Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 20 de 91
Filter
1.
Neurocrit Care ; 2023 Dec 21.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38129710

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: Patients with spontaneous subarachnoid hemorrhage (SAH) frequently encounter cognitive dysfunction and mental health issues with negative effects on health-related quality of life (HR-QoL). Here, we aimed to describe the prevalence of cognitive deficits, mental health problems, and HR-QoL impairments 1 year after SAH. METHODS: In this prospective observational study, 177 patients with SAH admitted to our neurointensive care unit over a time span of ten years followed the invitation for an in-person 1-year follow-up, including a standardized neuropsychological test battery. Mental health issues (anxiety and depression) and HR-QoL were evaluated using questionnaires (Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale; 36-item Short Form questionnaire). Functional outcome was assessed with the modified Rankin Scale (mRS) score. RESULTS: Patients were 54 years of age (interquartile range 47-62 years) and presented with a median Hunt and Hess score of 2 (interquartile range 1-3) at admission. Most patients (93%) achieved good functional 1-year outcomes (mRS score 0-2). Seventy-one percent of patients had deficits in at least one cognitive domain, with memory deficits being the most prevalent (51%), followed by deficits in executive functions (36%), visuoconstruction (34%), and attention (21%). Even patients with perimesencephalic SAH (18%) or with full functional recovery (mRS score = 0, 46%) had a comparable prevalence of cognitive deficits (61% and 60%, respectively). Symptoms of depression and anxiety were reported by 16% and 33% of patients, respectively. HR-QoL was impaired in 37% (55 of 147). Patients with cognitive deficits (p = 0.001) or mental health issues (p < 0.001) more frequently reported impaired HR-QoL. CONCLUSIONS: Most patients with SAH have cognitive deficits and mental health issues 1 year after SAH. These deficits impair patients' quality of life.

2.
J Sleep Res ; 31(1): e13433, 2022 02.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34240501

ABSTRACT

Patients with restless legs syndrome (RLS) use various terms when describing their symptoms. Whether gender might influence this has not been investigated so far. The aim of this study was to evaluate possible gender differences in spontaneous descriptions of RLS symptoms. This prospective study, conducted in 100 consecutive German-speaking RLS patients, used a single standardized question. Answers were digitally recorded and transcribed. A content-related linguistic analysis of the transcripts was performed by two independent blinded raters. The lengths of the answers and content-related linguistic features were compared between women and men. Ninety-eight patients were included in the final analysis, 59 women (60.2%) and 39 men (39.8%), with a median age of 62 (23-94) and 63 (31-82) years, respectively (p = 0.602). Demographic and clinical features, including educational level and RLS treatment class, did not differ between genders (p > 0.05). Total word or sentence count showed no gender differences (p = 0.159 and 0.259, respectively), although men used more words per sentence than women (p = 0.018). More men than women described quiescegenic (i.e., triggered by rest or inactivity) symptoms (p = 0.006) and successful attempts at relief (p = 0.039). There was a non-significant trend toward a more frequent use of the first-person perspective in men (median times used = 5 [0-10.5] vs. 3.8 [0-17.5], p = 0.068). The more frequent mention of quiescegenic symptoms and successful attempts at relief in men could indicate differences in phenotypic presentation of RLS between genders, a more precise description of RLS symptoms or a higher experience of self-efficacy in men compared to women.


Subject(s)
Restless Legs Syndrome , Female , Humans , Language , Male , Prospective Studies , Sex Factors
3.
Brain Sci ; 11(9)2021 Sep 17.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34573250

ABSTRACT

Media news during the Coronavirus Disease 2019 (COVID-19) pandemic often entail complex numerical concepts such as exponential increase or reproduction number. This study investigated whether people have difficulties in understanding such information and whether these difficulties are related to numerical competence, reflective thinking, and risk proneness. One hundred sixty-three participants provided answers to a numeracy scale focusing on complex numerical concepts relevant to COVID-19 (COV Numeracy Scale). They also provided responses to well-established objective and subjective scales, questions about affective states, and questions about the COVID-19 pandemic. Higher scores on the COV Numeracy Scale correlated with higher scores on the Health Numeracy Scale, in the Cognitive Reflection Test (CRT), and in self-assessments of verbal comprehension, mathematical intelligence, and subjective numeracy. Interestingly, scores on the COV Numeracy Scale also positively correlated with the number of consulted information sources about COVID-19. Accuracy in the CRT emerged as a significant predictor, explaining ca. 14% of variance on the COV Numeracy Scale. The results suggest that people with lower reflective thinking skills and lower subjective and objective numerical competence can be more at disadvantage when confronted with COVID-related numerical information in everyday life. These findings advise caution in the communication of relevant public health information that entails complex numerical concepts.

4.
J Alzheimers Dis ; 82(2): 727-735, 2021.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34057089

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: Agraphia is a typical feature in the clinical course of Alzheimer's disease (AD). OBJECTIVE: Assess the differences between AD and normal aging as regards kinematographic features of handwriting and elucidate writing deficits in AD. METHODS: The study included 23 patients with AD (78.09 years/SD = 7.12; MMSE 21.39/SD = 3.61) and 34 healthy controls (75.56 years/SD = 5.85; MMSE 29.06/SD = 0.78). Both groups performed alphabetical and non-alphabetical writing tasks. The kinematographic assessment included the average number of inversions per stroke (NIV; number of peaks in the velocity profile in a single up or down stroke), percentage of automated segments, frequency (average number of strokes per second), writing pressure, and writing velocity on paper. RESULTS: A total of 14 patients showed overt writing difficulties reflected by omissions or substitutions of letters. AD patients showed less automated movements (as measured by NIV), lower writing velocity, and lower frequency of up-and-down strokes in non-alphabetical as well as in alphabetical writing. In the patient group, Spearman correlation analysis between overt writing performance and NIV was significant. That means patients who had less errors in writing a sentence showed a higher automaticity in handwriting. The correctness of alphabetical writing and some kinematographic measures in writing non-alphabetical material reached excellent diagnostic values in ROC analyses. There was no difference in the application of pressure on the pen between patients and controls. CONCLUSION: Writing disorders are multi-componential in AD and not strictly limited to one processing level. The slow and poorly automated execution of motor programs is not bound to alphabetical material.


Subject(s)
Agraphia , Alzheimer Disease , Handwriting , Language Tests , Neuropsychological Tests , Aged , Agraphia/diagnosis , Agraphia/etiology , Agraphia/psychology , Alzheimer Disease/diagnosis , Alzheimer Disease/physiopathology , Alzheimer Disease/psychology , Automatism , Female , Humans , Male , ROC Curve , Task Performance and Analysis
5.
J Alzheimers Dis ; 80(4): 1491-1501, 2021.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33720899

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: Education has a protective effect toward cognitive decline in advanced age and is an important factor contributing to cognitive reserve. OBJECTIVE: To elucidate the interaction effect of education and global mental status on cognitive performance of older patients with progressive cognitive decline. METHODS: This retrospective study included 1,392 patients. We performed moderation regressions to examine the interaction between education and global mental status (Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) score) on performance in episodic memory, executive functions (EF), language, and constructional praxis tests. Significant interaction effects were further explored through separate linear regressions by MMSE level (inferior: ≤24; intermediate: 25-27; superior: 28-30). RESULTS: There was an interaction between MMSE and education for some but not all variables. At intermediate and superior MMSE levels, high-educated people had a clear advantage relative to low-educated people in verbal memory and EF tests. This advantage was not significant at an inferior MMSE level. In object naming, constructional praxis recall, and constructional praxis, high-educated people performed better than low-educated people, independently of MMSE level. CONCLUSION: Education has a differential effect on cognitive performance in patients with cognitive decline. While high education is not helpful for episodic memory and EF at low cognitive levels, it is still beneficial for retrieving words or other semantic knowledge. These findings suggest an interaction between global mental status and education on different cognitive domains and have strong clinical implications. Diagnostic judgments should be based on the knowledge of such interaction. This study highlights the beneficial but selective effects of high education.


Subject(s)
Cognitive Dysfunction/diagnosis , Educational Status , Executive Function , Language , Memory, Episodic , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Cognitive Dysfunction/psychology , Female , Humans , Linear Models , Male , Mental Status and Dementia Tests , Middle Aged , Multivariate Analysis , Retrospective Studies
6.
Brain Struct Funct ; 226(4): 1023-1030, 2021 May.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33555422

ABSTRACT

Both, decline of sensorimotor functions and cortical thickness are known processes in healthy aging. Physical activity has been suggested to enhance the execution of daily routine activities and to extend the time of functional independence in advanced age. We hypothesized that cortical thickness of motor areas in retired individuals could be related to physical demands of the profession carried out during working life. Depending on their former occupations, 69 cognitively healthy individuals (range 70-85 years) were divided into higher and lower physically complex occupations (HPCO n = 27 and LPCO n = 42) according to the international standard classification of occupations (ISCO-08). Participants underwent a high-resolution 3T T1-weighted MRI scan. Surface-based analysis revealed higher cortical thickness in the left precentral (P = 0.001) and postcentral gyrus (P < 0.001) and right postcentral gyrus (P = 0.001) for the HPCO relative to the LPCO group (corrected for multiple comparisons, sex, age and leisure activities in the past 20 years). Physical leisure activities associated with exertion were positively correlated with cortical thickness in the left pre- and postcentral gyrus (P = 0.037) of the LPCO group. Time since retirement was negatively associated with cortical thickness in the left postcentral gyrus (P = 0.004) of the HPCO group. Executing a higher physically complex occupation before retirement was related to relative higher cortical thickness in the primary motor and somatosensory cortex in later life, supporting the hypothesis that physical activity contributes to neural reserve in these regions. However, these benefits appear to vanish when physical activity is reduced due to retirement.


Subject(s)
Motor Cortex , Cognition , Exercise , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Motor Cortex/diagnostic imaging , Occupations
7.
Eur J Neurol ; 28(4): 1292-1298, 2021 04.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33296528

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: Previous studies reported reduced decision-making abilities for patients with multiple sclerosis (MS) relative to healthy controls (HC). This study aimed to evaluate whether these problems arise when sampling information or when pondering about the evidence collected. METHODS: In a cross-sectional, controlled study, 43 relapsing-remitting MS patients (RRMS; Expanded Disability Status Scale 1.5, range 0-4) and 53 HC performed an information sampling task ('beads task'), a health-related framing task, and neuropsychological background tests. RESULTS: In the beads task, patients collected as much information as HC prior to a decision. However, there were twice as many patients as HC making irrational decisions, that is, decisions against the evidence collected (RRMS: 26/43, 60%; HC: 16/53, 30%; p = 0.003). Compared to HC, patients also showed a stronger framing effect, that is, they were more strongly biased by the way health-related information was presented (p < 0.05, Cohen's d = 0.5). Overall, the framing effect predicted whether a participant would make irrational decisions (OR 2.12, 95% CI 1.29-3.49, p < 0.001). CONCLUSIONS: Predecisional information sampling is intact in RRMS. However, compared to HC, patients are more likely to make irrational decisions and to be biased by the way health-related information is framed. This warrants caution in communication, especially in the medical context, with patients.


Subject(s)
Multiple Sclerosis, Relapsing-Remitting , Multiple Sclerosis , Cross-Sectional Studies , Decision Making , Humans , Neuropsychological Tests
8.
Front Oncol ; 10: 591884, 2020.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33330076

ABSTRACT

To minimize recurrence following resection of a cerebral metastasis, whole-brain irradiation therapy (WBRT) has been established as the adjuvant standard of care. With prolonged overall survival in cancer patients, deleterious effects of WBRT gain relevance. Sector irradiation (SR) aims to spare uninvolved brain tissue by applying the irradiation to the resection cavity and the tumor bed. 40 were randomized to receive either WBRT (n = 18) or SR (n = 22) following resection of a singular brain metastasis. Local tumor control was satisfactory in both groups. Recurrence was observed earlier in the SR (median 3 months, 1-6) than in the WBRT cohort (median 8 months, 7-9) (HR, 0.63; 95% CI, 0.03-10.62). Seventeen patients experienced a distant intracranial recurrence. Most relapses (n = 15) occurred in the SR cohort, whereas only two patients in the WBRT group had new distant tumor manifestation (HR, 6.59; 95% CI, 1.71-11.49; p = 0.002). Median overall survival (OS) was 15.5 months (range: 1-61) with longer OS in the SR group (16 months, 1-61) than in the WBRT group (13 months, 3-52), without statistical significance (HR, 0.55; 95% CI, 0.69-3.64). Concerning neurocognition, patients in the SR group improved in the follow-up assessments, while this was not observed in the WBRT group. There were positive signals in terms of QOL within the SR group, but no significant differences in the global QLQ and QLQ-C30 summary scores were found. Our results indicate comparable efficacy of SR in terms of local control, with better maintenance of neurocognitive function. Unsurprisingly, more distant intracranial relapses occurred. Clinical Trial Registration: ClinicalTrials.gov, identifier NCT01667640.

9.
Gerontology ; 66(6): 582-592, 2020.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32980844

ABSTRACT

INTRODUCTION: Previous studies have shown an association between a high health numeracy and good cognitive functioning. OBJECTIVE: To investigate the moderation effect of education on this relationship and which brain structures support health numeracy. METHODS: We examined 70 healthy older persons (66% females; mean ± SD: age, 75.73 ± 4.52 years; education, 12.21 ± 2.94 years). The participants underwent a T1-weighted 3-T MRI and a neuropsychological assessment including a health numeracy task. Statistical parametric mapping was applied to identify focal changes in cortical thickness throughout the entire brain and to correlate image parameters with behavioral measures. RESULTS: Executive functions and mental calculation emerged as predictors of health numeracy (B = 0.22, p < 0.05, and B = 0.38, p < 0.01). An interaction was found between education and executive functions (B = -0.16, p = 0.01) and between education and mental calculation (B = -0.11, p < 0.05). Executive functions and mental calculation had an impact on health numeracy in participants with a low to intermediate edu-cation (≤12 years) but not in those with a higher education (>12 years). Health numeracy scores were associated with cortical thickness in the right dorsomedial prefrontal cortex and the right superior temporal gyrus (p = 0.01). CONCLUSIONS: Older people with a higher education perform better in health numeracy tasks than those with a lower education. They have access to previously acquired knowledge about ratio concepts and do not need to rely on executive functions and computational skills. This is highly relevant when decisions about health care have to be made.


Subject(s)
Aging/psychology , Cognition , Educational Status , Executive Function , Mathematics , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Decision Making , Female , Humans , Male , Neuropsychological Tests/statistics & numerical data
10.
Ann Clin Transl Neurol ; 7(9): 1620-1627, 2020 09.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32786065

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVE: To assess emotional processing and alexithymia in patients with restless legs syndrome (RLS) with augmentation versus those who never had augmentation. METHODS: We recruited 26 patients who had a history of augmentation (AUG), either current or past, 27 RLS patients treated with dopamine agonists who never had augmentation (RLS controls), and 21 healthy controls (HC). All participants were screened for impulse control disorders (ICDs). Alexithymia was assessed by means of the Toronto Alexithymia Scale - 20 (TAS-20). Facial emotion recognition was tested through an eye-tracking task. Furthermore, all participants performed neuropsychological tests assessing global cognitive status, impulsivity, anxiety, and depression. RESULTS: ICD symptoms occurred more frequently in AUG patients than in RLS controls (P = 0.047). Patients with AUG scored higher on the TAS-20 (P = 0.007) and the attentional subdomain of an impulsivity scale (BIS-11; P = 0.015) compared to HC. Patients with AUG also performed worse on the facial emotion recognition task relative to RLS controls (P = 0.009) and HC (P = 0.003). We found a group difference for the time to first fixation and the fixation count in the mouth region (P = 0.019 and P = 0.021, respectively). There were no other differences in the eye tracking examination. INTERPRETATION: This study showed evidence of poorer emotional processing in patients who had augmentation compared to RLS patients without augmentation and healthy controls. The altered exploration pattern of faces and the higher alexithymia scores suggest abnormalities in emotion processing in patients with augmentation.


Subject(s)
Affective Symptoms/physiopathology , Disruptive, Impulse Control, and Conduct Disorders/chemically induced , Disruptive, Impulse Control, and Conduct Disorders/physiopathology , Dopamine Agonists/adverse effects , Facial Recognition/physiology , Restless Legs Syndrome/chemically induced , Restless Legs Syndrome/physiopathology , Aged , Eye-Tracking Technology , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Restless Legs Syndrome/drug therapy
11.
Epilepsy Behav ; 110: 107138, 2020 09.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32464541

ABSTRACT

Trust is one of the foundations of human society and pervades all aspects of human live. Research on humans focused primarily on identifying the biological basis of trust behavior in healthy subjects, and this evidence hints to certain brain areas, hormones, and genetic factors to be fundamentally involved. The contribution of cortisol in trust has not yet elicited much attention in research, especially when specifically examined at basal cortisol levels. Trust has been previously studied in some neurological diseases but not in patients with epilepsy, and the influence of hormones on trust in these diseases remains yet unknown. Against this background, we designed an experimental study with a group of patients with juvenile myoclonic epilepsy and a group of healthy controls to compare trust behavior and plasma cortisol levels between the two groups. This economic game is frequently used in research to operationalize trust behavior. All participants further underwent neuropsychological assessment. Our results showed that there was no significant difference in trust behavior during the trust game, but a trend toward lower trust in patients. Furthermore, there was a significant difference in cortisol levels between groups with lower levels in patients. Interestingly, cortisol levels correlated with trust only in the patient group, but not in the control group. Future studies should specifically differentiate the effect of induced cortisol increases (e.g., acute stress) versus the effect of basal cortisol levels reflecting homeostasis or chronic stress on trust behavior and leverage the potential of comparison between patients and healthy controls.


Subject(s)
Hydrocortisone/blood , Myoclonic Epilepsy, Juvenile/blood , Myoclonic Epilepsy, Juvenile/psychology , Neuropsychological Tests , Trust/psychology , Adolescent , Adult , Biomarkers/blood , Female , Healthy Volunteers , Humans , Male , Myoclonic Epilepsy, Juvenile/diagnosis , Surveys and Questionnaires , Young Adult
12.
Neuropsychology ; 34(6): 667-674, 2020 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32352831

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVE: To assess the role of cognitive reserve, age, gender and brain structure in proper name retrieval in advanced age. METHOD: Performance in 2 naming tasks (asking for proper names or common names) and 2 memory tasks was assessed. In separate hierarchical regressions, we evaluated whether retrieval was predicted by gray matter thickness or volume in selected structures (Model 1) and whether the addition of age and gender (Model 2) or of education (Model 3) explained significantly more variance. Participants were healthy persons (ages 70-90 years). Out of 91 individuals, 18 were excluded after inspection of magnetic resonance imaging scans showing relevant white matter changes. The remaining 73 individuals (47 women) showed good cognitive abilities. RESULTS: Age was a significant predictor for the retrieval of well-known proper names, whereas selected gray matter measures and education had no significant effect. In contrast, education was predictive of common names retrieval and performance in the memory tasks. Gray matter measures predicted performance in the 2 memory tasks. CONCLUSIONS: Cognitive reserve has a differential effect on cognitive abilities in advances age. Education did not support the retrieval of well-known proper names but positively affected the retrieval of common names and performance in memory tasks. Cognitive reserve has to be considered in neuropsychological diagnostic procedures. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2020 APA, all rights reserved).


Subject(s)
Cognitive Reserve/physiology , Mental Recall/physiology , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Educational Status , Female , Gray Matter/diagnostic imaging , Gray Matter/physiology , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Male , Memory/physiology , Names , Neuropsychological Tests , Psychomotor Performance/physiology
13.
Ann Clin Transl Neurol ; 7(2): 219-228, 2020 02.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32031752

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVE: Cognitive impairment in multiple system atrophy (MSA) is common, but remain poorly characterized. We evaluated cognitive and behavioral features in MSA patients and assessed between-group differences for MSA subtypes and the effect of orthostatic hypotension (OH) on cognition. METHODS: This retrospective study included 54 patients with clinical diagnosis of possible and probable MSA referred to the Department of Neurology at Medical University of Innsbruck between 2000 and 2018. Neurological work-up included comprehensive neuropsychological testing including Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer's Disease (CERAD-plus) test battery, Frontal Assessment Battery (FAB), digit span test (DST), clock drawing task (CLOX1), and Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale (HADS-D). RESULTS: The mean MMSE score was 27.6 points. Overall, slight to moderate cognitive impairment was noted in up to 40% of patients, with predominant impairment of executive function and verbal memory. Patients with the cerebellar variant performed significantly worse than patients with the parkinsonian type (P < 0.05) in a screening of executive functions (FAB) and in phonemic verbal fluency. Depression and anxiety scores were elevated in 28% and 22% of MSA patients, respectively. Cognitive profile, depression, and anxiety levels were comparable between patients with and without OH. INTERPRETATION: Cognitive deficits are relatively frequent in MSA and primarily affect executive functions and verbal memory. Future comparative studies including Parkinson dementia, Lewy body disease, and MSA cases with and without OH are required to elucidate disease-specific cognitive profiles in these synucleinopathies and to examine the influence of cardiovascular autonomic dysfunction on cognitive function in MSA.


Subject(s)
Cognitive Dysfunction/physiopathology , Executive Function/physiology , Hypotension, Orthostatic/physiopathology , Multiple System Atrophy/physiopathology , Aged , Anxiety/physiopathology , Cognitive Dysfunction/etiology , Depression/physiopathology , Female , Humans , Hypotension, Orthostatic/etiology , Male , Middle Aged , Multiple System Atrophy/complications , Neuropsychological Tests , Retrospective Studies
14.
Neurocrit Care ; 32(2): 492-501, 2020 04.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31222466

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: Subarachnoid hemorrhage (SAH) is a devastating disease associated with high mortality and morbidity. Besides neurological sequelae, neuropsychological deficits largely contribute to patients' long-term quality of life. Little is known about the pituitary gland volume (PGV) after SAH compared to healthy referents and the association of PGV with long-term outcome including cognitive function. METHODS: Sixty consecutive non-traumatic SAH patients admitted to the neurological intensive care unit between 2010 and 2014 were enrolled. 3-Tesla magnetic resonance imagining was performed at baseline (16 days) and 12 months after SAH to measure PGV semi-automatically using the software iPlan Net 3.5.0. PGV was compared to age and sex matched healthy referents. The difference between baseline and 1-year-PGV was classified as increase (> 20 mm3 PGV increase), stable (± 20 mm3), or decrease (> 20 mm3 PGV decrease). In addition, total intracerebral volume was calculated. Neuropsychological testing was applied in 43 SAH patients at 1-year follow up encompassing several domains (executive, attention, memory) and self-assessment (questionnaire for self-perceived deficits in attention [German: FEDA]) of distractibility in mental processes, fatigue and decrease in motivation. Multivariable regression with multivariable generalized linear models was used for comparison of PGVs and for subgroup analysis to evaluate a potential association between PGV and neuropsychological outcome. RESULTS: Patients were 53 years old (IQR = 44-63) and presented with a median Hunt&Hess grade of 2 (IQR = 1-3). SAH patients had a significantly lower PGV both at baseline (360 ± 19 mm3, p < 0.001) and 1 year (367 ± 18 mm3p < 0.001) as compared to matched referents (mean 505 ± 18 mm3). PGV decreased by 75 ± 8 mm3 in 28 patients, increased by 120 ± 22 mm3 in 22 patients and remained stable in 10 patients at 1-year follow-up. PGV in patients with PGV increase at 12 months was not different to healthy referents (p = 0.062). Low baseline PGV was associated with impaired executive functions at 1 year (adjOR = 8.81, 95%-CI = 1.46-53.10, p = 0.018) and PGV decrease within 1 year was associated with self-perceived worse motivation (FEDA; Wald-statistic = 6.6, df = 1, p = 0.010). CONCLUSIONS: Our data indicate significantly lower PGVs following SAH. The association of sustained PGV decrease with impaired neuropsychological long-term outcome warrants further investigations including neuroendocrine hormone measurements.


Subject(s)
Cognitive Dysfunction/physiopathology , Pituitary Gland/diagnostic imaging , Subarachnoid Hemorrhage/physiopathology , Adult , Aged , Atrophy/etiology , Attention , Cognitive Dysfunction/etiology , Cognitive Dysfunction/psychology , Executive Function , Female , Humans , Linear Models , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Male , Memory , Mental Fatigue/etiology , Mental Fatigue/physiopathology , Mental Fatigue/psychology , Middle Aged , Motivation , Multivariate Analysis , Neuropsychological Tests , Organ Size , Pituitary Gland/pathology , Subarachnoid Hemorrhage/complications , Subarachnoid Hemorrhage/diagnostic imaging , Subarachnoid Hemorrhage/psychology , Surveys and Questionnaires
15.
Front Neurol ; 10: 1209, 2019.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31824399

ABSTRACT

Background: Language function may be reorganized in patients with malformations of cortical development (MCD). This prospective cohort study aimed in assessing language dominance in a large group of patients with MCD and epilepsy using functional MRI (fMRI). Methods: Sixty-eight patients (40 women) aged 10-73 years (median, 28.0; interquartile range, 19) with MCD and epilepsy underwent 1.5 T MRI and fMRI (word generation task). Single-subject image analysis was performed with statistical parametric mapping (SPM12). Language lateralization indices (LIs) were defined for statistically significantly activated voxels in Broca's and Wernicke's areas using the formula: LI = (V L - V R)/(V L + V R) × 100, where V L and V R were sets of activated voxels on the left and on the right, respectively. Language laterality was considered typical if LI was between +20 and +100 or atypical if LI was between +19 and -100. Results: fMRI signal was elicited in 55 of 68 (81%) patients. In 18 of 55 (33%) patients, language dominance was typical, and in 37 of 55 (67%) patients, atypical (in 68%, right hemispheric; in 32%, bilateral). Language dominance was not influenced by handedness, electroclinical, and imaging features. Conclusions: In this prospective study on a large group of patients with MCD and epilepsy, about two-thirds had atypical language dominance. These results may contribute to assessing risks of postsurgical language deficits and could assist in planning of "cortical mapping" with intracranial electrodes in patients who undergo presurgical assessment.

16.
Brain Cogn ; 134: 1-8, 2019 08.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31054405

ABSTRACT

Recent evidence has suggested that the hippocampus supports learning and retrieval of arithmetic facts during childhood and adolescence. Whether the hippocampus is also involved in retrieving overlearned arithmetic facts (such as 3 × 5 = 15) during adult age is open for investigation. In this study, we assessed whether patients with hippocampal atrophy due to Alzheimer's disease (AD) are still able to retrieve overlearned arithmetic facts from memory. Sixteen patients (n = 13 with AD, n = 3 with Mild Cognitive Impairment - MCI) were evaluated using standard radiological, neurological, and neuropsychological test procedures. We adopted a multiple single-case analysis in order to acknowledge possible dissociations between hippocampal degeneration and intact arithmetic fact retrieval. All patients performed a neuropsychological screening battery assessing episodic memory as well as arithmetic processing, and underwent a 3-Tesla MRI procedure. A morphometric analysis comprising estimation of both cortical thickness and hippocampal volume, which also included a subfield analysis, was conducted. All patients had marked hippocampal atrophy (bilateral n = 15, unilateral n = 1) in comparison to healthy matched controls and showed deficits in episodic memory (delayed recall). However, 13 out of 16 patients performed in the average range of standardised norms during retrieval of overlearned arithmetic facts (i.e. multiplication tables). Our results suggest that intact retrieval of consolidated arithmetic facts from memory does not depend on the integrity of the hippocampus. This is in line with the view that the hippocampus plays a dynamic and time-limited role in arithmetic processing. While the hippocampus seems to be necessary for learning and consolidating new arithmetic facts in memory, it might not be critically involved in retrieving arithmetic facts when these are well consolidated in memory.


Subject(s)
Alzheimer Disease/pathology , Hippocampus/pathology , Memory, Episodic , Temporal Lobe/pathology , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Alzheimer Disease/psychology , Cognitive Dysfunction/pathology , Cognitive Dysfunction/psychology , Female , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Male , Mental Recall , Neuropsychological Tests
18.
Cogn Process ; 20(1): 125-131, 2019 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30377871

ABSTRACT

Studies indicate that a brief period of wakeful rest after learning supports memory retention, whereas distraction weakens it. It is open for investigation whether advanced age has a significant effect on the impact of post-learning wakeful rest on memory retention for verbal information when compared to a cognitively demanding distraction task. In this study, we examined (1) whether post-learning rest promotes verbal memory retention in younger and older adults and (2) whether the magnitude of the rest benefit changes with increasing age. Younger adults and older adults learned and immediately recalled two consecutive word lists. After one word list, participants rested wakefully for 8 min; after the other list, they solved matrices. Memory performance was again tested in a surprise free recall test at the end of the experimental session. We found that, overall, younger adults outperformed older adults. Also, memory retention was higher following a wakeful rest phase compared to distraction. A detailed analysis revealed that this wakeful rest benefit was significant for the older adults group, whereas the younger adults group retained a similar amount of information in both post-encoding conditions. We assume that older adults can profit more from a wakeful rest phase after learning and are more prone to distraction than younger adults. With increasing age, a short break immediately after information uptake may help better retain the previously learned information, while distraction after learning tends to weaken memory retention.


Subject(s)
Memory/physiology , Rest/psychology , Wakefulness/physiology , Adolescent , Adult , Age Factors , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Female , Humans , Learning , Mental Recall , Middle Aged , Random Allocation , Young Adult
19.
J Alzheimers Dis ; 64(4): 1213-1226, 2018.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30010137

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: Patients with mild cognitive impairment (MCI) show lower decision making and ratio processing abilities as compared to healthy peers. OBJECTIVE: To evaluate whether cognitive training on number processing and/or executive functions improves performance on ratio processing and decision making under risk. METHODS: In a controlled cross-over study, patients with MCI (n = 23; mean MMSE 26.48, SD 2.43) underwent a week of numerical training followed by a week of executive-functions training (subgroup A), or vice versa (subgroup B). Before training (T1), patients performed experimental tasks of decision making (Game of Dice Task, GDT; Probability-Associated Gambling task, PAG-60 task) and of ratio processing as well as a neuropsychological background assessment. Experimental tasks were also administered after the first (T2) and the second (T3) training week. RESULTS: The numerical training and the training of executive functions had a differential effect on experimental tasks of ratio processing. Only the numerical training proved to be effective. The effects of the two training types on decision making under risk were less clear-cut. While no changes over time were observed in the GDT, performance on the PAG-60 task improved in both training subgroups. These improvements were apparent in one subgroup after a period of executive-functions training, in the other subgroup after both training weeks. That means, improvements are not attributable to one specific training type. CONCLUSION: Patients with MCI can profit from a cognitive training on number processing and executive functions. Improvements are reflected in higher ratio processing abilities and more advantageous decisions after training. These results are consistent with assumptions of current cognitive models.


Subject(s)
Cognitive Behavioral Therapy/methods , Cognitive Dysfunction/rehabilitation , Decision Making/physiology , Executive Function/physiology , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Cross-Over Studies , Female , Games, Experimental , Humans , Male , Neuropsychological Tests , Probability , Psychomotor Performance/physiology
20.
Front Neurol ; 9: 359, 2018.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29942277

ABSTRACT

The eye-tracking study aimed at assessing spatial biases in visual exploration in patients after acute right MCA (middle cerebral artery) stroke. Patients affected by unilateral neglect show less functional recovery and experience severe difficulties in everyday life. Thus, accurate diagnosis is essential, and specific treatment is required. Early assessment is of high importance as rehabilitative interventions are more effective when applied soon after stroke. Previous research has shown that deficits may be overlooked when classical paper-and-pencil tasks are used for diagnosis. Conversely, eye-tracking allows direct monitoring of visual exploration patterns. We hypothesized that the analysis of eye-tracking provides more sensitive measures for spatial exploration deficits after right middle cerebral artery stroke. Twenty-two patients with right MCA stroke (median 5 days after stroke) and 28 healthy controls were included. Lesions were confirmed by MRI/CCT. Groups performed comparably in the Mini-Mental State Examination (patients and controls median 29) and in a screening of executive functions. Eleven patients scored at ceiling in neglect screening tasks, 11 showed minimal to severe signs of unilateral visual neglect. An overlap plot based on MRI and CCT imaging showed lesions in the temporo-parieto-frontal cortex, basal ganglia, and adjacent white matter tracts. Visual exploration was evaluated in two eye-tracking tasks, one assessing free visual exploration of photographs, the other visual search using symbols and letters. An index of fixation asymmetries proved to be a sensitive measure of spatial exploration deficits. Both patient groups showed a marked exploration bias to the right when looking at complex photographs. A single case analysis confirmed that also most of those patients who showed no neglect in screening tasks performed outside the range of controls in free exploration. The analysis of patients' scoring at ceiling in neglect screening tasks is of special interest, as possible deficits may be overlooked and thus remain untreated. Our findings are in line with other studies suggesting considerable limitations of laboratory screening procedures to fully appreciate the occurrence of neglect symptoms. Future investigations are needed to explore the predictive value of the eye-tracking index and its validity in everyday situations.

SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...