Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 9 de 9
Filter
Add more filters










Database
Language
Publication year range
1.
Neuropsychologia ; 39(2): 173-83, 2001.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11163374

ABSTRACT

The oculomotor and manual motor systems were studied in a two-segment movement task in a group of patients with Parkinson's disease and in age matched normal controls. In order to avoid reflexive motor movements the selection of the correct motor sequence was derived from the interpretation of symbolic (coloured) cues. The latencies and dynamics of eye and hand (pointing) movements performed during the first (fixed) movement segment were measured and the planning and execution processes were manipulated by varying the complexity of the second movement segment relative to the first (with regard to direction and amplitude). The results showed that the eye and hand movements made by patients with Parkinson's disease were not impaired in the initiation of the first movement segment. Interestingly, both Parkinson's patients and controls showed increased eye and hand reaction time latencies for the first movement when the second movement was in the direction opposite to the first. This indicates that the complexity of the second movement influences the execution of the first movement, and importantly that complexity affects motor initiation and execution processes in both normal subjects and in patients with Parkinson's disease. The execution of hand movements was found to be impaired in patients with Parkinson's disease as indicated by a reduced peak velocity of manual pointing responses when compared to age matched controls. By contrast, no differences were found in the dynamics of saccadic eye movements. This dissociation is consistent with the notion that the skeletomotor loop passes through the functionally corresponding portions of the basal ganglia independently of the oculomotor loop. Together, these results demonstrate that Parkinson's patients are able to generate multiple non-reflexive eye and hand movements and that the observed (manual) motor deficits are specific to the processes of motor execution.


Subject(s)
Basal Ganglia/physiopathology , Hand , Hypokinesia/physiopathology , Parkinson Disease/physiopathology , Psychomotor Performance , Saccades , Aged , Case-Control Studies , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Models, Neurological , Neural Pathways/physiopathology , Parkinson Disease/psychology , Refractory Period, Electrophysiological
2.
Neuropsychologia ; 37(8): 927-38, 1999 Jul.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10426518

ABSTRACT

Mechanisms of spatial working memory and eye movement control were investigated in eight mild to moderate Parkinson's disease patients (PDs). Subjects were presented with a sequence of four targets which had to be memorized and then recalled by moving their eyes to fixate the four locations in the correct order. Two variations on this procedure were used in which either a different sequence of lights was presented on each trial, or an identical sequence of lights was repeated on each trial. In both conditions subjects made memory-guided eye movements in the dark, without any visual cues to eye movement accuracy or the locations of the previously illuminated lights. Analysis of the amplitude of the primary eye movement and final eye position for each step in the sequence showed that PDs made several discrete saccadic eye movements of reduced amplitude before reaching the final eye position (multi-stepping). When a novel target sequence had to be memorized on each trial, the final eye position reached by PDs for each location was also found to undershoot relative to controls. In contrast, when an identical sequence of targets was repeated on each trial, PDs' final eye position was found to be normal, although primary movement amplitudes were still reduced. PDs showed no multi-stepping and normal final eye position gain under conditions for which the target lights in the sequence were illuminated during movement execution. PDs also made an increased proportion of overt errors in target sequence recall. Parallel neuropsychological testing in PDs and controls revealed that error rates in the sequential memory-guided saccade task were significantly correlated with performance in a task thought to be sensitive to spatial working memory dysfunction. The findings suggest that short-term spatial memory representations are disrupted in the early stages of PD.


Subject(s)
Memory, Short-Term , Ocular Motility Disorders/physiopathology , Parkinson Disease/physiopathology , Space Perception , Aged , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Neuropsychological Tests , Parkinson Disease/drug therapy , Parkinson Disease/psychology , Saccades
3.
Behav Processes ; 46(1): 97-102, 1999 May 03.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24925502

ABSTRACT

Pigeons (Columba livia) were trained on a visual discrimination task using a novel apparatus which enabled pinned specimens of insects, illuminated by natural daylight, to be presented under a pecking key transparent to ultraviolet light. Three birds showed evidence of learning to discriminate between sets of wasp and fly specimens. This response transferred to specimens of four hoverfly species, the strength of the response varying between the different hoverfly species. This conditioning technique offers a promising means of analysing mechanisms of visual processing in birds that are relevant to theories of the evolution of camouflage and mimicry.

4.
Br J Psychol ; 89 ( Pt 4): 531-54, 1998 Nov.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9854803

ABSTRACT

Almost from the inception of psychological enquiry into reaction time (RT), a broad distinction has been drawn between two types of process-serving performance in RT tasks, namely, the on-line processes initiated by the arrival of the imperative signal and preparatory processes, which precede the imperative signal and may contribute to performance efficiency. Restriction of attention to on-line processing fosters the conclusion that the processes serving simple RT are no more than a subset of those involved in choice reactions. If, however, latencies can be reduced by specific preparation then the certitudes of the simple reaction task may invest it with distinctive properties. The possibility of preparation being sensory as well as motor needs to be considered. Until recently, attempts to study preparedness have failed to make controlled comparisons across simple and choice RT. However, recent work has shown that simple reactions have distinctive characteristics that set them apart from minimal choice reactions. The optimization of simple RT seems to depend upon attention-demanding processes which are probably preparatory in nature. When attention has to be shared with a concurrent task, much of the normal advantage of the simple reaction is lost. Neuropsychological studies suggest that patients may be found with abnormally extended simple RT but spared choice reactions. In these patients, the simple reactions are prolonged relative to those of controls but less vulnerable to imposition of a secondary task. Evidence is beginning to accrue to the effect that some distinctive processing features of simple reactions may be mediated by activity in the frontal lobes.


Subject(s)
Attention , Reaction Time , Humans , Psychophysics , Signal Detection, Psychological
5.
Perception ; 25(6): 727-38, 1996.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8888304

ABSTRACT

It is well known that biological motion, as produced by point-light displays on a human body, gives a good representation of the represented body-eg its gender and the nature of the task which it is engaged in. The question is whether it is possible to judge the emotional state of a human body from motion information alone. An ability to make this kind of judgment may imply that people are able to perceive emotion from patterns of movement without having to compute the detailed shape first. Subjects were shown brief video clips of two trained dancers (one male, one female). The dancers were aiming to convey the following emotions: fear, anger, grief, joy, surprise, and disgust. The video clips portrayed fully lit scenes and point-light scenes, with thirteen small points of light attached to the body of each dancer. Half the stimuli were presented the right way up, while half were inverted. The subjects' task was to judge which emotion was being portrayed. Full-body clips gave good recognition of emotionality (88% correct), but the results for upright biological-motion displays were also significantly above chance (63% correct). Inversion of the display reduced biological-motion (but not full-body) performance to close to chance but still significantly above chance. A space-time analysis of the motion of the points of light was carried out, and was related to the discriminability of the different emotions. Biological-motion displays, which convey no information while static, are able to give a rich description of the subject matter, including the ability to judge emotional state. This ability is disrupted when the image is inverted.


Subject(s)
Dancing , Emotions , Kinesics , Pattern Recognition, Visual , Analysis of Variance , Female , Humans , Lighting , Male , Posture , Sex Factors
6.
Perception ; 23(3): 253-68, 1994.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-7971105

ABSTRACT

A series of experiments were performed to investigate how motion sequences provide information about the intentional structure of moving figures or actors. Observers had to detect simulations of biologically meaningful motion within a set of moving letters. In the first two experiments a factorial design was used, with type of instruction as a between-subject factor and six movement parameters (number of items, speed and directness of target and distractors, and 'relentlessness' of target movement) as within-subject factor; in the final two experiments, the visibility of the goal towards which the target moved and the use of a tracking movement to distinguish the target were varied. In such displays search time increases with increasing number of stimuli. It was found that (a) the more direct the motion, the more likely it was to be interpreted as intentional; (b) intentional motion was much easier to detect when the target moved faster than the distractors than when it moved more slowly; (c) recognition of intentionality was impaired but not abolished if the goal towards which the target was moving was invisible; and (d) participants did not report intentional movement when the target was distinguished by brightness rather than the manner in which it moved. We argue that the perception of intentionality is strongly related to observers' use of conceptual knowledge, which in turn is activated by particular combinations of features. This supports a process model, in which intentionality is seen as the result of a conceptual integration of objective visual features.


Subject(s)
Attention , Motion Perception , Motivation , Pattern Recognition, Visual , Animals , Awareness , Discrimination Learning , Humans , Orientation , Predatory Behavior , Psychophysics
7.
J Exp Anal Behav ; 59(1): 115-29, 1993 Jan.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16812680

ABSTRACT

Three groups of pigeons were trained with a modified discriminative autoshaping procedure to discriminate video images of other pigeons on the basis of movement. Birds of all groups were shown the same video images of other pigeons, which were either moving or still. The group to whom food was presented only after moving images learned the discrimination very quickly. A second group, to whom food was given only after still images, and a pseudocategory group, to whom food was presented after arbitrarily chosen stimuli, showed no evidence of discrimination during acquisition training. Extinction conditions led to clear differences in peck rates to moving and still images in the second group but not in the pseudocategory group. The result is related to the feature-positive effect. Generalization tests showed that the discrimination performance was based on visual features of the stimuli but was invariant against changes of size, perspective, brightness, and color. Furthermore, discrimination was maintained when novel images of pigeons under different viewing angles and seven other types of motion categories were presented. It is argued that the discrimination is based not on a common motion feature but on motion concepts or high-order generalization across motion categories.

8.
Perception ; 22(1): 15-22, 1993.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8474831

ABSTRACT

Johansson filmed walkers and runners in a dark room with lights attached to their main joints and demonstrated that such moving light spots were perceived as human movements. To extend this finding the detection and recognition of Johansson displays of different kinds of movements under three light-spot conditions were studied to determine how human actions are perceived on the basis of biological-motion information. Locomotory, instrumental, and social actions were presented in each condition, namely in normal Johansson (light attached to joints), inter-joint (light attached between joints), and upside-down Johansson. Subjects' verbal responses and recognition times were measured. Locomotory actions were recognised better and faster than social and instrumental actions. Furthermore, biological motions were recognised much better and faster when the light-spot displays were presented in the normal orientation rather than upside down. Recognition rate was only slightly impaired under the inter-joint condition. It is argued that the perceptual analysis of actions and movements starts primarily on an intermediate level of action coding and comprises more than just the similarity of movement patterns or simple structures. Additionally, coding of dynamic phase relations and semantic coding take place at very early stages of the processing of biological motion. Implications of these results for computer vision, perceptual models, and mental representations are discussed.


Subject(s)
Locomotion , Visual Perception , Female , Humans , Male , Perceptual Masking , Sex Factors
9.
Exp Brain Res ; 89(1): 157-71, 1992.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-1601094

ABSTRACT

Neural mechanisms underlying recognition of objects must overcome the changes in an object's appearance caused by inconsistent viewing conditions, particularly those that occur with changes in lighting. In humans, lesions to the posterior visual association cortex can impair the ability to recognize objects and faces across different lighting conditions. Inferotemporal lesions in monkey have been shown to produce a similar difficulty in object matching tasks. Here we report on the extent to which cell responses selective for the face and other views of the head in monkey temporal cortex tolerate changes in lighting. For each cell studied the (preferred) head view eliciting maximal response was first established under normal lighting. Cells were then tested with the preferred head view lit from different directions (i.e. front, above, below or from the side). Responses of some cells failed to show complete generalization across all lighting conditions but together as a "population" they responded equally strongly under all four lighting conditions. Further tests on sub-groups of cells revealed that stimulus selectivity was maintained despite unusual lighting. The cells discriminated between head and control stimuli and between different views of the head independent of the lighting direction. The results indicate that constancy of recognition across different lighting conditions is apparent in the responses of single cells in the temporal cortex. Lighting constancy appears to be established by matching the retinal image to view-specific descriptions of objects (i.e. neurons which compute object structure from a limited range of perspective views).


Subject(s)
Discrimination, Psychological , Neurons/physiology , Temporal Lobe/physiology , Visual Perception , Animals , Brain Damage, Chronic/physiopathology , Eye Movements , Face , Female , Humans , Lighting , Macaca mulatta , Male , Models, Psychological , Retina/physiology , Time Factors
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...