Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 20 de 267
Filter
1.
Sci Rep ; 9(1): 2243, 2019 02 19.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30783174

ABSTRACT

Self-initiated voluntary acts, such as pressing a button, are preceded by a surface-negative electrical brain potential, the Bereitschaftspotential (BP), that can be recorded over the human scalp using electroencephalography (EEG). While the BP's early component (BP1, generated in the supplementary and cingulate motor area) was linked to motivational, intentional and timing properties, the BP's late component (BP2, generated in the primary motor cortex) was found to be linked to motor execution and performance. Up to now, the BP required to initiate voluntary acts has only been recorded under well-controlled laboratory conditions, and it was unknown whether possible life-threatening decision making, e.g. required to jump into a 192-meter abyss, would impact this form of brain activity. Here we document for the first time pre-movement brain activity preceding 192-meter bungee jumping. We found that the BP's spatiotemporal dynamics reflected by BP1 and BP2 are comparable before 192-meter bungee jumping and jumping from 1-meter. These results, possible through recent advancements in wireless and portable EEG technology, suggest that possible life-threatening decision-making has no impact on the BP's spatiotemporal dynamics.


Subject(s)
Contingent Negative Variation/physiology , Electroencephalography , Gyrus Cinguli/physiology , Motor Cortex/physiology , Adult , Brain Mapping , Humans , Male
3.
J Neural Transm (Vienna) ; 114(3): 359-66, 2007 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16969626

ABSTRACT

In the present study gender differences related to the contingent negative variation (CNV) were investigated. A series of two acoustic stimuli was presented to participants across a wide age range. The first stimulus was consistent throughout the experiment whereas the second one was either a high frequency or a low frequency tone. One of them had to be answered by a button press (go condition) the other did not require any response (nogo condition). Between the first and the second tone there was a time period of two seconds in which the CNV appeared as a slow negative potential shift. Within this episode data were analysed with respect to gender differences. Statistical analysis revealed topographical differences between men and women in go conditions for both left and right index finger movements. Differences were found over frontal regions where women showed higher brain activity than men and over temporo-parietal regions where men produced higher brain activity than women. In order to explain the fact that only in "go" conditions significant gender differences occurred we introduce the phenomenon of implicit learning. Due to implicit learning assumed predictions related to S2 might have occurred from time to time. This is so, because a 50% chance for one of two different stimuli to occur leads to reasonable assumed predictions after two or more stimuli of a kind occurring in a series. The present data now provide evidence that if such assumed prediction or expectancy is directed towards an upcoming demand to act then brain activity is subject to gender differences. Further studies providing controlled sequences of "go" conditions versus "nogo" conditions have to be done to prove this idea true.


Subject(s)
Cerebral Cortex/physiology , Contingent Negative Variation/physiology , Decision Making/physiology , Electroencephalography/methods , Movement/physiology , Sex Characteristics , Acoustic Stimulation , Adult , Aged , Attention/physiology , Brain Mapping , Cerebral Cortex/anatomy & histology , Evoked Potentials/physiology , Evoked Potentials, Motor/physiology , Female , Fingers/innervation , Fingers/physiology , Functional Laterality/physiology , Humans , Learning/physiology , Male , Middle Aged , Neuropsychological Tests
4.
Int J Obes (Lond) ; 31(6): 987-95, 2007 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17160089

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVE: Olfacto-gustatory sensory-specific satiety plays an important role in the termination of food ingestion. A defect in this mechanism, by increasing food intake, could be a factor in development of overweight. The present study was conducted to explore whether sensory-specific satiety in the overweight may be different from that in normal-weight subjects. SUBJECTS: 144 subjects (half men, half women; age range: 17-62 years; BMI range: 17-39 kg m(-2)). MEASUREMENTS: Olfactory pleasure (OP) and flavor pleasure (FP) were evaluated before and after ingestion of a single chosen food. Six foods from three classes were offered: cucumber and tomato, pineapple and banana, and peanut and pistachio. According to the subjects' preference for one of them, subjects were classified into six groups (24 subjects each with equal sex ratio). The experimental sequence was (1) evaluation of the six foods (OP), (2) ad libitum intake of the preferred food (FP) and (3) second evaluation of the six foods (OP). RESULTS: Food intake was limited by sensory-specific satiety (that is, a decline in FP for the ingested food) in overweight subjects just as it was in the leanest. There was no significant correlation between BMI and hedonic parameters (OP and FP) or intakes (quantity and volume). Pre-ingestive OP and FP correlated with the ingested food's weight (OP: r=0.468; FP: r=0.415; P<0.01), volume (OP: r=0.428; FP: r=0.407; P<0.01) and intake duration (OP: r=0.184; FP: r=0.343; P<0.05). The decline in OP, but not in FP, correlated with ingested weight (r=0.271, P<0.01) and volume (r=0.263, P<0.01) but not with duration. CONCLUSION: After intake of a single food, olfacto-gustatory sensory-specific satiety correlated with the ingested food's weight and volume and with the duration of ingestion, but not with bodyweight. This suggests that overweight and lean subjects have similar hedonic control of food intake with simple foods.


Subject(s)
Overweight/physiology , Satiety Response/physiology , Sensation/physiology , Adolescent , Adult , Age Factors , Appetite Regulation/physiology , Body Mass Index , Eating/physiology , Female , Food Preferences/physiology , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Sex Factors , Smell/physiology , Taste/physiology
5.
J Clin Exp Neuropsychol ; 27(7): 859-66, 2005 Oct.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16183618

ABSTRACT

Percutaneous transluminal angioplasty with stenting (PTAS) has become a treatment option for severe carotid stenosis. The goal of our study was to determine prospectively neurocognitive outcome 6 months after unilateral stent-protected carotid angioplasty. Twenty consecutive patients who underwent stent-protected angioplasty for symptomatic (n=9) or asymptomatic (n=11) high-grade carotid stenosis were investigated and compared to an age and disease matched control group. Patients were administered preprocedurally and 6 months postprocedurally a battery of neuropsychological tests. We used reliable change indices methodology in order to control for practice and statistical effects unrelated to intervention. We found no cognitive change in approximately 90% of patients and cognitive improvement in approximately 10% of patients for concentration and attention variables. We further found no cognitive change in 61% of patients, cognitive improvement in 11% of patients and cognitive deterioration in 28% of patients for psychomotor speed. No cognitive change in 94% of patients and cognitive deterioration in 6% of patients was found for sustained attention; no cognitive change in 80% of patients, cognitive improvement in 15% of patients and cognitive deterioration in 5% of patients was found for verbal fluency; no cognitive change in 100% of patients was found for interference (Stroop test): no cognitive change in 95% of patients, cognitive improvement in 5% of patients was found for interference (c.I. test), respectively. Our study showed that 6 months after PTAS cognitive functioning did not change in most patients significantly. For some patients, however, significant improvement or deterioration in single neurocognitive domains can be expected. The reasons for these changes are unclear but may depend on variable type; magnitude of microemboli production; right vs. left cerebral vasculature, respectively.


Subject(s)
Angioplasty, Balloon/methods , Carotid Stenosis/therapy , Cognition/physiology , Neuropsychological Tests/statistics & numerical data , Aged , Carotid Stenosis/physiopathology , Case-Control Studies , Evaluation Studies as Topic , Female , Follow-Up Studies , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Retrospective Studies , Treatment Outcome
6.
Physiol Behav ; 86(1-2): 92-5, 2005 Sep 15.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16095639

ABSTRACT

The goal of this study was to investigate the impact of the essential oils of orange and lavender on anxiety, mood, alertness and calmness in dental patients. Two hundred patients between the ages of 18 and 77 years (half women, half men) were assigned to one of four independent groups. While waiting for dental procedures patients were either stimulated with ambient odor of orange or ambient odor of lavender. These conditions were compared to a music condition and a control condition (no odor, no music). Anxiety, mood, alertness and calmness were assessed while patients waited for dental treatment. Statistical analyses revealed that compared to control condition both ambient odors of orange and lavender reduced anxiety and improved mood in patients waiting for dental treatment. These findings support the previous opinion that odors are capable of altering emotional states and may indicate that the use of odors is helpful in reducing anxiety in dental patients.


Subject(s)
Affect , Anxiety/drug therapy , Citrus sinensis/chemistry , Dental Offices , Lavandula/chemistry , Odorants , Oils, Volatile/therapeutic use , Adult , Aged , Anxiety/psychology , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Surveys and Questionnaires
7.
Neuroimage ; 27(1): 83-94, 2005 Aug 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15927488

ABSTRACT

In our current study we employed whole-head magnetoencephalography (MEG) to identify neurophysiological correlates (event-related fields, ERFs) of different phenomenologies in human recognition memory. Words which had previously been semantically processed were presented along with previously unstudied words. Via button presses, participants provided subjective indices of three forms of memory: confident recognition, familiarity-based recognition, and misclassification of previously presented items as new (no recognition, misses). Behavioral results revealed a clear distinction between confident recognition (shortest reaction times) and familiarity-based recognition and misses, respectively, and physiological data pointed to bilateral anterior and left anterior/central regions in which magnetic field patterns were directly related to word recognition from approximately 300 ms to 500 ms after word onset. In the context of the prevalent dual process controversy on the roles of familiarity and recollection in recognition memory, we first highlight that two operationalizations of recollection need to be differentiated: We argue that a strategic search for a particular contextual feature stands in clear contrast to the fast and incidental availability of some contextual feature and derive experimental and behavioral indicators for either form of recollection. These indicators are used to select from manifold cognitive neuroscientific work on recognition memory in order to further discuss the neurocognitive characteristics of incidental recollection in contrast to other forms of episodic memory.


Subject(s)
Magnetoencephalography , Memory/physiology , Recognition, Psychology/physiology , Adult , Female , Humans , Male , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Reaction Time/physiology
8.
Neuroimage ; 25(2): 430-43, 2005 Apr 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15784422

ABSTRACT

In our current study, we applied whole head magnetoencephalography (MEG) in a subsequent memory paradigm. Magnetic fields were recorded while 20 healthy subjects (10 females, 10 males) incidentally encoded words during semantic and structural verbal processing tasks. Physiological data were then sorted according to the performance in subsequent memory tests and according to levels of processing, respectively, and analyzed for gender effects. Behavioral results show a clear advantage of semantic processing over structural processing with respect to retrieval success for both females and males. Despite alikeness of behavioral data, MEG results show considerable differences between males and females concerning both subsequent memory effects and levels of processing effects. For male subjects, we identified more distinct magnetic fields in anterior regions for subsequently remembered than for subsequently forgotten words (latency range from 300-650 ms after word onset) and for semantic processing than for structural processing, respectively. For female subjects, corresponding magnetic field differences pointed to posterior regions (subsequent memory effects from 450 ms to 750 ms after word onset). No qualitative differences were observed between subsequent memory effects during semantic processing compared to subsequent memory effects during structural processing. We try to reconcile results from male data with previous findings concerning subsequent memory effects by proposing the concept of width of processing, which holds that incidental memory formation is mediated by frontal activity on a physiological level and brought forward on a cognitive level by enhanced associating imposed by the task demands of semantic processing. Female data cannot be fully incorporated in this framework, but all the more prompt further gender-specific analyses of subsequent memory effects.


Subject(s)
Language , Magnetoencephalography , Memory/physiology , Adult , Female , Humans , Male , Sex Factors
9.
J Neural Transm (Vienna) ; 112(5): 713-30, 2005 May.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15480853

ABSTRACT

Effects related to depth of verbal information processing were investigated in probable Alzheimer's disease patients (AD) and age matched controls. During word encoding sessions 10 patients and 10 controls had either to decide whether the letter "s" appeared in visually presented words (alphabetical decision, shallow encoding), or whether the meaning of each presented word was animate or inanimate (lexical decision, deep encoding). These encoding sessions were followed by test sessions during which all previously encoded words were presented again together with the same number of new words. The task was then to discriminate between repeated and new words. Magnetic field changes related to brain activity were recorded with a whole cortex MEG.5 probable AD patients showed recognition performances above chance level related to both depths of information processing. Those patients and 5 age matched controls were then further analysed. Recognition performance was poorer in probable AD patients compared to controls for both levels of processing. However, in both groups deep encoding led to a higher recognition performance than shallow encoding. We therefore conclude that the performance reduction in the patient group was independent of depth of processing. Reaction times related to false alarms differed between patients and controls after deep encoding which perhaps could already be used for supporting an early diagnosis. The analysis of the physiological data revealed significant differences between correctly recognised repetitions and correctly classified new words (old/new-effect) in the control group which were missing in the patient group after deep encoding. The lack of such an effect in the patient group is interpreted as being due to the respective neuropathology related to probable AD. The present results demonstrate that magnetic field recordings represent a useful tool to physiologically distinguish between probable AD and age matched controls.


Subject(s)
Alzheimer Disease/physiopathology , Brain Mapping , Language , Magnetoencephalography , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Aged , Female , Humans , Male , Reaction Time
10.
Neuroimage ; 20(4): 2235-44, 2003 Dec.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-14683725

ABSTRACT

We recorded changes of brain activity from 10 MCI patients and 10 controls related to shallow (nonsemantic) and deep (semantic) word encoding using a whole-head MEG. During the following recognition tasks, all participants had to recognize the previously encoded words, which were presented again together with new words. In both groups recognition performance significantly varied as a function of depth of processing. No significant differences were found between the groups. Reaction times related to correctly classified new words (correct rejections) and incorrectly classified repetitions (misses) of MCI patients showed a strong tendency toward prolongation compared to controls, although no statistically significant differences occurred. Strikingly, in patients the neurophysiological data associated with nonsemantic and semantic word encoding differed significantly between 250 and 450 ms after stimulus onset mainly over left frontal and left temporal sensors. They showed higher electrophysiological activation during shallow encoding as compared to deep encoding. No such significant differences were found in controls. The present results might reflect a dysfunction with respect to shallow encoding of visually presented verbal information. It is interpreted that additional neural activation is needed to compensate for neurodegeneration. This finding is suggested to be an additional tool for MCI diagnosis.


Subject(s)
Cognition Disorders/physiopathology , Magnetoencephalography , Aged , Behavior/physiology , Cognition Disorders/diagnosis , Electroencephalography , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Neuropsychological Tests , Reaction Time/physiology , Reading
11.
J Neural Transm (Vienna) ; 110(5): 537-43, 2003 May.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-12721814

ABSTRACT

A male patient with bilateral thalamic lesions (medio-ventral nuclei) was investigated. Despite explicit memory impairments his lexical ability was normal. We recorded magnetic field changes (magnetoencephalography, MEG) during the performance of an animate/inanimate discrimination task in which some words where repeated after long delays. Normally, repeated items are classified significantly faster than their first presentations which is accomplished by an unconscious process called priming. The patient did not show any behavioural evidence of priming but the physiological data indicated preservation of this robust form of memory. Brain activation associated with repetitions was attenuated at early stages. The activity difference was posteriorly distributed which is consistent with previous reports about repetition priming. The present study indicated that the bilateral thalamic lesions of our patient disconnected the information processing stream between the primed information and the behavioural response.


Subject(s)
Amnesia/physiopathology , Memory , Thalamus/pathology , Amnesia/pathology , Diencephalon/pathology , Discrimination, Psychological , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Magnetoencephalography , Male , Middle Aged , Neuropsychological Tests , Photic Stimulation , Reading , Recognition, Psychology
12.
Neurology ; 58(8): 1277-9, 2002 Apr 23.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11971099

ABSTRACT

Levels of the potent pro-inflammatory prostaglandin E(2) (PGE(2)) are elevated in postmortem spinal cords from patients with ALS, and inhibition of a key PGE(2)-synthesizing enzyme, cylcooxygenase-2, is neuroprotective in an in vitro model of ALS. The authors report that 82% of the patients with ALS studied had 2 to 10 times higher PGE(2) levels in CSF compared with normal control subjects. That affected areas of the CNS are inflamed in ALS supports this. CSF PGE(2) measurement may be useful in monitoring treatment for ALS.


Subject(s)
Amyotrophic Lateral Sclerosis/cerebrospinal fluid , Dinoprostone/cerebrospinal fluid , Adult , Biomarkers , Female , Humans , Immunoassay , Luminescent Measurements , Male , Middle Aged
13.
Neuroimage ; 15(2): 373-85, 2002 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11798272

ABSTRACT

Studies of functional brain imaging in humans and single cell recordings in monkeys have generally shown preferential involvement of the medially located supplementary motor area (SMA) in self-initiated movement and the lateral premotor cortex in externally cued movement. Studies of event-related cortical potentials recorded during movement preparation, however, generally show increased cortical activity prior to self-initiated movements but little activity at early stages prior to movements that are externally cued at unpredictable times. In this study, the spatial location and relative timing of activation for self-initiated and externally triggered movements were examined using rapid event-related functional MRI. Twelve healthy right-handed subjects were imaged while performing a brief finger sequence movement (three rapid alternating button presses: index-middle-index finger) made either in response to an unpredictably timed auditory cue (between 8 to 24 s after the previous movement) or at self-paced irregular intervals. Both movement conditions involved similar strong activation of medial motor areas including the pre-SMA, SMA proper, and rostral cingulate cortex, as well as activation within contralateral primary motor, superior parietal, and insula cortex. Activation within the basal ganglia was found for self-initiated movements only, while externally triggered movements involved additional bilateral activation of primary auditory cortex. Although the level of SMA and cingulate cortex activation did not differ significantly between movement conditions, the timing of the hemodynamic response within the pre-SMA was significantly earlier for self-initiated compared with externally triggered movements. This clearly reflects involvement of the pre-SMA in early processes associated with the preparation for voluntary movement.


Subject(s)
Brain/physiology , Evoked Potentials/physiology , Motor Activity/physiology , Adult , Cues , Functional Laterality , Hemodynamics , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging/methods , Mesencephalon/physiology , Somatosensory Cortex/physiology
14.
Neurosci Lett ; 313(1-2): 5-8, 2001 Nov 02.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11684326

ABSTRACT

This study provides the first investigation of supplementary motor area (SMA) and primary motor cortex (MI) activation with similar movements differing only in subjective difficulty of motor control. Brain activation with simple tapping of the right index finger (well trained during daily life and easy to perform) was compared with tapping of the little finger (less trained and difficult to perform) using functional magnetic resonance imaging at 3 Tesla. Due to optimised movement standardisation, extrinsic influences on activation levels such as movement complexity, amplitude and frequency were minimised. Fifth finger tapping significantly increased the number of activated SMA voxels by 450% whereas MI activation showed no significant difference between fingers. We conclude that with similar movements the degree of subjective difficulty specifically modifies SMA but not MI activation.


Subject(s)
Fingers/physiology , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Motor Cortex/physiology , Movement/physiology , Adult , Female , Humans , Male
15.
Exp Gerontol ; 36(10): 1761-71, 2001 Nov.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11672995

ABSTRACT

The antiviral drug amantadine, that is effective in idiopathic Parkinson's disease (PD), may affect the composition and function of peripheral blood lymphocytes. In an explorative study, we therefore compared lymphocyte subpopulations and IL-2 secreting T cell precursors frequencies (HTLp-frequencies) in 15 PD patients without amantadine and six patients on long-term treatment. Five patients were investigated before and three months after the start of treatment. Group comparisons for long-term amantadine treatment showed no differences in subpopulations of B-, T-, and NK cells, and HTLp-frequencies. However, three months after initiation of treatment we noted in all five patients an increase of CD3+CD4+ and decrease of CD3+CD8+ cells, associated with an increase of the CD3+CD4+/CD3+CD8+ ratio. These changes had no effect on the HTLp-frequencies. Thus, at least for a short period of time, amantadine improves the T cell mediated immune system in PD patients.


Subject(s)
Amantadine/therapeutic use , Antiparkinson Agents/therapeutic use , Interleukin-2/metabolism , Lymphocyte Subsets/pathology , Parkinson Disease/blood , Parkinson Disease/drug therapy , Stem Cells/pathology , T-Lymphocytes/metabolism , T-Lymphocytes/pathology , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Female , Humans , Lymphocyte Count , Male , Middle Aged , Parkinson Disease/pathology
17.
Hum Brain Mapp ; 14(2): 65-80, 2001 Oct.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11500991

ABSTRACT

For low rhythmic rates (1.0 to approximately 2.0 Hz), subjects are able to successfully coordinate finger flexion with an external metronome in either a syncopated (between the beats) or synchronized (on each beat) fashion. Beyond this rate, however, syncopation becomes unstable and subjects spontaneously switch to synchronization to maintain a 1:1 stimulus/response relationship. We used a whole-head magnetometer to investigate the spatiotemporal dynamics of neuromagnetic activity (MEG) associated with both coordinative patterns at eight different rates spanning the range 1.0-2.75 Hz. Timing changes in the event-related fields accompanied transitions from syncopation to synchronization and followed the placement of the motor response within each stimulus/response cycle. Decomposition of event-related fields into component auditory and motor brain responses revealed that the amplitude of the former decreased with increasing coordination rate whereas the motor contribution remained approximately constant across all rates. Such an interaction may contribute to changes in auditory-motor integration that cause syncopation to become unstable. Examination of event-related changes in high frequency bands revealed that MEG signal power in the beta band (15-30 Hz) was significantly lower during syncopated coordination in sensors covering the contralateral sensorimotor area suggesting a dependence of beta rhythm amplitude on task difficulty. Suppression of beta rhythms was also stronger during synchronization preceded by syncopation, e.g., after subjects had switched, when compared with a control condition in which subjects synchronized throughout the entire range of rates.


Subject(s)
Auditory Perception/physiology , Cerebral Cortex/physiology , Cortical Synchronization , Evoked Potentials/physiology , Motor Skills/physiology , Movement/physiology , Acoustic Stimulation , Adult , Auditory Cortex/anatomy & histology , Auditory Cortex/physiology , Beta Rhythm , Brain Mapping , Female , Functional Laterality/physiology , Humans , Magnetoencephalography , Male , Motor Cortex/anatomy & histology , Motor Cortex/physiology , Periodicity , Reaction Time/physiology , Time Factors , Time Perception/physiology
18.
Clin Neurophysiol ; 112(9): 1685-97, 2001 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11514252

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVE: To investigate how learning induced increases in stability on a syncopation task are manifest in the dynamics of cortical activity. METHOD: Magnetoencephalography was recorded from 143 sensors (CTF Systems, Inc). A pre-training procedure determined the critical frequency (F(c)) for each subject (n=4). Subjects either syncopated or synchronized to a metronome that increased in frequency from 1.2 to 3.0 Hz in 0.2 Hz steps. The F(c) was the point at which subjects spontaneously switched from syncopation to synchronization. Subjects then underwent 100 training trials (with feedback) at F(c). Following the learning phase the pre-training procedure was repeated. RESULTS: An increase in the F(c) occurred indicating that practice improved the stability of syncopation. The transition delay was also observed in the phase of the time-averaged signal in sensors over the contralateral sensorimotor area and in power analysis in the 8-12 Hz and 18-24 Hz frequency bands. Initially, reduced power was observed bilaterally during syncopation compared to synchronization. Following training, these differences were reduced or eliminated. CONCLUSION: Pre-training power differences can be explained by the greater difficulty of the syncopation task. The reduction in power differences following training suggests that at the cortical level, syncopation became more similar to synchronization possibly reflecting a decrease in task and/or attention demands.


Subject(s)
Brain/physiology , Learning/physiology , Magnetoencephalography , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Acoustic Stimulation/methods , Adult , Cortical Synchronization , Humans , Movement/physiology , Periodicity
19.
Brain Res Cogn Brain Res ; 12(1): 49-54, 2001 Aug.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11489608

ABSTRACT

Magnetic field recordings were made in order to describe brain processes during a word recognition experiment. We investigated 26 healthy young subjects (14 females) and focused on gender differences related to recognition performance and brain activity. From about 200 ms to 350 ms after word onset the event-related field (ERF) patterns differed significantly between women and men, although the mean recognition performances did not. Differences were due to different strengths of activation as well as due to the involvement of different neural structures as underlined with statistical analysis. We interpret that our physiological findings demonstrate that different mental strategies are used for correct word recognition in the brains of women and men as assessed with magnetoencephalography (MEG). Our data might be linked to previous findings about the hemispheric asymmetry in male subjects (left lateralized) compared to women in whom both hemispheres seem to be equally involved in word processing.


Subject(s)
Cognition/physiology , Magnetoencephalography , Reading , Adult , Brain Mapping , Female , Humans , Male , Sex Characteristics
20.
Brain Res Cogn Brain Res ; 12(1): 167-70, 2001 Aug.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11489620

ABSTRACT

We investigated the spatiotemporal cortical dynamics during the perception of object-motion and visually-induced self-motion perception in six normal subjects, using a 143-channel neuromagnetometer. Object-motion specific tasks evoked early transient activity over the right temporooccipital cortex, while self-motion perception, or vection, additionally was followed by sustained bilateral activity in the temporoparietal area. The specific signal distributions suggest to represent the different perceptual modes of object-motion and self-motion sensation.


Subject(s)
Cerebral Cortex/physiology , Motion Perception/physiology , Visual Perception/physiology , Adult , Head Movements/physiology , Humans , Magnetoencephalography , Male , Occipital Lobe/physiology , Temporal Lobe/physiology
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...