Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 9 de 9
Filter
Add more filters










Database
Language
Publication year range
1.
J Opt Soc Am A Opt Image Sci Vis ; 18(9): 2237-54, 2001 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11551059

ABSTRACT

We examined the effect of changing the composition of the carrier on the perception of motion in a drifting contrast envelope. Human observers were required to discriminate the direction of motion of contrast modulations of an underlying carrier as a function of temporal frequency and scaled (carrier) contrast. The carriers were modulations of both color and luminance, defined within a cardinal color space. Random-noise carriers had either binary luminance profiles or flat (gray-scale-white) or 1/f (pink) spectral power functions. Independent variables investigated were the envelope spatial frequency and temporal-drift frequency and the fundamental spatial frequency, color, and temporal-update frequency of the carrier. The results show that observers were able to discriminate correctly the direction of envelope motion for binary-noise carriers at both high (16 Hz) and low (2 Hz) temporal-drift frequencies. Changing the carrier format from binary noise to a flat (gray-scale) or 1/f amplitude profile reduced discrimination performance slightly but only in the high-temporal-frequency condition. Manipulation of the fundamental frequency of the carrier elicited no change in performance at the low temporal frequencies but produced ambiguous or reversed motion at the higher temporal frequencies as soon as the fundamental frequency was higher than the envelope modulation frequency. We found that envelope motion detection was sensitive to the structure of the carrier.


Subject(s)
Motion Perception/physiology , Color , Contrast Sensitivity , Discrimination, Psychological , Humans , Light , Models, Psychological , Photic Stimulation/methods , Psychophysics/methods , Sensory Thresholds , Time Factors
2.
J Opt Soc Am A Opt Image Sci Vis ; 15(8): 1969-86, 1998 Aug.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9691482

ABSTRACT

The data presented in this paper examine the ability of observers to detect a modulation in the contrast of chromatic and luminance gratings as a function of the carrier contrast, duration, and spatial frequency. The nature of the signal underlying this ability is investigated by examining both the paradigm used to make the measurement and the effect of grating masks on performance in the tasks. The results show that observers' ability to discriminate amplitude modulation from an unmodulated carrier is dependent on carrier contrast but only up to approximately 5-8 times carrier-detection threshold. Discrimination is, however, independent of spatial frequency [10-1 cycles per degree (cpd) component-frequency range], carrier color, and, most surprisingly, stimulus duration (1000-30 ms). This set of experiments compliments data from previous papers and assimilates many of the conclusions drawn from this previous data. There is absolutely no evidence for the existence of a distortion product mediating performance under any of the current conditions, and the data seriously question whether the visual system might use such a signal even if it does exist under more extreme conditions than those used here. The evidence suggests that the visual system detects variations in both chromatic and luminance contrast by means of a mechanism operating locally upon the spatial structure of the carrier.


Subject(s)
Color Perception/physiology , Contrast Sensitivity/physiology , Light , Visual Pathways/physiology , Discrimination, Psychological/physiology , Humans , Models, Biological , Perceptual Masking , Photic Stimulation/methods , Sensory Thresholds/physiology , Time Factors
3.
Vision Res ; 37(16): 2247-59, 1997 Aug.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9578906

ABSTRACT

It has become apparent from recent work that the spatial frequency and orientation content of the first-order (luminance) carrier is very important in determining the properties of a second-order (contrast) modulation of that carrier. In light of this we examined whether there was any evidence for a motion aftereffect in one-dimensional second-order patterns containing only two sinusoidal luminance components: a spatial beat. The stimuli were either 1 cpd luminance sinusoids or 1 cpd luminance beats modulating a carrier sinusoid of 5 cpd. The magnitude of any motion aftereffect, or any directionally specific effect of adaptation, was measured for all combinations of first and second-order test and adapting patterns. Both flickering and non-flickering stimuli were used. The results indicate that a motion aftereffect is only induced by first-order adapting stimuli, and likewise, is only measurable in first-order test stimuli. We find no evidence for any directionally specific effect of adaptation in second-order stimuli, whether the test is counterphased or otherwise. These results apparently conflict with recent reports of a second-order induced motion aftereffect, but are consistent with many other findings which show differences between the detection of motion for first and second-order stimuli. We conclude that the induction of a motion aftereffect for second-order stimuli is not a general result and is critically dependent upon (amongst other things) the local properties of the stimulus, including the spatial frequency and orientation content of the first-order carrier.


Subject(s)
Adaptation, Ocular/physiology , Figural Aftereffect/physiology , Motion Perception/physiology , Contrast Sensitivity/physiology , Humans , Light
4.
Vision Res ; 37(9): 1157-65, 1997 May.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9196733

ABSTRACT

We have tested the independence of red-green chromatic and luminance mechanisms at detection threshold using a method of subthreshold summation. Stimuli were isoluminant red-green gratings and yellow-black luminance gratings that uniquely activate the red-green color and luminance mechanisms, respectively. Stimuli were Gaussian enveloped 0.25, 1 or 4 cpd sinewave gratings, counter-phase flickered at 0, 5 or 9 Hz. The threshold detection of red-green color contrast was measured in the presence of a subthreshold amount of luminance contrast, and vice versa. The results allow a model of linear summation between the color and luminance mechanisms to be rejected, but are well fitted by a model, assuming that these mechanisms are independent but combine to determine detection by probability summation, with a high summation index (median value = 4). We conclude that there are independent red-green chromatic mechanism and luminance detection mechanisms over this range of spatio-temporal conditions.


Subject(s)
Color Perception/physiology , Contrast Sensitivity/physiology , Female , Humans , Light , Male , Models, Neurological , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Psychophysics , Sensory Thresholds/physiology
5.
Vision Res ; 36(16): 2475-88, 1996 Aug.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8917809

ABSTRACT

It has been reported that equiluminant plaid patterns constructed from component gratings modulated along different axes of a cardinal colour space fail to create a coherent impression of two-dimensional motion [Krauskopf and Farell (1990). Nature, 348, 328-331]. In this paper we assess whether this lack of interaction between cardinal axes is a general finding or is instead dependent upon specific stimulus parameters. Type I and Type II plaids were made from sinusoidal components (1 cpd) each modulated along axes in a cardinal colour space and presented at equivalent perceived contrasts. The spatial angular difference between the two components was varied from 5 to 90 deg whilst keeping the Intersection of Constraints (I.O.C.) solution of the pattern constant. Observers were required to indicate the perceived direction of motion of the pattern in a single interval direction-identification task. We find that: (i) When plaids were made from components modulated along the same cardinal axis, coherent "pattern" motion was perceived at all angular differences. As the angular difference between the components decreased in a Type II plaid, the perceived direction of motion moved closer to the I.O.C. solution and away from that predicted by the vector sum. (ii) A plaid made from components modulated along red-green and blue-yellow cardinal axes (cross-cardinal axis) did not cohere at high angular differences (> 30 deg) but had a perceived direction of the fastest moving component. At lower angular differences, however, pattern motion was detected and approached the I.O.C. solution in much the same way as a same-cardinal axis Type II plaid. (iii) A plaid made from a luminance grating and a cardinal chromatic grating (red-green or blue-yellow) failed to cohere under all conditions, demonstrating that there is no interaction between luminance and chromatic cardinal axes. These results indicate that there are conditions under which red-green and blue-yellow cardinal components interact for the purposes of motion detection.


Subject(s)
Motion Perception/physiology , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Color Perception/physiology , Contrast Sensitivity , Female , Humans , Male , Mathematics , Models, Neurological , Psychophysics
6.
Nature ; 379(6560): 72-4, 1996 Jan 04.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8538743

ABSTRACT

The human visual system is much better at analysing the motion of luminance (black and white) patterns than it is at analysing the motion of colour patterns, especially if the pattern is presented very briefly or moves rapidly. We report here that observers reliably distinguish the direction of motion of a colour pattern presented for only 17 milliseconds, provided that the contrast is several times the threshold value (the contrast needed to detect the presence of the pattern). A control experiment, in which a static luminance 'mask' is added to the moving colour pattern, proves that discrimination of the direction of motion of these brief stimuli is colour-specific. The mask drastically impairs discrimination of the direction of motion of a luminance pattern, but it has little effect on a colour pattern. We conclude that the human visual system contains colour-specific motion-detection mechanisms that are capable of analysing very brief signals.


Subject(s)
Color Perception/physiology , Motion Perception/physiology , Eye Movements , Humans , Lighting , Neurons/physiology , Reaction Time
7.
Vision Res ; 34(19): 2609-12, 1994 Oct.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-7975299

ABSTRACT

Second-order Type I and Type II plaids were constructed by combining two random-dot gratings. Each component consisted of a dynamic random-dot field, the contrast of which was modulated by a drifting sinusoidal grating. Orienting the two components suitably and interleaving at 120 Hz allowed us to produce a two-dimensional plaid pattern made from one-dimensional second-order components. The perceived direction of motion of both Type I and Type II plaids was measured as a function of stimulus duration. Type I plaids had a perceived direction close to the intersection of constraints/vector sum solution (which only coincide for these patterns) at all durations. Type II plaids had a perceived direction that moved away from the vector sum and toward the intersection of constraints solution as the duration of presentation increased. These results are similar in form to those found for plaids made from first-order (luminance-defined) components [Yo & Wilson (1992), Vision Research, 32, 135-147]. This suggests that a delay which operates specifically on second-order signals cannot be the sole cause for the change in perceived direction of Type II plaids made from first-order components [Wilson, Ferrera & Yo (1992), Visual Neuroscience, 9, 79-97].


Subject(s)
Motion Perception/physiology , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Humans , Male , Optical Illusions/physiology , Rotation , Time Factors
8.
Vision Res ; 34(1): 41-8, 1994 Jan.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8116267

ABSTRACT

It is commonly assumed that the ability to discriminate velocity in a stimulus directly reflects the properties of the underlying directionally-selective mechanism. The results presented here show that this assumption is not always correct. Speed discrimination tasks over a range of base velocities were carried out for luminance gratings, chromatic gratings and contrast (beat) gratings of equivalent periodicity and contrasts. At low contrasts (0.5 log units above detection threshold), speed discrimination in luminance gratings was at least twice as good (when expressed as a Weber fraction), than in either chromatic gratings or beats. This is similar to the situation seen for tasks of direction discrimination using these stimuli [e.g. Cropper and Derrington (1990) Perception, 19, A31]. When the stimulus contrasts were increased to 1.5 log units above detection threshold, the ability to discriminate speed in both chromatic and beat stimuli improved to a performance level comparable to that shown for luminance gratings at all contrasts. This effect is not seen for tasks of direction discrimination when the same increase in stimulus contrast has little effect on the lower threshold of motion (LTM) measured for beat patterns. These results indicate that the ability to discriminate velocity in a stimulus does not necessarily directly reflect the characteristics of the ability to discriminate the direction of motion of that stimulus.


Subject(s)
Color Perception/physiology , Motion Perception/physiology , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Contrast Sensitivity/physiology , Differential Threshold/physiology , Discrimination, Psychological/physiology , Humans , Male , Sensory Thresholds/physiology , Time Factors
9.
Vision Res ; 34(1): 49-58, 1994 Jan.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8116268

ABSTRACT

This paper measures the minimum velocity required to discriminate the direction of motion (the lower threshold of motion--LTM) for patterns which consisted of spatial variations in luminance, chromaticity or luminance contrast in an attempt to distinguish between the underlying directionally-selective mechanisms. The characteristics of these patterns can be defined as first-order/Fourier stimuli (luminance and chromatic gratings) or second-order/non-Fourier stimuli (contrast gratings or "beats"). Measurements for each pattern were made at durations ranging from 0.015 to 0.96 sec and at contrasts of 0.5 log units above detection threshold and 1.5 log units above the threshold for detecting the stationary pattern. Observers were able to discriminate the direction of motion in luminance gratings and high contrast chromatic gratings at all durations above 0.015 sec. The direction of motion of beats and low contrast chromatic gratings was indiscriminable until they had been presented for at least 0.12 sec. This was taken to indicate the existence of a fast-acting and a slow-acting system dealing with the first- and second-order patterns respectively. When defined on this basis, the chromatic stimulus acts as a first-order (luminance) pattern at high contrasts and a second-order (beat) pattern at low contrasts.


Subject(s)
Color Perception/physiology , Motion Perception/physiology , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Contrast Sensitivity/physiology , Discrimination, Psychological/physiology , Humans , Male , Psychometrics , Sensory Thresholds/physiology , Time Factors
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...