Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 7 de 7
Filter
Add more filters










Database
Language
Publication year range
1.
Neurophotonics ; 10(2): 023520, 2023 Apr.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37077217

ABSTRACT

Open science practices work to increase methodological rigor, transparency, and replicability of published findings. We aim to reflect on what the functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) community has done to promote open science practices in fNIRS research and set goals to accomplish over the next 10 years.

2.
J Autism Dev Disord ; 51(3): 961-972, 2021 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32594334

ABSTRACT

Individuals with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) show atypical processing of facial expressions. Research with autistic toddlers suggests that abnormalities in processing of spatial frequencies (SFs) contribute to such differences. The current event-related-potential (ERP) study investigated differences between 10-month-old infants with high- and low-likelihood for ASD in SF processing and in discrimination of fearful and neutral faces, filtered to contain specific SF. Results indicate no group differences in general processing of higher (HSF, detailed) and lower-SF (LSF, global) information. However, unlike low-likelihood infants, high-likelihood infants do not discriminate between facial expressions when either the LSF or HSF information is available. Combined with previous findings in toddlers, the current results indicate a developmental delay in efficient processing of facial expressions in ASD.


Subject(s)
Autism Spectrum Disorder/psychology , Brain/physiology , Evoked Potentials/physiology , Facial Expression , Fear/physiology , Fear/psychology , Autism Spectrum Disorder/diagnosis , Autism Spectrum Disorder/physiopathology , Electroencephalography/methods , Female , Humans , Infant , Male , Photic Stimulation/methods
3.
Dev Cogn Neurosci ; 45: 100840, 2020 10.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32877890

ABSTRACT

From infancy onwards, EEG is widely used to measure face-categorization, i.e. differential brain activity to faces versus non-face stimuli. Four ERP components likely signal infants' face-sensitivity but reflect different underlying mechanisms: the P1, N290, P400, Nc. We test whether these components reveal similar developmental patterns from early to late infancy, using a longitudinal dataset of 80 infants tested at 5 and 10 months. The P1, N290, and the Nc show face-categorization already in 5-months-olds, a pattern which did not change over time. Development is visible as increased amplitudes in all components, but similar for face and non-face stimuli. By using Markov models, we illustrate that there are differences in the distribution of individual trajectories of face-categorization components from 5 to 10 months. Whereas individual trajectories appear more varied for the Nc and the P1, the N290 reveals a more consistent pattern: a larger proportion of 5-month-olds shows the dominant group response; a larger proportion of 10-month-olds remains in this group, and larger proportions of the alternative trajectories from 5- to 10-month-olds move towards the dominant group. This is vital information when one wants to examine individual differences in infant ERPs related to face-categorization.


Subject(s)
Electroencephalography/methods , Evoked Potentials/physiology , Face/physiology , Female , Humans , Infant , Longitudinal Studies , Male
4.
Neuroimage ; 200: 511-527, 2019 10 15.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31247300

ABSTRACT

Despite motion artifacts are a major source of noise in fNIRS infant data, how to approach motion correction in this population has only recently started to be investigated. Homer2 offers a wide range of motion correction methods and previous work on simulated and adult data suggested the use of Spline interpolation and Wavelet filtering as optimal methods for the recovery of trials affected by motion. However, motion artifacts in infant data differ from those in adults' both in amplitude and frequency of occurrence. Therefore, artifact correction recommendations derived from adult data might not be optimal for infant data. We hypothesized that the combined use of Spline and Wavelet would outperform their individual use on data with complex profiles of motion artifacts. To demonstrate this, we first compared, on infant semi-simulated data, the performance of several motion correction techniques on their own and of the novel combined approach; then, we investigated the performance of Spline and Wavelet alone and in combination on real cognitive data from three datasets collected with infants of different ages (5, 7 and 10 months), with different tasks (auditory, visual and tactile) and with different NIRS systems. To quantitatively estimate and compare the efficacy of these techniques, we adopted four metrics: hemodynamic response recovery error, within-subject standard deviation, between-subjects standard deviation and number of trials that survived each correction method. Our results demonstrated that (i) it is always better correcting for motion artifacts than rejecting the corrupted trials; (ii) Wavelet filtering on its own and in combination with Spline interpolation seems to be the most effective approach in reducing the between- and the within-subject standard deviations. Importantly, the combination of Spline and Wavelet was the approach providing the best performance in semi-simulation both at low and high levels of noise, also recovering most of the trials affected by motion artifacts across all datasets, a crucial result when working with infant data.


Subject(s)
Artifacts , Cerebral Cortex/physiology , Functional Neuroimaging/standards , Image Processing, Computer-Assisted/standards , Spectroscopy, Near-Infrared/standards , Cerebral Cortex/diagnostic imaging , Female , Head Movements/physiology , Humans , Infant , Male
5.
Front Psychol ; 10: 1240, 2019.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31191416

ABSTRACT

Processing faces and understanding facial expressions are crucial skills for social communication. In adults, basic face processing and facial emotion processing rely on specific interacting brain networks. In infancy, however, little is known about when and how these networks develop. The current study uses functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) to measure differences in 5-month-olds' brain activity in response to fearful and happy facial expressions. Our results show that the right occipital region responds to faces, indicating that the face processing network is activated at 5 months. Yet sensitivity to facial emotions appears to be still immature at this age: explorative analyses suggest that if the facial emotion processing network was active this would be mainly visible in the temporal cortex. Together these results indicate that at 5 months, occipital areas already show sensitivity to face processing, while the facial emotion processing network seems not fully developed.

6.
PLoS One ; 13(6): e0198779, 2018.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29902186

ABSTRACT

It is suggested that testosterone may play a part in the higher prevalence of Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) in males compared to females. Previous studies have reported elevated postnatal testosterone levels in children and women with ASD but not in men. We compared levels of salivary testosterone across 67 undergraduate males (Mage 19.5 yrs, SD 1.92) selected for low, mid-range and high levels of autistic traits assessed using the Autism-spectrum Quotient. Analyses revealed no significant differences in testosterone concentrations across the three groups. The current data add to the increasing evidence for the lack of relationship between autistic traits and postnatal levels of testosterone in men.


Subject(s)
Autism Spectrum Disorder/psychology , Saliva/metabolism , Testosterone/metabolism , Adolescent , Adult , Autism Spectrum Disorder/metabolism , Humans , Male , Personality/physiology , Young Adult
7.
J Autism Dev Disord ; 46(9): 2924-39, 2016 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27312717

ABSTRACT

People with autism show superior performance to controls on the Embedded Figures Test (EFT). However, studies examining the relationship between autistic-like traits and EFT performance in neurotypical individuals have yielded inconsistent findings. To examine the inconsistency, a meta-analysis was conducted of studies that (a) compared high and low Autism-Spectrum Quotient (AQ) groups, and (b) treated AQ as a continuous variable. Outcomes are consistent with superior visual search forming part of the broader autism phenotype, but in existing literature, this is evident only when comparing extreme groups. Reanalysis of data from previous studies suggests findings are unlikely to be driven by a small number of high scorers. Monte Carlo simulations are used to illustrate the effect of methodological differences on results.


Subject(s)
Autism Spectrum Disorder/psychology , Cognition , Female , Humans , Male , Monte Carlo Method , Phenotype
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...