Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 5 de 5
Filter
Add more filters











Database
Language
Publication year range
1.
Eur J Behav Anal ; 17(1): 49-68, 2016.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28490976

ABSTRACT

This paper discusses recent methodological approaches and investigations that are aimed at developing reliable behavioral technology for teaching stimulus-stimulus relations to individuals who are minimally verbal and show protracted difficulty in acquiring such relations. The paper has both empirical and theoretical content. The empirical component presents recent data concerning the possibility of generating rapid relational learning in individuals who do not initially show it. The theoretical component (1) considers decades of methodological investigations with this population and (2) suggests a testable hypothesis concerning some individuals exhibit unusual difficulties in learning. Given this background, we suggest a way forward to better understand and perhaps resolve these learning challenges.

2.
Phys Rev E Stat Nonlin Soft Matter Phys ; 72(4 Pt 1): 041702, 2005 Oct.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16383397

ABSTRACT

The classification of phase transitions in first-order and second-order (or continuous) ones is widely used. The nematic-to-isotropic (NI) transition in liquid crystals is a weakly first-order transition, with only small discontinuities in enthalpy and specific volume at the transition which are not always easy to measure. On the other hand, fluctuation effects near the transition, typical for a continuous transition, are present because of the only weakly first-order character. In a recent paper [Phys. Rev. E 69, 022701 (2004)], it was concluded from the static dielectric permittivity in the isotropic phase near the NI transition that less polar mesogens (with little or no pretransitional effects) are characteristic for a first-order NI phase transition, whereas in the case of strongly polar ones (with large pretransitional effects) the NI transition is close to second order. In this paper, we address the question whether it is, indeed, possible to use these fluctuation effects in the isotropic phase to quantify the "strength" of a weakly first-order transition, i.e., how far it is from second order. Therefore, we measured the temperature dependence of the enthalpy near the NI transition of seven liquid crystals with adiabatic scanning calorimetry and compared the measured values of the latent heat with pretransitional effects in the dielectric constant and the specific heat capacity. The compounds used in the comparison are MBBA, 5CB, 8CB, 5NCS, 5CN, 8CHBT, and D7AB. From our analysis we find, contrary to the assertion in the above reference, no correlation between the strength of the NI transition of a given compound and the pretransitional effects observed, neither dielectrically, nor thermally.

3.
Eur Phys J E Soft Matter ; 16(4): 361-4, 2005 Apr.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15696268

ABSTRACT

We investigated the smectic-A-hexatic-B (SmA-HexB) transition in the liquid-crystal n-hexyl-4'-n-pentyloxybiphenyl-4-carboxilate (650BC) with adiabatic scanning calorimetry. We were able to prove in a direct way that this transition is indeed very weakly first order, as was already suggested in the literature. The latent heat at the transition was determined to be deltaHL = 0.04 +/- 0.02 J/g. Our experiments confirm the high value for the heat capacity critical exponent earlier reported, yielding alpha = 0.64 +/- 0.05.

4.
J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process ; 27(3): 239-51, 2001 Jul.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11497323

ABSTRACT

Pigeons were trained on symbolic matching with 2 samples, 2 pairs of comparisons, and different outcomes for the correct responses within each comparison pair. For one group, the 2 samples were also associated with different outcomes, whereas for another group, they were not. When the response-outcome (R-O) relations for one pair were subsequently reversed, the group trained with differential sample-outcome (S-O) associations was significantly disrupted in its performance on both reversed- and nonreversed-outcome trials. By contrast, the group trained with just differential R-O associations was disrupted only on reversed-outcome trials. These results were replicated when the outcomes on the initially nonreversed trials were then reversed. The findings indicate that differential S-O associations, when present, have a stronger influence on matching performances than differential R-O associations. They are also consistent with hierarchical and configural models of discriminative control.


Subject(s)
Discrimination Learning , Animals , Behavior, Animal , Columbidae , Conditioning, Psychological
5.
J Exp Anal Behav ; 73(2): 141-61, 2000 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10784006

ABSTRACT

This study examined the conditions under which conditional stimulus control by the sample stimuli in three-key matching-to-sample paradigms would generalize across the different possible sample locations. In Experiments 1 and 2, the samples appeared on the left and right side keys during initial training and then on the center key during testing. Transfer of pigeons' matching performances to the center-key samples was evident after both identity and symbolic matching training. In Experiment 3, pigeons trained on symbolic matching with two side-key samples or with a side-key and a center-key sample generally transferred their learned matching performances to those samples when they subsequently appeared in the remaining (novel) location. These results indicate that, when two-choice conditional discriminations are learned with more than one sample location, the visual characteristics of the sample per se predominantly come to control the pigeons' comparison choices. This finding encourages the use of the multiple-location training procedure as a way of reducing control by location, thus providing a more discriminating test of symmetry in animals.


Subject(s)
Behavior, Animal/physiology , Animals , Columbidae , Reinforcement, Psychology
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL