Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 20 de 129
Filter
1.
Genes Brain Behav ; 13(7): 686-701, 2014 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25065397

ABSTRACT

Reading and language abilities are heritable traits that are likely to share some genetic influences with each other. To identify pleiotropic genetic variants affecting these traits, we first performed a genome-wide association scan (GWAS) meta-analysis using three richly characterized datasets comprising individuals with histories of reading or language problems, and their siblings. GWAS was performed in a total of 1862 participants using the first principal component computed from several quantitative measures of reading- and language-related abilities, both before and after adjustment for performance IQ. We identified novel suggestive associations at the SNPs rs59197085 and rs5995177 (uncorrected P ≈ 10(-7) for each SNP), located respectively at the CCDC136/FLNC and RBFOX2 genes. Each of these SNPs then showed evidence for effects across multiple reading and language traits in univariate association testing against the individual traits. FLNC encodes a structural protein involved in cytoskeleton remodelling, while RBFOX2 is an important regulator of alternative splicing in neurons. The CCDC136/FLNC locus showed association with a comparable reading/language measure in an independent sample of 6434 participants from the general population, although involving distinct alleles of the associated SNP. Our datasets will form an important part of on-going international efforts to identify genes contributing to reading and language skills.


Subject(s)
Dyslexia/genetics , Genome, Human , Polymorphism, Single Nucleotide , Adolescent , Case-Control Studies , Child , Female , Genetic Pleiotropy , Genome-Wide Association Study , Humans , Language Tests , Male , Neoplasm Proteins/genetics , RNA Splicing Factors , RNA-Binding Proteins/genetics , Repressor Proteins/genetics
2.
Behav Genet ; 40(6): 751-8, 2010 Nov.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20333543

ABSTRACT

In order to test the hypothesis that the genetic etiology of reading disability differs as a function of IQ, composite reading performance data from 308 pairs of identical (monozygotic, MZ) twins and 440 pairs of fraternal (dizygotic, DZ) twins (254 same-sex and 186 opposite-sex) in which at least one member of each pair was classified as reading-disabled were subjected to multiple regression analysis (DeFries and Fulker, Behav Genet 15:467-473, 1985; Acta Genet Med Gemellol 37:205-216, 1988). In the total sample, heritability of the group deficit in reading performance (h(g)(2)) was .61 (±.06). However, results of fitting an extended regression model to reading performance and IQ data suggested that the genetic etiology of reading disability differs as a linear function of IQ (p ≤ .04). When the basic regression model was fitted separately to data from twin pairs with Wechsler (Examiner's manual: Wechsler intelligence scale for children-revised, 1974; Examiner's manual: Wechsler adult intelligence scale-revised, 1981) Full Scale IQ scores in the upper and lower 25% of the sample, resulting estimates of h(g)(2) were .75 (±.12) and .50 (±.10), respectively (p ≤ .045). These results suggest that reading difficulties in children with a higher IQ are due substantially to genetic influences and may require intensive remediation efforts.


Subject(s)
Diseases in Twins/genetics , Dyslexia/genetics , Intelligence/genetics , Adolescent , Child , Female , Humans , Male , Models, Genetic , Phenotype , Quantitative Trait Loci/genetics , Twins, Dizygotic/genetics , Twins, Monozygotic/genetics , Wechsler Scales , Young Adult
3.
Mol Psychiatry ; 15(11): 1112-20, 2010 Nov.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19488046

ABSTRACT

Although common sense suggests that environmental influences increasingly account for individual differences in behavior as experiences accumulate during the course of life, this hypothesis has not previously been tested, in part because of the large sample sizes needed for an adequately powered analysis. Here we show for general cognitive ability that, to the contrary, genetic influence increases with age. The heritability of general cognitive ability increases significantly and linearly from 41% in childhood (9 years) to 55% in adolescence (12 years) and to 66% in young adulthood (17 years) in a sample of 11 000 pairs of twins from four countries, a larger sample than all previous studies combined. In addition to its far-reaching implications for neuroscience and molecular genetics, this finding suggests new ways of thinking about the interface between nature and nurture during the school years. Why, despite life's 'slings and arrows of outrageous fortune', do genetically driven differences increasingly account for differences in general cognitive ability? We suggest that the answer lies with genotype-environment correlation: as children grow up, they increasingly select, modify and even create their own experiences in part based on their genetic propensities.


Subject(s)
Adolescent Development/physiology , Aging/genetics , Child Development/physiology , Cognition/physiology , Quantitative Trait, Heritable , Adolescent , Adult , Child , Child, Preschool , Female , Humans , Intelligence Tests , Male , Twins, Dizygotic/genetics , Twins, Monozygotic/genetics , United States
4.
Behav Genet ; 36(6): 820-32, 2006 Nov.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16710776

ABSTRACT

Whereas the majority of research on adolescent sexual initiation has focused solely on environmental factors, the present study used behavioral genetic analyses to investigate the relative contributions of genetic and environmental influences. Structural equation models were fitted to data from adoptive and non-adoptive sibling pairs (231 biologically related pairs and 169 unrelated pairs) from the Colorado Adoption Project. Information from censored individuals who had not yet experienced sexual initiation was maximized by adapting the twin survival analysis method of Pickles et al. (Behav Genet 24(5):457-468, 1994) to accommodate adoptive and non-adoptive siblings. Point estimates of variance components from an ACE model, including additive genetic (A), shared environmental (C), and non-shared environmental (E) influences were 28%, 24%, and 48%, respectively. Despite the lower point estimate for shared environmental effects than additive genetic effects, a CE model provided the best fit to the data. However, because adoptive siblings provide a direct estimate of shared environmental influences there is greater power to detect shared environmental effects in adoption designs. Evidence for genetic influences from our data were somewhat lower than those obtained in previous twin studies, possibly reflecting a return to more socially conservative sexual attitudes, changing sexual behaviors, or ambiguities in the wording of questions commonly used in research on adolescent sexuality.


Subject(s)
Adoption , Environment , Sexual Behavior/physiology , Adolescent , Adult , Age Factors , Child , Colorado , Female , Humans , Longitudinal Studies , Male , Reproducibility of Results , Siblings , Twins, Dizygotic , Twins, Monozygotic
5.
J Child Psychol Psychiatry ; 46(10): 1045-56, 2005 Oct.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16178928

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: There is a growing interest in the study of the genetic origins of comorbidity, a direct consequence of the recent findings of genetic loci that are seemingly linked to more than one disorder. There are several potential causes for these shared regions of linkage, but one possibility is that these loci may harbor genes with manifold effects. The established genetic correlation between reading disability (RD) and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) suggests that their comorbidity is due at least in part to genes that have an impact on several phenotypes, a phenomenon known as pleiotropy. METHODS: We employ a bivariate linkage test for selected samples that could help identify these pleiotropic loci. This linkage method was employed to carry out the first bivariate genome-wide analysis for RD and ADHD, in a selected sample of 182 sibling pairs. RESULTS: We found evidence for a novel locus at chromosome 14q32 (multipoint LOD=2.5; singlepoint LOD=3.9) with a pleiotropic effect on RD and ADHD. Another locus at 13q32, which had been implicated in previous univariate scans of RD and ADHD, seems to have a pleiotropic effect on both disorders. 20q11 is also suggested as a pleiotropic locus. Other loci previously implicated in RD or ADHD did not exhibit bivariate linkage. CONCLUSIONS: Some loci are suggested as having pleiotropic effects on RD and ADHD, while others might have unique effects. These results highlight the utility of this bivariate linkage method to study pleiotropy.


Subject(s)
Attention Deficit Disorder with Hyperactivity/epidemiology , Attention Deficit Disorder with Hyperactivity/genetics , Dyslexia/epidemiology , Dyslexia/genetics , Genetic Linkage/genetics , Adolescent , Child , Colorado/epidemiology , Comorbidity , Female , Humans , Male , Multivariate Analysis , Regression Analysis , Siblings
6.
J Child Psychol Psychiatry ; 43(6): 769-74, 2002 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-12236611

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: The study aimed to conduct the first analysis of CAP parent-offspring resemblance for reading performance in children aged 7, 12 and 16 years, and to assess the etiology of individual differences in reading performance of children at 16 years of age. METHOD: The Reading Recognition subtest of the Peabody Individual Achievement Test was administered to children in the Colorado Adoption Project (CAP) at 7, 12 and 16 years of age, and to their adoptive and nonadoptive parents when the children were 7 years of age. RESULTS: Resulting parent-offspring correlations in adoptive families were not significant at any age, but correlations between scores of nonadoptive control parents and their offspring were significant at all three ages. CONCLUSIONS: Results obtained from behavioral genetic model-fitting analyses of data from parents and their children tested at age 16 are consistent with results of studies of twins and siblings indicating that individual differences in reading performance are due substantially to genetic influences. In contrast, environmental transmission from parents to offspring was negligible, suggesting that environmental influences on individual differences in the reading performance of children are largely independent of parental reading performance.


Subject(s)
Adoption , Parent-Child Relations , Reading , Adolescent , Child , Female , Follow-Up Studies , Humans , Male
7.
Am J Hum Genet ; 70(5): 1287-98, 2002 May.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11951179

ABSTRACT

Reading disability (RD), or dyslexia, is a common heterogeneous syndrome with a large genetic component. Several studies have consistently found evidence for a quantitative-trait locus (QTL) within the 17 Mb (14.9 cM) that span D6S109 and D6S291 on chromosome 6p21.3-22. To characterize further linkage to the QTL, to define more accurately the location and the effect size, and to identify a peak of association, we performed Haseman-Elston and DeFries-Fulker linkage analyses, as well as transmission/disequilibrium, total-association, and variance-components analyses, on 11 quantitative reading and language phenotypes. One hundred four families with RD were genotyped with a new panel of 29 markers that spans 9 Mb of this region. Linkage results varied widely in degree of statistical significance for the different linkage tests, but multipoint analysis suggested a peak near D6S461. The average 6p QTL heritability for the 11 reading and language phenotypes was 0.27, with a maximum of 0.66 for orthographic choice. Consistent with the region of linkage described by these studies and others, there was a peak of transmission disequilibrium with a QTL centered at JA04 (chi2=9.48; empirical P=.0033; orthographic choice), and there was strong evidence for total association at this same marker (chi2=11.49; P=.0007; orthographic choice). Although the boundaries of the peak could not be precisely defined, the most likely location of the QTL is within a 4-Mb region surrounding JA04.


Subject(s)
Chromosome Mapping , Chromosomes, Human, Pair 6/genetics , Dyslexia/genetics , Adolescent , Alleles , Child , Diseases in Twins/genetics , Genetic Markers/genetics , Genotype , Humans , Linkage Disequilibrium/genetics , Quantitative Trait, Heritable , Tandem Repeat Sequences/genetics
8.
Behav Genet ; 31(4): 353-9, 2001 Jul.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11720121

ABSTRACT

The etiology of the longitudinal stability of reading performance was assessed by analyzing data from adoptive and nonadoptive sibling pairs (206 pairs at age 7, 195 pairs at age 12, and 110 pairs at age 16) tested in the Colorado Adoption Project (CAP). Results of longitudinal behavioral genetic analyses confirmed previous findings of moderate genetic influence on individual differences in reading performance at 7 and 12 years of age (a2 = .44 and .38, respectively), with somewhat higher heritability at age 16 (a2 = .57). Corresponding shared environmental influences were negligible (c2 = .07, .09, and .07). Moreover, common genetic influences were responsible for 66% of the observed stability (rp) between ages 7 and 12 (.62), 62% of that between ages 12 and 16 (rp = .74), and 88% of that between ages 7 and 16 (rp = .55). Of particular interest, no new heritable variation was detected at either 12 or 16 years of age, suggesting that genetic influences at 7 years of age are amplified at the later ages. In contrast, new nonshared environmental influences (including measurement error) were manifested at each age, suggesting the possible importance of nonshared environmental factors (e.g., instructional methods, teachers, peers) for the development of individual differences in reading performance between 7 and 16 years of age.


Subject(s)
Adoption , Environment , Genetics, Medical , Reading , Adolescent , Child , Humans , Likelihood Functions , Longitudinal Studies , Models, Genetic , Phenotype
9.
Genetics ; 158(3): 1217-26, 2001 Jul.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11454769

ABSTRACT

Bidirectional selection in rodents has been used to derive animal models of human behavior. An important question is whether selection for behavior operates on a limited number of QTL or whether the number and individual contribution of QTL varies between selection experiments. To address this question, we mapped QTL in two large F2 intercrosses (N = 815 and 821) from the four lines derived from a replicated selection experiment for open-field activity, an animal model for susceptibility to anxiety. Our analyses indicate that selection operated on the same relatively small number of loci in both crosses. Haplotype information and the direction of effect of each QTL allele were used to confirm that the QTL mapped in the two crosses lie in the same chromosomal regions, although we were unable to determine whether QTL in the two crosses represent the same genes. We conclude that the genetic architecture of the selected strains is similar and relatively simple.


Subject(s)
Quantitative Trait, Heritable , Animals , Haplotypes , Mice , Mice, Inbred BALB C , Mice, Inbred C57BL
10.
Behav Genet ; 31(6): 625-35, 2001 Nov.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11838539

ABSTRACT

Children with reading deficits perform more slowly than normally-achieving readers on speed of processing measures, such as rapid naming (RN). Although rapid naming is a well-established correlate of reading performance and both are heritable, few studies have attempted to assess the cause of their covariation. Measures of rapid naming (numbers, colors, objects, and letters subtests), phonological decoding, orthographic choice, and a composite variable (DISCR) derived from the reading recognition, reading comprehension, and spelling subtests of the Peabody Individual Achievement Test were obtained from a total of 550 twin pairs with a positive school history of reading problems. Basic DeFries and Fulker (DF) multiple regression models for the analysis of selected twin data confirmed the heritable nature of phonological decoding, orthographic choice, DISCR, and rapid-naming composites. Bivariate DF models were employed to examine the extent to which deficits in the three reading-related measures covary genetically with rapid naming. Significant bivariate heritability estimates for each of the reading measures with the numbers and letters rapid-naming composite were also obtained. As expected, univariate sib-pair linkage analyses indicated the presence of a quantitative trait locus (QTL) on chromosome 6p21.3 for phonological decoding and orthographic choice deficits. Bivariate linkage analyses were then conducted to test the hypothesis that this QTL for reading difficulties is pleiotropic for slower performance on RN tasks. The results obtained from these analyses did not provide substantial evidence that the 6p QTL for reading difficulties has significant effects on rapid naming; however, larger samples would be required to test this hypothesis more rigorously.


Subject(s)
Anomia/genetics , Diseases in Twins , Dyslexia/genetics , Reaction Time/genetics , Adolescent , Child , Chromosomes, Human, Pair 6 , Female , Genetic Markers/genetics , Humans , Intelligence/genetics , Male , Phenotype , Phonetics , Quantitative Trait, Heritable , Regression Analysis
11.
Dyslexia ; 7(3): 125-49, 2001.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11765981

ABSTRACT

The double-deficit hypothesis (Wolf, M. and Bowers, P.G. (1999) The double-deficit hypothesis for the developmental dyslexias. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 415-438) proposes that deficits in phonological processing and rapid automatized naming (RAN) are separable sources of reading dysfunction. Further, the double-deficit hypothesis predicts that the presence of deficits in both phonological processing and RAN have an additive negative influence on reading performance above and beyond that of a single deficit. The purpose of this study was to examine the additive nature of phonological awareness (PA)- and RAN-deficits on written language skill in children with reading disabilities (RD). Concurrent relationships between PA, RAN, and written language skills were examined in 476 children with RD, ranging in age from 8 to 18 years of age. Hierarchical regression analysis revealed that PA and RAN skill have an additive effect on a majority of the reading and spelling measures. When participants were classified into three deficit subtypes based on the double-deficit model (i.e. phonological-, rate-, and double-deficit), comparisons across the subtypes confirmed that individuals with double-deficits performed below the single-deficit groups on both subtyping variables (RAN and PA) and all measures of written language. When the double- and single-deficit groups were matched on the subtyping variable (i.e. double- and rate-deficit groups matched on RAN and double- and phonological-deficit groups matched on PA) differences between the double- and rate-deficit groups remained in non-word reading, whereas differences between the double- and phonological-deficit groups remained in timed word recognition and reading comprehension. These results support an additive model in which RAN-deficits primarily affect tasks that require speeded/fluent response, and PA-deficits primarily affect tasks that emphasize phonological processing skill. Results are also presented that illustrate several statistical problems associated with the formation of deficit groups by dichotomizing the RAN and PA variables.


Subject(s)
Awareness , Dyslexia/diagnosis , Language Disorders/diagnosis , Speech Perception/physiology , Child , Cross-Sectional Studies , Humans , Language Tests , Phonetics , Reaction Time
12.
Dev Neuropsychol ; 17(3): 339-60, 2000.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11056848

ABSTRACT

The heritable nature of reading disability has been well documented (DeFries & Alarcón, 1996), and possible abnormalities of brain structures have been associated with the disorder (Filipek, 1995). However, the etiology of individual differences in morphological brain measures has not been examined extensively. The purpose of this study was to apply behavioral genetic methods to assess the etiology of individual differences in neuroanatomical structures. Measures of reading performance, cognitive ability, and magnetic resonance imaging scans were obtained from 25 monozygotic (MZ) and 23 same-sex dizygotic (DZ) twin pairs with reading disability, and 9 MZ and 9 DZ control twin pairs participating in the Colorado Learning Disabilities Research Center. Results obtained from multiple regression analyses (DeFries & Fulker, 1985, 1988) of these twin data indicated that individual differences in the size of most cortical and subcortical structures were largely due to heritable influences. Moreover, estimates of heritability did not change appreciably after controlling for IQ and total brain size.


Subject(s)
Brain/abnormalities , Dyslexia/etiology , Adolescent , Child , Dyslexia/diagnosis , Female , Humans , Male , Twins
13.
Twin Res ; 3(2): 88-91, 2000 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10918621

ABSTRACT

Parents of 323 twin pairs with reading disability (RD) reported significantly more problems learning to read (16% of mothers and 33% of fathers) than parents of 309 twin pairs without reading difficulties (6% of mothers and 9% of fathers). These rates of self-reported reading problems in parents of twins are highly similar to those previously obtained in parents of non-twin children with RD and controls, suggesting that the etiology of reading deficits in twin and non-twin children may also be highly similar. Moreover, within both the RD and control samples, twins whose parents self-reported a positive history of reading problems had lower reading performance test scores, on average, than those whose parents reported no reading problems. Therefore, results of the present twin study support those of previous studies with non-twin children in which parental self-reports have been found to provide a valid index of family history status for reading difficulties.


Subject(s)
Diseases in Twins , Dyslexia/genetics , Parents , Twins , Achievement , Analysis of Variance , Discriminant Analysis , Fathers , Female , Humans , Income , Intelligence/physiology , Male , Mothers , Occupations , Parents/education , Reading , Self-Assessment , Sex Factors , Television
14.
Dev Psychol ; 36(4): 429-37, 2000 Jul.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10902695

ABSTRACT

The hypothesis that the association between parental divorce and children's adjustment is mediated by genetic factors was examined in the Colorado Adoption Project, a prospective longitudinal study of 398 adoptive and biological families. In biological families, children who experienced their parents' separation by the age of 12 years exhibited higher rates of behavioral problems and substance use, and lower levels of achievement and social adjustment, compared with children whose parents' marriages remained intact. Similarly, adopted children who experienced their (adoptive) parents' divorces exhibited elevated levels of behavioral problems and substance use compared with adoptees whose parents did not separate, but there were no differences on achievement and social competence. The findings for psychopathology are consistent with an environmentally mediated explanation for the association between parent divorce and children's adjustment; in contrast, the findings for achievement and social adjustment are consistent with a genetically mediated explanation involving passive genotype-environment correlation.


Subject(s)
Adaptation, Psychological , Adoption , Divorce , Genetic Predisposition to Disease , Self Concept , Social Adjustment , Stress, Psychological/genetics , Analysis of Variance , Child , Educational Status , Female , Humans , Male , Prospective Studies , Psychiatric Status Rating Scales , Risk Factors
15.
Am J Med Genet ; 96(3): 293-301, 2000 Jun 12.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10898903

ABSTRACT

This study utilized a sample of 313 eight- to sixteen-year-old same-sex twin pairs (183 monozygotic, 130 dizygotic) to assess the etiology of comorbidity between reading disability (RD) and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). RD was assessed by a discriminant function score based on the Peabody Individual Achievement Test, a standardized measure of academic achievement. The DSM-III version of the Diagnostic Interview for Children and Adolescents was used to assess symptoms of ADHD, and separate factor scores were computed for inattention and hyperactivity/impulsivity (hyp/imp). Individuals with RD were significantly more likely than individuals without RD to exhibit elevations on both symptom dimensions, but the difference was larger for inattention than hyp/imp. Behavior genetic analyses indicated that the bivariate heritability of RD and inattention was significant (h(2)(g(RD/Inatt)) = 0.39), whereas the bivariate heritability of RD and hyp/imp was minimal and nonsignificant (h(2)(g(RD/Hyp)) = 0.05). Approximately 95% of the phenotypic covariance between RD and symptoms of inattention was attributable to common genetic influences, whereas only 21% of the phenotypic overlap between RD and hyp/imp was due to the same genetic factors.


Subject(s)
Attention Deficit Disorder with Hyperactivity/complications , Diseases in Twins , Dyslexia/complications , Adolescent , Attention Deficit Disorder with Hyperactivity/genetics , Child , Comorbidity , Dyslexia/genetics , Female , Genotype , Humans , Male , Regression Analysis , Statistics as Topic , Twins, Dizygotic , Twins, Monozygotic
16.
J Abnorm Child Psychol ; 28(2): 149-59, 2000 Apr.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10834767

ABSTRACT

A community sample of 373 8 to 18 year-old twin pairs in which at least one twin in each pair exhibited a history of learning difficulties was utilized to examine the etiology of inattention and hyperactivity/impulsivity (hyp/imp). Symptoms of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) were assessed by the DSM-III Diagnostic Interview for Children and Adolescents. Inattention and hyp/imp composite scores were created based on results of a factor analysis. Results indicated that extreme ADHD scores were almost entirely attributable to genetic influences across several increasingly extreme diagnostic cutoff scores. Extreme inattention scores were also highly heritable whether or not the proband exhibited extreme hyp/imp. In contrast, the heritability of extreme hyp/imp increased as a linear function of the number of inattention symptoms exhibited by the proband. This finding suggests that extreme hyp/imp may be attributable to different etiological influences in individuals with and without extreme inattention. If this result can be replicated in other samples, it would provide evidence that the hyp/imp symptoms exhibited by individuals with Combined Type ADHD and Predominantly Hyp/Imp Type ADHD may be attributable to different etiological influences.


Subject(s)
Attention Deficit Disorder with Hyperactivity/genetics , Attention/physiology , Diseases in Twins/genetics , Impulsive Behavior/genetics , Achievement , Adolescent , Attention Deficit Disorder with Hyperactivity/complications , Child , Factor Analysis, Statistical , Female , Humans , Learning Disabilities/complications , Male , Regression Analysis , Risk Factors , Sex Factors
17.
J Cogn Neurosci ; 12(1): 223-32, 2000 Jan.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10769318

ABSTRACT

Although it is well known that there is considerable variation among individuals in the size of the human brain, the etiology of less extreme individual differences in brain size is largely unknown. We present here data from the first large twin sample (N=132 individuals) in which the size of brain structures has been measured. As part of an ongoing project examining the brain correlates of reading disability (RD), whole brain morphometric analyses of structural magnetic response image (MRI) scans were performed on a sample of adolescent twins. Specifically, there were 25 monozygotic (MZ) and 23 dizygotic (DZ) pairs in which at least one member of each pair had RD and 9 MZ and 9 DZ pairs in which neither member had RD. We first factor-analyzed volume data for 13 individual brain structures, comprising all of the neocortex and most of the subcortex. This analysis yielded two factors ("cortical" and "subcortical") that accounted for 64% of the variance. We next tested whether genetic and environmental influences on brain size variations varied for these two factors or by hemisphere. We computed intraclass correlations within MZ and DZ pairs in each sample for the cortical and subcortical factor scores, for left and right neocortex, and for the total cerebral volume. All five MZ correlations were substantial (r's=.78 to.98) and significant in both samples, as well as being larger than the corresponding DZ correlations, (r's=0.32 to 0.65) in both samples. The MZ-DZ difference was significant for 3 variables in the RD sample and for one variable in the smaller control sample. These results indicate significant genetic influences on these variables. The magnitude of genetic influence did not vary markedly either for the 2 factors or the 2 hemispheres. There was also a positive correlation between brain size and full-scale IQ, consistent with the results of earlier studies. The total cerebral volume was moderately correlated (r=.42, p<.01, two-tailed) with full-scale IQ in the RD sample; there was a similar trend in the smaller control sample (r=.31, p<.07, two-tailed). Corrections of similar magnitude were found between the subcortical factor and full-scale IQ, whereas the results for the cortical factor (r=.16 and.13) were smaller and not significant. In sum, these results provide evidence for the heritability of individual differences in brain size which do not vary markedly by hemisphere or for neocortex relative to subcortex. Since there are also correlations between brain size and full-scale IQ in this sample, it is possible that genetic influences on brain size partly contribute to individual differences in IQ.


Subject(s)
Dyslexia/pathology , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Neocortex/pathology , Adolescent , Adult , Data Interpretation, Statistical , Electroencephalography , Female , Functional Laterality , Humans , Male
18.
J Learn Disabil ; 33(2): 192-9, 2000.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15505948

ABSTRACT

To test the hypothesis that the genetic etiology of reading disability differs as a function of IQ, composite reading performance data from 223 pairs of identical twins and 169 pairs of same-gender fraternal twins in which at least one member of each pair was classified with reading disability were subjected to multiple regression analysis (DeFries & Fulker, 1985, 1988). In the total sample, heritability of the group deficit in reading performance (h(g)2) was .58 (+/- .08). However, when the basic regression model was fitted separately to data from twin pairs with average Wechsler (1974, 1981) full scale IQ scores below 100 or 100 and above, resulting estimates of h(g)2 were .43 and .72, respectively, a significant difference (p < or = .03, one-tailed). The results of fitting extended regression models to reading performance and continuous IQ data provide evidence that the genetic etiology of reading disability differs as a linear function of IQ (p < or = .007, one-tailed). These results suggest that IQ is relevant for the diagnosis of reading disability and that environmental influences may be more salient as a cause of reading difficulties in children with lower IQ scores.


Subject(s)
Dyslexia/etiology , Dyslexia/genetics , Intelligence/genetics , Adolescent , Adult , Child , Diagnosis, Differential , Dyslexia/diagnosis , Female , Humans , Inheritance Patterns , Intelligence Tests , Male , Regression Analysis
19.
Twin Res ; 2(3): 226-34, 1999 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10555134

ABSTRACT

The etiology of the observed relationship between reading and mathematics performance was examined by analyzing data from samples of same-sex twin pairs tested in the Colorado Learning Disabilities Research Center. Bivariate phenotypic and genetic structural equation models were fitted to data from 526 twin pairs selected for reading deficits (290 identical and 236 same-sex fraternal) and 355 control pairs (220 identical and 135 same-sex fraternal). Subtests of the Peabody Individual Achievement Test (PIAT; Reading Recognition, Reading Comprehension, and Spelling) were used as measures of reading performance, and scores from the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Revised (WISC-R) or Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised (WAIS-R) Arithmetic subtest, the Wide Range Achievement Test Arithmetic subtest, and the PIAT Math subtest were used as indices for mathematics performance. The results of these confirmatory factor analyses indicate that genetic and environmental covariances between reading and math latent factors do not differ significantly for twin pairs in the proband and control groups. Estimates of heritability for reading performance in the proband and control samples were 0.81 and 0.69, respectively, and those for math performance were 0.88 and 0.67, respectively. Moreover, genetic influences accounted for 83% of the covariation between the reading and math factors in the proband group and for 58% of the covariation between these two latent variables in the control group; in contrast, shared environmental influences did not contribute significantly to the relationship between the reading and math latent factors nor to their independent variation.


Subject(s)
Achievement , Learning Disabilities/genetics , Mathematics , Reading , Adolescent , Adult , Child , Humans , Phenotype
20.
Neurology ; 53(4): 723-9, 1999 Sep 11.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10489032

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVE: To test for brain structure differences in reading disability (RD) by means of MRI-based morphometry. BACKGROUND: Consensus is lacking on the brain structural correlates of RD. The current study reports on a wider set of structures in the largest sample yet studied, controlling for age, gender, IQ, and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). METHODS: A case-control study was performed that was comprised of 75 individuals with RD (mean age, 17.43+/-4.29 years) and 22 control subjects without RD (mean age, 18.69+/-3.75 years), each a single member of a twin pair. The two groups were similar in age, gender, and handedness, but differed in full-scale IQ (FSIQ), with the RD group having a lower mean FSIQ (101.8+/-9.9 versus 118.3+/-10.3). Using three group-by-structure analyses of covariance, groups were compared in terms of volume (in cubic centimeters) of major neocortical subdivisions, subcortical structures, and midsagittal areas (in square millimeters) of three subdivisions of the corpus callosum. RESULTS: Controlling for age, gender, and IQ, the authors found a significant group-by-structure interaction for the major neocortical subdivisions (p = 0.002), reflecting a different developmental pattern in the RD group, with the insula and anterior superior neocortex being smaller and the retrocallosal cortex being larger in the RD group. In contrast, they found no group main or interaction effects for the subcortical or callosal structures. The pattern of results was essentially the same in subjects without ADHD. CONCLUSIONS: Most brain structures do not differ in size in RD, but cortical development is altered subtly. This study replicates in a larger sample previous findings of insular differences in RD and demonstrates further that those differences are not attributable to comorbid ADHD.


Subject(s)
Brain/pathology , Diseases in Twins , Learning Disabilities/pathology , Reading , Adolescent , Adult , Female , Humans , Learning Disabilities/psychology , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Male , Neuropsychological Tests
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...