Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 20 de 114
Filter
1.
Med Image Comput Comput Assist Interv ; 15(Pt 3): 607-14, 2012.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23286181

ABSTRACT

Analyzing geometry of sulcal curves on the human cortical surface requires a shape representation invariant to Euclidean motion. We present a novel shape representation that characterizes the shape of a curve in terms of a coordinate system based on the eigensystem of the anisotropic Helmholtz equation. This representation has many desirable properties: stability, uniqueness and invariance to scaling and isometric transformation. Under this representation, we can find a point-wise shape distance between curves as well as a bijective smooth point-to-point correspondence. When the curves are sampled irregularly, we also present a fast and accurate computational method for solving the eigensystem using a finite element formulation. This shape representation is used to find symmetries between corresponding sulcal shapes between cortical hemispheres. For this purpose, we automatically generate 26 sulcal curves for 24 subject brains and then compute their invariant shape representation. Left-right sulcal shape symmetry as measured by the shape representation's metric demonstrates the utility of the presented invariant representation for shape analysis of the cortical folding pattern.


Subject(s)
Algorithms , Image Interpretation, Computer-Assisted/methods , Imaging, Three-Dimensional/methods , Magnetic Resonance Imaging/methods , Pattern Recognition, Automated/methods , Anisotropy , Humans , Image Enhancement/methods , Reproducibility of Results , Sensitivity and Specificity
2.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 107(10): 4705-9, 2010 Mar 09.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20176936

ABSTRACT

General intelligence (g) captures the performance variance shared across cognitive tasks and correlates with real-world success. Yet it remains debated whether g reflects the combined performance of brain systems involved in these tasks or draws on specialized systems mediating their interactions. Here we investigated the neural substrates of g in 241 patients with focal brain damage using voxel-based lesion-symptom mapping. A hierarchical factor analysis across multiple cognitive tasks was used to derive a robust measure of g. Statistically significant associations were found between g and damage to a remarkably circumscribed albeit distributed network in frontal and parietal cortex, critically including white matter association tracts and frontopolar cortex. We suggest that general intelligence draws on connections between regions that integrate verbal, visuospatial, working memory, and executive processes.


Subject(s)
Brain/pathology , Brain/physiopathology , Cognition , Intelligence , Aged , Brain Diseases/pathology , Brain Diseases/physiopathology , Brain Mapping/methods , Female , Humans , Intelligence Tests , Male , Middle Aged , Psychomotor Performance , Young Adult
3.
Brain ; 131(Pt 5): 1311-22, 2008 May.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18390562

ABSTRACT

The ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC) and insular cortex are implicated in distributed neural circuitry that supports emotional decision-making. Previous studies of patients with vmPFC lesions have focused primarily on decision-making under uncertainty, when outcome probabilities are ambiguous (e.g. the Iowa Gambling Task). It remains unclear whether vmPFC is also necessary for decision-making under risk, when outcome probabilities are explicit. It is not known whether the effect of insular damage is analogous to the effect of vmPFC damage, or whether these regions contribute differentially to choice behaviour. Four groups of participants were compared on the Cambridge Gamble Task, a well-characterized measure of risky decision-making where outcome probabilities are presented explicitly, thus minimizing additional learning and working memory demands. Patients with focal, stable lesions to the vmPFC (n = 20) and the insular cortex (n = 13) were compared against healthy subjects (n = 41) and a group of lesion controls (n = 12) with damage predominantly affecting the dorsal and lateral frontal cortex. The vmPFC and insular cortex patients showed selective and distinctive disruptions of betting behaviour. VmPFC damage was associated with increased betting regardless of the odds of winning, consistent with a role of vmPFC in biasing healthy individuals towards conservative options under risk. In contrast, patients with insular cortex lesions failed to adjust their bets by the odds of winning, consistent with a role of the insular cortex in signalling the probability of aversive outcomes. The insular group attained a lower point score on the task and experienced more 'bankruptcies'. There were no group differences in probability judgement. These data confirm the necessary role of the vmPFC and insular regions in decision-making under risk. Poor decision-making in clinical populations can arise via multiple routes, with functionally dissociable effects of vmPFC and insular cortex damage.


Subject(s)
Brain Damage, Chronic/psychology , Cerebral Cortex/physiopathology , Decision Making , Gambling , Adult , Aged , Brain Damage, Chronic/pathology , Brain Mapping/methods , Cerebral Cortex/pathology , Choice Behavior , Female , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging/methods , Male , Middle Aged , Neural Pathways/pathology , Neural Pathways/physiopathology , Neuropsychological Tests , Prefrontal Cortex/pathology , Prefrontal Cortex/physiopathology
4.
J Clin Exp Neuropsychol ; 29(1): 1-12, 2007 Jan.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17162717

ABSTRACT

Two experienced drivers who developed severe amnesia due to bilateral hippocampal lesions participated in a series of standardized challenges of driving performance and knowledge of driving rules. During drives in a high fidelity simulator and on the road in an instrumented vehicle, they demonstrated vehicle control similar to that of normal drivers on measures of steering, speed control, safety errors, and driving with distraction. Their knowledge of driving rules, safety procedures, and road sign meaning also was normal. However, both participants were impaired at following route directions, and both had unsafe responses in a difficult crash avoidance scenario on the simulator. These findings suggest that memory impairment acquired by experienced drivers does not impair most aspects of driving performance, but may increase safety risk under some challenging circumstances.


Subject(s)
Amnesia/physiopathology , Attention/physiology , Automobile Driving , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Vision, Ocular , Amnesia/pathology , Automobile Driver Examination , Computer Simulation , Female , Hippocampus/pathology , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging/methods , Male , Middle Aged , Neuropsychological Tests
5.
Trends Cogn Sci ; 9(4): 159-62; discussion 162-4, 2005 Apr.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15808493

ABSTRACT

A recent study by Maia and McClelland on participants' knowledge in the Iowa Gambling Task suggests a different interpretation for an experiment we reported in 1997. The authors use their results to question the evidence for the somatic marker hypothesis. Here we consider whether the authors' conclusions are justified.


Subject(s)
Awareness/physiology , Decision Making , Galvanic Skin Response/physiology , Gambling/psychology , Games, Experimental , Risk-Taking , Adaptation, Psychological , Biomarkers , Consciousness/physiology , Emotions/physiology , Humans , Models, Psychological , Prefrontal Cortex/physiology , Psychological Theory
6.
Cognition ; 92(1-2): 179-229, 2004.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15037130

ABSTRACT

Using both the lesion method and functional imaging (positron emission tomography) in large cohorts of subjects investigated with the same experimental tasks, we tested the following hypotheses: (A) that the retrieval of words which denote concrete entities belonging to distinct conceptual categories depends upon partially segregated regions in higher-order cortices of the left temporal lobe; and (B) that the retrieval of conceptual knowledge pertaining to the same concrete entities also depends on partially segregated regions; however, those regions will be different from those postulated in hypothesis A, and located predominantly in the right hemisphere (the second hypothesis tested only with the lesion method). The analyses provide support for hypothesis A in that several regions outside the classical Broca and Wernicke language areas are involved in name retrieval of concrete entities, and that there is a partial segregation in the temporal lobe with respect to the conceptual category to which the entities belong, and partial support for hypothesis B in that retrieval of conceptual knowledge is partially segregated from name retrieval in the lesion study. Those regions identified here are seen as parts of flexible, multi-component systems serving concept and word retrieval for concrete entities belonging to different conceptual categories. By comparing different approaches the article also addresses a number of method issues that have surfaced in recent studies in this field.


Subject(s)
Aphasia/diagnosis , Aphasia/physiopathology , Brain/physiopathology , Concept Formation/physiology , Vocabulary , Adult , Aphasia/diagnostic imaging , Brain/diagnostic imaging , Humans , Nerve Net/anatomy & histology , Nerve Net/diagnostic imaging , Nerve Net/physiology , Neuropsychological Tests , Random Allocation , Tomography, Emission-Computed
7.
Nat Neurosci ; 5(3): 272-6, 2002 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11850633

ABSTRACT

Some of the outstanding cognitive capabilities of humans are commonly attributed to a disproportionate enlargement of the human frontal lobe during evolution. This claim is based primarily on comparisons between the brains of humans and of other primates, to the exclusion of most great apes. We compared the relative size of the frontal cortices in living specimens of several primate species, including all extant hominoids, using magnetic resonance imaging. Human frontal cortices were not disproportionately large in comparison to those of the great apes. We suggest that the special cognitive abilities attributed to a frontal advantage may be due to differences in individual cortical areas and to a richer interconnectivity, none of which required an increase in the overall relative size of the frontal lobe during hominid evolution.


Subject(s)
Frontal Lobe/anatomy & histology , Hominidae/anatomy & histology , Animals , Biological Evolution , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Statistics as Topic
8.
Neuropsychology ; 15(3): 396-404, 2001 Jul.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11499994

ABSTRACT

The anteromedial temporal lobe has been found to participate in processing emotion, but there are unresolved discrepancies in the literature. To address this issue, the authors investigated recognition of emotion from faces and from prosody in 26 participants with unilateral temporal lobectomy (15 left, 11 right) and in 50 brain-damaged controls. Participants with right, but not left, temporal lobectomy did significantly worse in recognizing fear from facial expressions. There were no group differences in recognizing emotional prosody. Neither IQ nor basic perceptual function accounted for task performance; however, there was a moderate negative correlation between extent of amygdala damage and overall performance. Consistent with some prior studies, these findings support a role for the right anteromedial temporal lobe (including amygdala) in recognizing emotion from faces but caution in drawing conclusions from small group samples.


Subject(s)
Affect , Epilepsy, Temporal Lobe/surgery , Face , Facial Expression , Linguistics , Perceptual Disorders/diagnosis , Perceptual Disorders/physiopathology , Psychosurgery/methods , Recognition, Psychology , Visual Perception/physiology , Adult , Female , Functional Laterality/physiology , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Postoperative Period , Temporal Lobe/physiopathology
9.
Brain ; 124(Pt 9): 1708-19, 2001 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11522574

ABSTRACT

Patients with pathological laughter and crying (PLC) are subject to relatively uncontrollable episodes of laughter, crying or both. The episodes occur either without an apparent triggering stimulus or following a stimulus that would not have led the subject to laugh or cry prior to the onset of the condition. PLC is a disorder of emotional expression rather than a primary disturbance of feelings, and is thus distinct from mood disorders in which laughter and crying are associated with feelings of happiness or sadness. The traditional and currently accepted view is that PLC is due to the damage of pathways that arise in the motor areas of the cerebral cortex and descend to the brainstem to inhibit a putative centre for laughter and crying. In that view, the lesions 'disinhibit' or 'release' the laughter and crying centre. The neuroanatomical findings in a recently studied patient with PLC, along with new knowledge on the neurobiology of emotion and feeling, gave us an opportunity to revisit the traditional view and propose an alternative. Here we suggest that the critical PLC lesions occur in the cerebro-ponto-cerebellar pathways and that, as a consequence, the cerebellar structures that automatically adjust the execution of laughter or crying to the cognitive and situational context of a potential stimulus, operate on the basis of incomplete information about that context, resulting in inadequate and even chaotic behaviour.


Subject(s)
Cerebellum/physiopathology , Crying/physiology , Laughter/physiology , Stroke/physiopathology , Cerebellum/cytology , Cognition/physiology , Expressed Emotion/physiology , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Male , Middle Aged , Motor Cortex/cytology , Neural Pathways , Pons/cytology , Stroke/diagnosis
10.
Hum Brain Mapp ; 13(4): 199-212, 2001 Aug.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11410949

ABSTRACT

Both lesion and functional imaging studies have implicated sectors of high-order association cortices of the left temporal lobe in the retrieval of words for objects belonging to varied conceptual categories. In particular, the cortices located in the left temporal pole have been associated with naming unique persons from faces. Because this neuroanatomical-behavioral association might be related to either the specificity of the task (retrieving a name at unique level) or to the possible preferential processing of faces by anterior temporal cortices, we performed a PET imaging experiment to test the hypothesis that the effect is related to the specificity of the word retrieval task. Normal subjects were asked to name at unique level entities from two conceptual categories: famous landmarks and famous faces. In support of the hypothesis, naming entities in both categories was associated with increases in activity in the left temporal pole. No main effect of category (faces vs. landmarks/buildings) or interaction of task and category was found in the left temporal pole. Retrieving names for unique persons and for names for unique landmarks activate the same brain region. These findings are consistent with the notion that activity in the left temporal pole is linked to the level of specificity of word retrieval rather than the conceptual class to which the stimulus belongs.


Subject(s)
Brain Mapping , Language , Names , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Recognition, Psychology/physiology , Temporal Lobe/physiology , Adult , Architecture , Classification , Face , Female , Geography , Humans , Male , Memory, Short-Term/physiology , Prosopagnosia/physiopathology , Radionuclide Imaging , Semantics , Temporal Lobe/diagnostic imaging
11.
Neuroimage ; 13(6 Pt 1): 1053-64, 2001 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11352611

ABSTRACT

Ina [(15)O] water PET experiment, 10 normal subjects retrieved words denoting actions (performed with or without an implement), and another 10 normal subjects retrieved words denoting the spatial relations between objects. Our objective was to test the following hypothesis: that the salient neural activity associated with naming actions and spatial relations occurs in left frontal operculum and left parietal association cortices, but not in the left inferotemporal cortices (IT) or in the right parietal association cortices. There were two control tasks, one requiring a decision on the orientation of unknown faces (a standard control task in our laboratory) and another requiring the retrieval of words denoting the concrete entities used in the action and spatial relations tasks. In accordance with the hypothesis, both naming actions and spatial relations (using the face orientation task as control activated the left frontal operculum; naming actions also activated the left parietal lobe. However, sectors of the left posterior IT were also engaged in both naming actions and spatial relations. When the naming of concrete entities was subtracted from the naming of actions performed with such entities, area MT in the posterior temporo-occipital region was activated bilaterally. On the other hand, when naming of the concrete entities was subtracted from the naming of spatial relations, left parietal activation was found, and when two tasks of naming spatial relations were contrasted to each other bilateral parietal activation was seen, right when abstract stimuli were used and left when concrete objects were used. The activity in posterior IT is thought to be related to object processing and possibly name retrieval at a subconscious level.


Subject(s)
Brain Mapping , Cerebral Cortex/physiology , Mental Recall/physiology , Orientation/physiology , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Tomography, Emission-Computed , Verbal Behavior/physiology , Verbal Learning/physiology , Adult , Cohort Studies , Dominance, Cerebral/physiology , Female , Humans , Male , Psycholinguistics , Reference Values , Semantics
12.
Nat Neurosci ; 4(1): 15-6, 2001 Jan.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11135639

ABSTRACT

Both lesion and functional imaging studies in humans, as well as neurophysiological studies in nonhuman primates, demonstrate the importance of the prefrontal cortex in representing the emotional value of sensory stimuli. Here we investigated single-neuron responses to emotional stimuli in an awake person with normal intellect. Recording from neurons within healthy tissue in ventral sites of the right prefrontal cortex, we found short-latency (120-160 ms) responses selective for aversive visual stimuli.


Subject(s)
Emotions/physiology , Neurons/physiology , Prefrontal Cortex/physiology , Adult , Arousal/physiology , Attention/physiology , Cognition/physiology , Electrodes, Implanted , Electroencephalography , Epilepsy/surgery , Humans , Male , Neuropsychological Tests , Photic Stimulation , Prefrontal Cortex/cytology , Prefrontal Cortex/surgery , Reaction Time/physiology
13.
Cogn Neuropsychol ; 18(7): 655-74, 2001 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20945232

ABSTRACT

Although much has been learned in recent years about the neural basis for retrieving words denoting concrete entities, the neural basis for retrieving words denoting actions remains poorly understood. We addressed this issue by testing two specific anatomical hypotheses. (1) Naming of actions depends not only on the classical implementation structures of the left frontal operculum, but also on mediational structures located in left premotor/prefrontal areas. (2) The neural systems subserving naming of actions and naming of concrete entities are segregated. The study used the lesion method and involved 75 subjects with focal, stable lesions in the left or right hemispheres, whose magnetic resonance data were analysed with a three-dimensional reconstruction method. The experimental tasks were standardised procedures for measuring action and object naming. The findings offered partial support for the hypotheses, in that: (1) lesions related to impaired action naming overlapped maximally in the left frontal operculum and in the underlying white matter and anterior insula; and (2) lesions of the left anterior temporal and inferotemporal regions, which produce impairments in naming of concrete entities, did not cause action naming deficits. A follow-up analysis indicated that action naming impairments, especially when they were disproportionate relative to concrete entity naming impairments, were not only associated with premotor/prefrontal lesions, but also with lesions of the left mesial occipital cortex and of the paraventricular white matter underneath the supramarginal and posterior temporal regions.

14.
Neuroimage ; 12(6): 640-56, 2000 Dec.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11112396

ABSTRACT

We describe and evaluate a practical, automated algorithm based on local statistical mixture modeling for segmenting single-channel, T1-weighted volumetric magnetic resonance images of the brain into gray matter, white matter, and cerebrospinal fluid. We employed a stereological sampling method to assess, prospectively, the performance of the method with respect to human experts on 10 normal T1-weighted brain scans acquired with a three-dimensional gradient echo pulse sequence. The overall kappa statistic for the concordance of the algorithm with the human experts was 0.806, while that among raters, excluding the algorithm, was 0.802. The algorithm had better agreement with the modal expert decision (kappa = 0.878). The algorithm could not be distinguished from the experts by this measure. We also validated the algorithm on a simulated MR scan of a digital brain phantom with known tissue composition. Global gray matter and white matter errors were 1% and <1%, respectively, and correlation coefficients with the underlying tissue model were 0.95 for gray matter, 0.98 for white matter, and 0.95 for cerebrospinal fluid. In both approaches to validation, we evaluated both local and global performance of the algorithm. Human experts generated slightly higher global gray matter proportion estimates on the test brain scans relative to the algorithm (3.7%) and on the simulated MR scan relative to the true tissue model (4.4%). The algorithm underestimated gray in some subcortical nuclei which contain admixed gray and white matter. We demonstrate the reliability of the method on individual 1 NEX data sets of the test subjects, and its insensitivity to the precise values of initial model parameters. The output of this algorithm is suitable for quantifying cerebral cortical tissue, using a commonly performed commercial pulse sequence.


Subject(s)
Brain/anatomy & histology , Image Interpretation, Computer-Assisted , Magnetic Resonance Imaging/methods , Algorithms , Artificial Intelligence , Cerebral Cortex/anatomy & histology , Cerebrospinal Fluid , Expert Systems , Humans , Imaging, Three-Dimensional , Myelin Sheath , Normal Distribution , Phantoms, Imaging , Reproducibility of Results
15.
Nat Neurosci ; 3(10): 1049-56, 2000 Oct.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11017179

ABSTRACT

In a series of [15O]PET experiments aimed at investigating the neural basis of emotion and feeling, 41 normal subjects recalled and re-experienced personal life episodes marked by sadness, happiness, anger or fear. We tested the hypothesis that the process of feeling emotions requires the participation of brain regions, such as the somatosensory cortices and the upper brainstem nuclei, that are involved in the mapping and/or regulation of internal organism states. Such areas were indeed engaged, underscoring the close relationship between emotion and homeostasis. The findings also lend support to the idea that the subjective process of feeling emotions is partly grounded in dynamic neural maps, which represent several aspects of the organism's continuously changing internal state.


Subject(s)
Brain Stem/diagnostic imaging , Brain Stem/physiology , Cerebral Cortex/diagnostic imaging , Cerebral Cortex/physiology , Emotions/physiology , Self Stimulation/physiology , Action Potentials/physiology , Adult , Anger/physiology , Fear/physiology , Happiness , Humans , Tomography, Emission-Computed
16.
Brain ; 123 ( Pt 11): 2189-202, 2000 Nov.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11050020

ABSTRACT

On a gambling task that models real-life decisions, patients with bilateral lesions of the ventromedial prefrontal cortex (VM) opt for choices that yield high immediate gains in spite of higher future losses. In this study, we addressed three possibilities that may account for this behaviour: (i) hypersensitivity to reward; (ii) insensitivity to punishment; and (iii) insensitivity to future consequences, such that behaviour is always guided by immediate prospects. For this purpose, we designed a variant of the original gambling task in which the advantageous decks yielded high immediate punishment but even higher future reward. The disadvantageous decks yielded low immediate punishment but even lower future reward. We measured the skin conductance responses (SCRs) of subjects after they had received a reward or punishment. Patients with VM lesions opted for the disadvantageous decks in both the original and variant versions of the gambling task. The SCRs of VM lesion patients after they had received a reward or punishment were not significantly different from those of controls. In a second experiment, we investigated whether increasing the delayed punishment in the disadvantageous decks of the original task or decreasing the delayed reward in the disadvantageous decks of the variant task would shift the behaviour of VM lesion patients towards an advantageous strategy. Both manipulations failed to shift the behaviour of VM lesion patients away from the disadvantageous decks. These results suggest that patients with VM lesions are insensitive to future consequences, positive or negative, and are primarily guided by immediate prospects. This 'myopia for the future' in VM lesion patients persists in the face of severe adverse consequences, i.e. rising future punishment or declining future reward.


Subject(s)
Brain Injuries/pathology , Brain Injuries/physiopathology , Cognition Disorders/pathology , Cognition Disorders/physiopathology , Decision Making/physiology , Prefrontal Cortex/pathology , Prefrontal Cortex/physiopathology , Adult , Aged , Behavior/physiology , Brain Injuries/psychology , Cognition Disorders/etiology , Female , Gambling/psychology , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Neuropsychological Tests , Prefrontal Cortex/injuries , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Punishment/psychology , Reward , User-Computer Interface
17.
Hum Brain Mapp ; 9(4): 192-211, 2000 Apr.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10770229

ABSTRACT

The study of subjects with acquired brain damage has been an invaluable tool for exploring human brain function, and the description of lesion locations within and across subjects is an important component of this method. Such descriptions usually involve the separation of lesioned from nonlesioned tissue (lesion segmentation) and the description of the lesion location in terms of a standard anatomical reference space (lesion warping). The objectives of this study were to determine the sources and magnitude of variability involved in lesion segmentation and warping using the MAP-3 approach. Each of two observers segmented the lesion volume in ten brain-damaged subjects twice, so as to permit pairwise comparisons of both intra- and interobserver agreement. The segmented volumes were then warped to a reference brain using both a manual (MAP-3) and an automated (AIR-3) technique. Observer agreement between segmented and warped volumes was analyzed using four measures: volume size, distance between the volume surfaces, percentage of nonoverlapping voxels, and percentage of highly discrepant voxels. The techniques for segmentation and warping produced high agreement within and between observers. For example, in most instances, the warped volume surfaces created by different observers were separated by less than 3 mm. The performance of the automated warping technique compared favorably to the manual technique in most subjects, although important exceptions were found. Overall, these results establish benchmark parameters for expert and automated lesion transfer, and indicate that a high degree of confidence can be placed in the detailed anatomical interpretation of focal brain damage based upon the MAP-3 technique.


Subject(s)
Brain Damage, Chronic/diagnosis , Brain Mapping/methods , Brain Ischemia/diagnosis , Frontal Lobe/pathology , Humans , Image Processing, Computer-Assisted , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Observer Variation , Reference Values
18.
J Neurosci ; 20(7): 2683-90, 2000 Apr 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10729349

ABSTRACT

Although lesion and functional imaging studies have broadly implicated the right hemisphere in the recognition of emotion, neither the underlying processes nor the precise anatomical correlates are well understood. We addressed these two issues in a quantitative study of 108 subjects with focal brain lesions, using three different tasks that assessed the recognition and naming of six basic emotions from facial expressions. Lesions were analyzed as a function of task performance by coregistration in a common brain space, and statistical analyses of their joint volumetric density revealed specific regions in which damage was significantly associated with impairment. We show that recognizing emotions from visually presented facial expressions requires right somatosensory-related cortices. The findings are consistent with the idea that we recognize another individual's emotional state by internally generating somatosensory representations that simulate how the other individual would feel when displaying a certain facial expression. Follow-up experiments revealed that conceptual knowledge and knowledge of the name of the emotion draw on neuroanatomically separable systems. Right somatosensory-related cortices thus constitute an additional critical component that functions together with structures such as the amygdala and right visual cortices in retrieving socially relevant information from faces.


Subject(s)
Brain Injuries/physiopathology , Brain Mapping , Somatosensory Cortex/physiology , Visual Perception/physiology , Adult , Discrimination, Psychological/physiology , Emotions , Face , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged
19.
Cereb Cortex ; 10(3): 295-307, 2000 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10731224

ABSTRACT

The somatic marker hypothesis provides a systems-level neuroanatomical and cognitive framework for decision making and the influence on it by emotion. The key idea of this hypothesis is that decision making is a process that is influenced by marker signals that arise in bioregulatory processes, including those that express themselves in emotions and feelings. This influence can occur at multiple levels of operation, some of which occur consciously and some of which occur non-consciously. Here we review studies that confirm various predictions from the hypothesis. The orbitofrontal cortex represents one critical structure in a neural system subserving decision making. Decision making is not mediated by the orbitofrontal cortex alone, but arises from large-scale systems that include other cortical and subcortical components. Such structures include the amygdala, the somatosensory/insular cortices and the peripheral nervous system. Here we focus only on the role of the orbitofrontal cortex in decision making and emotional processing, and the relationship between emotion, decision making and other cognitive functions of the frontal lobe, namely working memory.


Subject(s)
Cognition/physiology , Decision Making/physiology , Emotions/physiology , Frontal Lobe/physiology , Animals , Humans
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...