus (DBS-STN) have been shown to improve motor deficits and temporal estimation skills in such patients. So far, temporal estimation tasks in PD patients have evaluated the ability to reproduce intervals of time, but never the duration of an action. Here we investigated: (1) the ability of PD patients to reproduce the duration of their previous actions as compared to their ability to reproduce intervals of time and (2) the effect that DBS-STN has on both skills. Nineteen PD patients with DBS-STN and 19 controls were requested to reproduce the duration of an action and that of an interval of time. The patients were tested in the following treatment conditions: on stimulation/off medication, off stimulation/off medication and off stimulation/on medication. The results demonstrated that patients in the off stimulation/off medication condition under-reproduced the duration of their actions while accurately reproducing the duration of time intervals. The accuracy of the performance improved significantly in both treatment regimens. Our results indicate that in PD patients the ability to reproduce motor acts can be dissociated from that of reproducing time intervals and that it can be improved by the administration of medical or surgical treatment.


Subject(s)
Judgment/physiology , Movement Disorders/physiopathology , Parkinson Disease/physiopathology , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Time Perception/physiology , Aged , Deep Brain Stimulation/methods , Disability Evaluation , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Movement Disorders/psychology , Movement Disorders/therapy , Parkinson Disease/psychology , Parkinson Disease/therapy , Task Performance and Analysis
14.
Psychol Res ; 74(6): 545-59, 2010 Nov.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20174930

ABSTRACT

We investigated the characteristics of route and survey processing of a unique complex virtual environment both at the behavioral and brain levels. Prior to fMRI scanning, participants were trained to follow a route and to learn the spatial relationships between several places, acquiring both route and survey knowledge from a ground-level perspective. During scanning, snapshots of the environment were presented, and participants were required to either indicate the direction to take to follow the route (route task), or to locate unseen targets (survey task). Data suggest that route and survey processing are mainly supported by a common occipito-fronto-parieto-temporal neural network. Our results are consistent with those gathered in studies concerning the neural bases of route versus survey knowledge acquired either from different perspectives or in different environments. However, rather than arguing for a clear distinction between route and survey processing, "mixed" strategies are likely to be involved when both types of encoding take place in the same environment.


Subject(s)
Brain/physiology , Exploratory Behavior/physiology , Memory/physiology , Spatial Behavior/physiology , Adult , Brain Mapping , Humans , Image Processing, Computer-Assisted , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Male , Photic Stimulation , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Space Perception/physiology , User-Computer Interface
15.
J Cogn Neurosci ; 22(5): 888-902, 2010 May.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19320547

ABSTRACT

Following destruction or deafferentation of primary visual cortex (area V1, striate cortex), clinical blindness ensues, but residual visual functions may, nevertheless, persist without perceptual consciousness (a condition termed blindsight). The study of patients with such lesions thus offers a unique opportunity to investigate what visual capacities are mediated by the extrastriate pathways that bypass V1. Here we provide evidence for a crucial role of the collicular-extrastriate pathway in nonconscious visuomotor integration by showing that, in the absence of V1, the superior colliculus (SC) is essential to translate visual signals that cannot be consciously perceived into motor outputs. We found that a gray stimulus presented in the blind field of a patient with unilateral V1 loss, although not consciously seen, can influence his behavioral and pupillary responses to consciously perceived stimuli in the intact field (implicit bilateral summation). Notably, this effect was accompanied by selective activations in the SC and in occipito-temporal extrastriate areas. However, when instead of gray stimuli we presented purple stimuli, which predominantly draw on S-cones and are thus invisible to the SC, any evidence of implicit visuomotor integration disappeared and activations in the SC dropped significantly. The present findings show that the SC acts as an interface between sensory and motor processing in the human brain, thereby providing a contribution to visually guided behavior that may remain functionally and anatomically segregated from the geniculo-striate pathway and entirely outside conscious visual experience.


Subject(s)
Attention/physiology , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Superior Colliculi/physiology , Unconscious, Psychology , Visual Cortex/physiology , Blindness/pathology , Case-Control Studies , Color Perception/physiology , Functional Laterality/physiology , Hemianopsia/pathology , Hemianopsia/physiopathology , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Photic Stimulation/methods , Reaction Time/physiology , Superior Colliculi/physiopathology , Visual Cortex/physiopathology , Visual Fields/physiology , Visual Pathways/physiopathology
16.
Pain ; 146(3): 261-269, 2009 Dec.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19781854

ABSTRACT

In analgesic clinical trials, adverse events are reported for the painkiller under evaluation and compared with adverse events in the placebo group. Interestingly, patients who receive the placebo often report a high frequency of adverse events, but little is understood about the nature of these negative effects. In the present study, we compared the rates of adverse events reported in the placebo arms of clinical trials for three classes of anti-migraine drugs: NSAIDs, triptans and anticonvulsants. We identified 73 clinical trials in 69 studies describing adverse events in placebo groups: 8 were clinical trials with NSAIDs, 56 were trials with triptans, and 9 were trials with anticonvulsants. Studies were selected of all Medline/PubMed or CENTRAL referenced trials published until 2007. Adverse event profiles of the three classes were compared using a systematic review approach. We found that the rate of adverse events in the placebo arms of trials with anti-migraine drugs was high. In addition, and most interestingly, the adverse events in the placebo arms corresponded to those of the anti-migraine medication against which the placebo was compared. For example, anorexia and memory difficulties, which are typical adverse events of anticonvulsants, were present only in the placebo arm of these trials. These results suggest that the adverse events in placebo arms of clinical trials of anti-migraine medications depend on the adverse events of the active medication against which the placebo is compared. These findings are in accordance with the expectation theory of placebo and nocebo effects.


Subject(s)
Analgesics/adverse effects , Anti-Inflammatory Agents, Non-Steroidal/adverse effects , Anticonvulsants/adverse effects , Migraine Disorders/complications , Migraine Disorders/drug therapy , Serotonin Receptor Agonists/adverse effects , Tryptamines/adverse effects , Analgesics/therapeutic use , Anti-Inflammatory Agents, Non-Steroidal/therapeutic use , Anticonvulsants/therapeutic use , Data Interpretation, Statistical , Ethnicity , Humans , Randomized Controlled Trials as Topic , Serotonin Receptor Agonists/therapeutic use , Tryptamines/therapeutic use
18.
Brain Cogn ; 69(1): 81-8, 2009 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18619721

ABSTRACT

A right-neglect patient with focal left-hemisphere damage to the posterior superior parietal lobe was assessed for numerical knowledge and tested on the bisection of numerical intervals and visual lines. The semantic and verbal knowledge of numbers was preserved, whereas the performance in numerical tasks that strongly emphasize the visuo-spatial layout of numbers (e.g. number bisection) was impaired. The behavioral pattern of error in the two bisection tasks mirrored the one previously described in left-neglect patients. In other words, our patient misplaced the subjective midpoint (numerical or visual) to the left as function of the interval size. These data, paired with the patient's lesion site are strictly consistent with the tripartite organization of number-related processes in the parietal lobes as proposed by Dehaene and colleagues. According to these authors, the posterior superior parietal lobe on both hemispheres underpins the attentional orientation on the putative mental number line, the horizontal segment of the intraparietal sulcus is bilaterally related to the semantic of the numerical domain, whereas the left angular gyrus subserves the verbal knowledge of numbers. In summary, our results suggest that the processes involved in the navigation along the mental number line, which are related to the parietal mechanisms for spatial attention, and the processes involved in the semantic and verbal knowledge of numbers, are dissociable.


Subject(s)
Cognition/physiology , Mathematical Concepts , Parietal Lobe/physiopathology , Perceptual Disorders/physiopathology , Stroke/physiopathology , Aged , Cerebrum , Female , Humans , Imaging, Three-Dimensional , Linear Models , Models, Neurological , Neuropsychological Tests , Tomography, X-Ray Computed
19.
Memory ; 16(7): 678-88, 2008 Oct.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18720220

ABSTRACT

Spatial knowledge, necessary for efficient navigation, comprises route knowledge (memory of the landmarks along a route) and survey knowledge (map-like). Available data on the retention in humans of spatial knowledge show that this does not decline systematically over months or years. Here, two groups of participants elaborated route and survey knowledge during navigation in a complex virtual environment before performing route and survey tasks. Both groups were tested 5 minutes after learning and 3 months later, while one group was also tested 1 week and 1 month later (repeated testing). Performance was similar in both groups on the first testing session, remained stable in the repeated tested group, but decreased in the non-repeated tested group, especially on route tasks. These results are the first to reveal a substantial and selective decline of spatial knowledge, occurring only if there is no possibility of reactivating knowledge along repeated testing.


Subject(s)
Computer Simulation , Mental Recall/physiology , Retention, Psychology/physiology , Space Perception/physiology , Adult , Analysis of Variance , Attention , Female , Humans , Male , Time Factors
20.
Exp Brain Res ; 171(3): 389-404, 2006 May.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16374630

ABSTRACT

The present study used the redundant target paradigm on healthy subjects to investigate functional hemispheric asymmetries and interhemispheric cooperation in the perception of emotions from faces. In Experiment 1 participants responded to checkerboards presented either unilaterally to the left (LVF) or right visual half field (RVF), or simultaneously to both hemifields (BVF), while performing a pointing task for the control of eye movements. As previously reported (Miniussi et al. in J Cogn Neurosci 10:216-230, 1998), redundant stimulation led to shorter latencies for stimulus detection (bilateral gain or redundant target effect, RTE) that exceeded the limit for a probabilistic interpretation, thereby validating the pointing procedure and supporting interhemispheric cooperation. In Experiment 2 the same pointing procedure was used in a go/no-go task requiring subjects to respond when seeing a target emotional expression (happy or fearful, counterbalanced between blocks). Faster reaction times to unilateral LVF than RVF emotions, regardless of valence, indicate that the perception of positive and negative emotional faces is lateralized toward the right hemisphere. Simultaneous presentation of two congruent emotional faces, either happy or fearful, produced an RTE that cannot be explained by probability summation and suggests interhemispheric cooperation and neural summation. No such effect was present with BVF incongruent facial expressions. In Experiment 3 we studied whether the RTE for emotional faces depends on the physical identity between BVF stimuli, and we set a second BVF congruent condition in which there was only emotional but not physical or gender identity between stimuli (i.e. two different faces expressing the same emotion). The RTE and interhemispheric cooperation were present also in this second BVF congruent condition. This shows that emotional congruency is the sufficient condition for the RTE to take place in the intact brain and that the cerebral hemispheres can interact in spite of physical differences between stimuli.


Subject(s)
Cerebral Cortex/physiology , Emotions/physiology , Facial Expression , Functional Laterality/physiology , Social Perception , Adult , Fear/physiology , Female , Happiness , Humans , Male , Photic Stimulation , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Reaction Time/physiology
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...