Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 20 de 40
Filter
1.
Vision Res ; 164: 34-43, 2019 11.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31557606

ABSTRACT

Careful measurements of the temporal dynamics of speech have provided important insights into phonetic properties of spoken languages, which are important for understanding auditory perception. By contrast, analytic quantification of the visual properties of signed languages is still largely uncharted. Exposure to sign language is a unique experience that could shape and modify low-level visual processing for those who use it regularly (i.e., what we refer to as the Enhanced Exposure Hypothesis). The purpose of the current study was to characterize the visual spatiotemporal properties of American Sign Language (ASL) so that future studies can test the enhanced exposure hypothesis in signers, with the prediction that altered vision should be observed within, more so than outside, the range of properties found in ASL. Using an ultrasonic motion tracking system, we recorded the hand position in 3-dimensional space over time during sign language production of signs, sentences, and narratives. From these data, we calculated several metrics: hand position and eccentricity in space and hand motion speed. For individual signs, we also measured total distance travelled by the dominant hand and total duration of each sign. These metrics were found to fall within a selective range, suggesting that exposure to signs is a specific and unique visual experience, which might alter visual perceptual abilities in signers for visual information within the experienced range, even for non-language stimuli.


Subject(s)
Brain/physiology , Functional Laterality/physiology , Language , Motion Perception/physiology , Phonetics , Sign Language , Speech Perception/physiology , Humans , Young Adult
2.
J Autism Dev Disord ; 47(11): 3431-3445, 2017 Nov.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28770526

ABSTRACT

The current study investigated the prevalence and pattern of unusual sensory behaviors (USBs) in teens with Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) and infants (3-36 months) at risk for ASD. From two different sites (UCSD and UConn), caregivers of infants at high (n = 32) and low risk (n = 33) for ASD, and teenagers with (n = 12) and without ASD (n = 11), completed age-appropriate Sensory Profile questionnaires (Infant/Toddler Sensory Profile; Dunn 2002; Adolescent/Adult Sensory Profile; Brown and Dunn 2002). The results show that high-risk infants and teenagers with ASD exhibit higher-than-typical prevalence of USBs. Results of our distribution analyses investigating the direction of sensory atypicalities (greater-than-typical vs. less-than-typical) revealed a fair degree of consistency amongst teens, however, USB patterns were more varied in high-risk infants.


Subject(s)
Adolescent Behavior , Autism Spectrum Disorder/epidemiology , Child Behavior , Sensation , Adolescent , Child, Preschool , Cognition , Female , Humans , Infant , Male
3.
J Pediatr Gastroenterol Nutr ; 62(2): 317-27, 2016 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26230900

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVES: Parents of children with autism spectrum disorders (ASDs) often report gastrointestinal (GI) dysfunction in their children. The objectives of the present study were to determine whether infants at high risk for developing ASD (ie, siblings of children diagnosed as having ASD) show greater prevalence of GI problems and whether this prevalence is associated with diet and age at weaning from breast milk. METHODS: Using questionnaires, diet history and GI problems were tracked prospectively and retrospectively in 57 high-risk infants and for comparison in 114 low-risk infants (infants from families without ASD history). RESULTS: In low-risk infants, prevalence of GI symptoms, in aggregate, did not vary with diet or age of weaning. By contrast, high-risk infants with GI symptoms were weaned earlier than those without symptoms (P < 0.04), and high-risk infants showed greater prevalence of GI symptoms, in aggregate, on a no breast milk diet than on an exclusive breast milk diet (P < 0.017). Constipation, in particular, was more prevalent in high-risk infants compared with low-risk infants (P = 0.01), especially on a no breast milk diet (P = 0.002). High-risk infants who completed weaning earlier than 6 months showed greater prevalence of constipation (P = 0.001) and abdominal distress (P = 0.004) than those fully weaned after 6 months. CONCLUSIONS: The greater prevalence of GI symptoms in high-risk infants suggests that GI dysfunction during early infant development may be a part of the ASD endophenotype. Late weaning and exclusive breast milk were associated with protection against GI symptoms in high-risk infants.


Subject(s)
Autism Spectrum Disorder , Autistic Disorder , Breast Feeding , Constipation/prevention & control , Diet , Milk, Human , Weaning , Adult , Autism Spectrum Disorder/complications , Autistic Disorder/complications , Child, Preschool , Constipation/complications , Gastrointestinal Diseases/complications , Gastrointestinal Diseases/prevention & control , Humans , Infant , Middle Aged , Phenotype , Surveys and Questionnaires , Young Adult
4.
J Pediatr Gastroenterol Nutr ; 59(3): 365-73, 2014 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24840512

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVES: Fat is digested in the intestine into free fatty acids (FFAs), which are detergents and therefore toxic to cells at micromolar concentration. The mucosal barrier protects cells in the adult intestine, but this barrier may not be fully developed in premature infants. Lipase-digested infant formula, but not fresh human milk, has elevated FFAs and is cytotoxic to intestinal cells, and therefore could contribute to intestinal injury in necrotizing enterocolitis (NEC), but even infants exclusively fed breast milk may develop NEC. Our objective was to determine whether stored milk and milk from donor milk (DM) banks could also become cytotoxic, especially after digestion. METHODS: We exposed cultured rat intestinal epithelial cells or human neutrophils to DM and milk collected fresh and stored at 4°C or -20°C for up to 12 weeks and then treated for 2 hours (37°C) with 0.1 or 1 mg/mL pancreatic lipase and/or trypsin and chymotrypsin. RESULTS: DM and milk stored 3 days (at 4°C or -20°C) and then digested were cytotoxic. Storage at -20°C for 8 and 12 weeks resulted in an additional increase in cytotoxicity. Protease digestion decreased, but did not eliminate cell death. CONCLUSIONS: Present storage practices may allow milk to become cytotoxic and contribute to intestinal damage in NEC.


Subject(s)
Digestion , Fatty Acids, Nonesterified/metabolism , Food Storage , Lipase/metabolism , Milk, Human/metabolism , Animals , Cell Death/drug effects , Cells, Cultured , Chymotrypsin/metabolism , Epithelial Cells , Fatty Acids, Nonesterified/pharmacology , Humans , Intestinal Mucosa/cytology , Milk Banks , Milk, Human/chemistry , Neutrophils , Rats , Temperature , Time Factors , Trypsin/metabolism
5.
J Vis ; 14(1)2014 Jan 02.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24385345

ABSTRACT

To investigate the mechanisms underlying development of upright face preferences in infants, the current study measured inversion effects for faces that were spatial frequency (SF) filtered, into low SF and high SF, with the notion that different SFs are analyzed by different visual mechanisms. For comparison to faces, we used object stimuli that consisted of pictures of strollers. In 4 month olds, 8 month olds, and adults, we measured the strength of the selective face inversion effect (sFIE), operationally defined as an upright over inverted looking preference that is greater for faces than objects. In Study 1, we employed unfiltered stimuli, and found a clear sFIE in both infants and adults. To determine what drove this sFIE, in Study 2, the sFIE was measured for low-SF and high-SF stimuli, with all stimuli being equated for visibility. For adults, the sFIE was equally strong for low-SF and high-SF stimuli. A different pattern was seen for infants. Infants exhibited a significantly greater sFIE for high-SF, than for low-SF, stimuli (and only for high SF was the sFIE significant). In fact, the strength of infants' upright face preference for high-SF stimuli was indistinguishable from that observed for unfiltered faces, indicating that in natural (unfiltered) stimuli, high SFs are sufficient to account for infants' upright face preferences.


Subject(s)
Face , Form Perception/physiology , Orientation/physiology , Space Perception/physiology , Contrast Sensitivity/physiology , Female , Humans , Infant , Male , Sensory Thresholds/physiology , Young Adult
6.
J Autism Dev Disord ; 44(5): 1039-54, 2014 May.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24150884

ABSTRACT

The current study tested fine discrimination of upright and inverted faces and objects in adolescents with Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) as compared to age- and IQ-matched controls. Discrimination sensitivity was tested using morphed faces and morphed objects, and all stimuli were equated in low-level visual characteristics (luminance, contrast, spatial frequency make-up). Participants with ASD exhibited slight, non-significant impairments in discrimination sensitivity for faces, yet significantly enhanced discrimination sensitivity for objects. The ASD group also showed a protracted development of face and object inversion effects. Finally, for ASD participants, face sensitivity improved with increasing IQ while object sensitivity improved with age. By contrast, for controls, face sensitivity improved with age, but neither face nor object sensitivity was influenced by IQ. These findings suggest that individuals with ASD follow a qualitatively different path in the development of face and object processing abilities.


Subject(s)
Child Development Disorders, Pervasive/psychology , Face , Intelligence , Pattern Recognition, Visual , Adolescent , Age Factors , Case-Control Studies , Female , Humans , Intelligence Tests , Male
7.
J Vis ; 13(13): 8, 2013 Nov 06.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24198399

ABSTRACT

Although global motion processing is thought to emerge early in infancy, there is debate regarding the age at which it matures to an adult-like level. In the current study, we address the possibility that the apparent age-related improvement in global motion processing might be secondary to age-related increases in the sensitivity of mechanisms (i.e., local motion detectors) that provide input to global motion mechanisms. To address this, we measured global motion processing by obtaining motion coherence thresholds using stimuli that were equally detectable in terms of contrast across all individuals and ages (3-, 4-, 5-, 6-, and 7-month-olds and adults). For infants, we employed a directional eye movement (DEM) technique. For adults, we employed both DEM and a self-report method. First, contrast sensitivity was obtained for a local task, using a stochastic motion display in which all the dots moved coherently. Contrast sensitivity increased significantly between 3 and 7 months, and between infancy and adulthood. Each subject was then tested on the global motion task with the contrast of the dots set to 2.5 × each individual's contrast threshold. Coherence thresholds were obtained by varying the percentage of coherently moving "signal" versus "noise" dots in the stochastic motion display. Results revealed remarkably stable global motion sensitivity between 3 and 7 months of age, as well as between infancy and adulthood. These results suggest that the mechanisms underlying global motion processing develop to an adult-like state very quickly.


Subject(s)
Contrast Sensitivity/physiology , Motion Perception/physiology , Eye Movements , Female , Humans , Infant , Male , Sensory Thresholds , Young Adult
8.
Vis Neurosci ; 30(5-6): 251-62, 2013 Nov.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23906372

ABSTRACT

The current study tested the development of face and object processing in young children (mean age = 5.24 years), adolescents (mean age = 15.8 years), and adults (mean age = 21.1 years) using stimuli that were equated for low-level visual characteristics (luminance, contrast, and spatial frequency make-up) and methods that equate for difficulty across ages. We also tested sensitivity to luminance and chromatic contrast (i.e., thought to be mediated primarily by the subcortical Magnocellular (M) and Parvocellular (P) pathways, respectively) to determine whether age-related improvements in face or object discrimination were driven by age-related changes in the M and/or P pathways. Results showed a selective age-related improvement in face sensitivity and a relationship between age-related increases in face sensitivity and luminance contrast sensitivity. These results add to the mounting evidence that the M pathway may influence face processing.


Subject(s)
Discrimination, Psychological/physiology , Face , Human Development/physiology , Visual Pathways/physiology , Visual Perception/physiology , Adolescent , Adult , Child , Contrast Sensitivity/physiology , Female , Humans , Male , Young Adult
9.
Doc Ophthalmol ; 127(1): 57-68, 2013 Aug.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23744448

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: The current study assessed whether contrast sensitivity is affected in preterm infants with a history of spontaneously regressed retinopathy of prematurity (ROP, Stages 1-3). Specifically, we employed luminance (light/dark) and chromatic (red/green) stimuli, which are mediated by the magnocellular (M) and parvocellular (P) subcortical pathways, respectively. METHODS: Contrast sensitivity (CS) was measured using forced-choice preferential looking testing in 21 infants with a history of ROP and 41 control preterm infants who were born prematurely but did not develop ROP, tested between 8 and 47 weeks (2-11 months) postterm age. Infants were presented with chromatic and luminance drifting sinusoidal gratings, which appeared randomly on the left or right side of the monitor in each trial. The contrast of the stimuli varied across trials and was defined in terms of root mean squared cone contrast for long- and medium-wavelength cones. RESULTS: Between 8 and 25 weeks postterm, ROP infants had significantly worse CS, and there was a trend for greater impairment for luminance than chromatic CS. This delay was not seen at older ages between 26 and 47 weeks postterm. CONCLUSIONS: These findings are consistent with the concept that early maturation of the M pathway is vulnerable to biological insult, as in the case of ROP, to a greater extent than in the P pathway.


Subject(s)
Color Perception/physiology , Contrast Sensitivity/physiology , Retinopathy of Prematurity/physiopathology , Dark Adaptation/physiology , Gestational Age , Humans , Infant , Infant, Newborn , Infant, Premature , Photic Stimulation , Remission, Spontaneous
10.
Brain Cogn ; 82(1): 117-26, 2013 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23455437

ABSTRACT

Previous studies have asked whether visual sensitivity and attentional processing in deaf signers are enhanced or altered as a result of their different sensory experiences during development, i.e., auditory deprivation and exposure to a visual language. In particular, deaf and hearing signers have been shown to exhibit a right visual field/left hemisphere advantage for motion processing, while hearing nonsigners do not. To examine whether this finding extends to other aspects of visual processing, we compared deaf signers and hearing nonsigners on motion, form, and brightness discrimination tasks. Secondly, to examine whether hemispheric lateralities are affected by attention, we employed a dual-task paradigm to measure form and motion thresholds under "full" vs. "poor" attention conditions. Deaf signers, but not hearing nonsigners, exhibited a right visual field advantage for motion processing. This effect was also seen for form processing and not for the brightness task. Moreover, no group differences were observed in attentional effects, and the motion and form visual field asymmetries were not modulated by attention, suggesting they occur at early levels of sensory processing. In sum, the results show that processing of motion and form, believed to be mediated by dorsal and ventral visual pathways, respectively, are left-hemisphere dominant in deaf signers.


Subject(s)
Attention/physiology , Deafness/physiopathology , Discrimination, Psychological/physiology , Functional Laterality/physiology , Motion Perception/physiology , Orientation/physiology , Visual Perception/physiology , Adult , Female , Humans , Male , Persons With Hearing Impairments/psychology , Photic Stimulation , Sign Language
11.
Vision Res ; 82: 31-41, 2013 Apr 19.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23485427

ABSTRACT

In order to investigate the effects of visual experience on early visual development, the current study compared contrast sensitivity across infants born with different degrees of moderate-to-late prematurity. Here the logic is that at any given postterm age, the most premature infants will have the oldest postnatal age. Given that postnatal age is a proxy for visual experience, the visual experience hypothesis predicts that infants who are more premature, yet healthy, should have higher sensitivity. Luminance (light/dark) and chromatic (red/green) contrast sensitivities (CS) were measured in 236 healthy infants (born -10 to +2 weeks relative to due date) between 5 and 32 weeks postterm age from due date and 8-38 weeks postnatal from birth date. For chromatic CS, we found clear evidence that infants who were most premature within our sample had the highest sensitivity. Specifically, 4-10 additional weeks of visual experience, by virtue of being born early, enhanced chromatic CS. For luminance CS, similar but weaker results were seen. Here, only infants with an additional 6-10 weeks of visual experience, and only at later age points in development, showed enhanced sensitivity. However, CS in preterm infants was still below that of fullterm infants with equivalent postnatal age. In sum, these results suggest that chromatic CS is influenced more by prematurity (and possibly visual experience) than luminance CS, which has implications for differential development of parvocellular and magnocellular pathways.


Subject(s)
Contrast Sensitivity/physiology , Infant, Premature/physiology , Premature Birth/physiopathology , Child, Preschool , Female , Gestational Age , Humans , Infant , Infant, Newborn , Male , Photic Stimulation/methods , Regression Analysis
12.
Pediatr Res ; 72(6): 560-7, 2012 Dec.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23007028

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: Premature infants fed formula are more likely to develop necrotizing enterocolitis (NEC) than those who are breastfed, but the mechanisms of intestinal necrosis in NEC and protection by breast milk are unknown. We hypothesized that after lipase digestion, formula, but not fresh breast milk, contains levels of unbound free fatty acids (FFAs) that are cytotoxic to intestinal cells. METHODS: We digested multiple term and preterm infant formulas or human milk with pancreatic lipase, proteases (trypsin and chymotrypsin), lipase + proteases, or luminal fluid from a rat small intestine and tested FFA levels and cytotoxicity in vitro on intestinal epithelial cells, endothelial cells, and neutrophils. RESULTS: Lipase digestion of formula, but not milk, caused significant death of neutrophils (ranging from 47 to 99% with formulas vs. 6% with milk) with similar results in endothelial and epithelial cells. FFAs were significantly elevated in digested formula vs. milk and death from formula was significantly decreased with lipase inhibitor pretreatment, or treatments to bind FFAs. Protease digestion significantly increased FFA binding capacity of formula and milk but only enough to decrease cytotoxicity from milk. CONCLUSION: FFA-induced cytotoxicity may contribute to the pathogenesis of NEC.


Subject(s)
Cell Death , Enterocolitis, Necrotizing/etiology , Infant Food , Milk, Human , Animals , Cattle , Enterocolitis, Necrotizing/pathology , Humans , In Vitro Techniques , Infant, Newborn , Infant, Premature
13.
J Autism Dev Disord ; 42(12): 2611-21, 2012 Dec.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22456817

ABSTRACT

Social referencing was investigated in 18-month-old siblings of children with autism spectrum disorders (ASD; "high-risk infants"). Infants were exposed to novel toys, which were emotionally tagged via adults' facial and vocal signals. Infants' information seeking (initiation of joint attention with an adult) and their approach/withdrawal behavior toward the toys before versus after the adults' emotional signals was measured. Compared to both typically developing infants and high-risk infants without ASD, infants later diagnosed with ASD engaged in slower information seeking, suggesting that this aspect of referencing may be an early indicator of ASD. High-risk infants, both those who were and those who were not later diagnosed with ASD, exhibited impairments in regulating their behavior based on the adults' emotional signals, suggesting that this aspect of social referencing may reflect an endophenotype for ASD.


Subject(s)
Child Development Disorders, Pervasive/psychology , Child Development , Infant Behavior/psychology , Siblings/psychology , Social Behavior , Attention , Child , Child Development Disorders, Pervasive/genetics , Child, Preschool , Emotions , Endophenotypes , Female , Humans , Infant , Male , Risk Factors
14.
Psychophysiology ; 49(2): 198-206, 2012 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22092174

ABSTRACT

Visual motion processing is compromised in schizophrenia (SZ), but it is uncertain what neural deviations account for their motion analysis abnormalities. Neural activations were measured with dense-array electroencephalography while 14 medicated SZ and 14 healthy persons performed a paired-stimuli forced choice speed discrimination task. SZ had (a) worse-at-speed discrimination, replicating previous findings, (b) normal early extrastriate neural activity (N1) to both motion stimuli, (c) reduced later extrastriate activity (P2) specifically to the second stimulus, and (d) following P2, an enhanced later N2 over parietal cortex. Stronger P2 and N2 responses were associated with better speed discrimination performance across groups. These findings indicate that the neural correlates of poor motion analysis in SZ may not be an early visual analysis abnormality but a problem with efficient use of speed information later in cognitive processing.


Subject(s)
Discrimination, Psychological/physiology , Evoked Potentials, Visual/physiology , Motion Perception/physiology , Parietal Lobe/physiology , Schizophrenia/physiopathology , Adult , Case-Control Studies , Electroencephalography , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged
15.
Vision Res ; 52(1): 11-9, 2012 Jan 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22051893

ABSTRACT

In order to investigate differences in the effects of spatial attention between the left visual field (LVF) and the right visual field (RVF), we employed a full/poor attention paradigm using stimuli presented in the LVF vs. RVF. In addition, to investigate differences in the effects of spatial attention between the dorsal and ventral processing streams, we obtained motion thresholds (motion coherence thresholds and fine direction discrimination thresholds) and orientation thresholds, respectively. The results of this study showed negligible effects of attention on the orientation task, in either the LVF or RVF. In contrast, for both motion tasks, there was a significant effect of attention in the LVF, but not in the RVF. These data provide psychophysical evidence for greater effects of spatial attention in the LVF/right hemisphere, specifically, for motion processing in the dorsal stream.


Subject(s)
Attention/physiology , Discrimination, Psychological/physiology , Motion Perception/physiology , Space Perception/physiology , Visual Fields/physiology , Analysis of Variance , Dominance, Ocular/physiology , Female , Humans , Male , Orientation/physiology , Psychometrics , Sensory Thresholds/physiology , Young Adult
16.
Psychol Sci ; 22(8): 1067-72, 2011 Aug.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21771964

ABSTRACT

Early development is characterized by a period of exuberant neural connectivity followed by a retraction and reweighting of connections over the course of development. It has been proposed that this connectivity may facilitate arbitrary sensory experiences in infants that are unlike anything experienced by typical adults but are similar to the sensory experiences of adults with synaesthesia, a rare sensory phenomenon that has been associated with exuberant neural connectivity and that is characterized by strong arbitrary associations between different sensations. We provide the first evidence for this infant-synaesthesia hypothesis by showing that the presence of particular shapes influences color preferences in typical 2- and 3-month-olds, but not in 8-month-olds or adults. These results are consistent with the possibility that exuberant neural connectivity facilitates synaesthetic associations during infancy that are typically eliminated during development, but that a failure of the retraction process leads in rare cases to synaesthesia in adults.


Subject(s)
Child Development , Sensation , Adolescent , Age Factors , Brain/growth & development , Color Perception/physiology , Form Perception/physiology , Humans , Infant , Photic Stimulation , Psychology, Child , Young Adult
17.
Cereb Cortex ; 20(7): 1749-55, 2010 Jul.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19915096

ABSTRACT

The results of multiple investigations indicate visual motion-processing abnormalities in schizophrenia. There is little information, however, about the time course and neural correlates of motion-processing abnormalities among these subjects. For the present study, 13 schizophrenia and 13 healthy subjects performed a simple motion direction discrimination task with peripherally presented moving grating stimuli (5 or 10 deg/s). Dense-array electroencephalography data were collected simultaneously. The goal was to discern whether neural deviations associated with motion-processing abnormalities among schizophrenia patients occur early or late in the visual-processing stream. Schizophrenia patients were worse at judging the direction of motion gratings, had enhanced early neural activity (about 90 ms after stimulus onset), and deficient target detection-related late neural activity over parietal cortex (about 400 ms after stimulus onset). In addition, there was a strong association (accounting for 36% of performance variance) between poor behavioral performance and lower target detection-related brain activity among schizophrenia patients. These findings suggest that abnormalities in later stages of motion-processing mechanisms, perhaps beyond extrastriate cortex, may account for behavioral deviations among schizophrenia subjects.


Subject(s)
Brain Mapping , Discrimination, Psychological/physiology , Motion Perception/physiology , Parietal Lobe/physiopathology , Schizophrenia/pathology , Adult , Electroencephalography/methods , Evoked Potentials, Visual/physiology , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Neuropsychological Tests , Orientation/physiology , Photic Stimulation/methods , Reaction Time/physiology , Schizophrenia/physiopathology , Statistics as Topic , Time Factors
18.
J Vis ; 9(10): 19.1-21, 2009 Sep 30.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19810800

ABSTRACT

To investigate effects of visual experience versus preprogrammed mechanisms on visual development, we used multiple regression analysis to determine the extent to which a variety of variables (that differ in the extent to which they are tied to visual experience) predict luminance and chromatic (red/green) contrast sensitivity (CS), which are mediated by the magnocellular (M) and parvocellular (P) subcortical pathways, respectively. Our variables included gestational length (GL), birth weight (BW), gender, postnatal age (PNA), and birth order (BO). Two-month-olds (n = 60) and 6-month-olds (n = 122) were tested. Results revealed that (1) at 2 months, infants with longer GL have higher luminance CS; (2) at both ages, CS significantly increases over a approximately 21-day range of PNA, but this effect is stronger in 2- than 6-month-olds and stronger for chromatic than luminance CS; (3) at 2 months, boys have higher luminance CS than girls; and (4) at 2 months, firstborn infants have higher CS, while at 6 months, non-firstborn infants have higher CS. The results for PNA/GL are consistent with the possibility that P pathway development is more influenced by variables tied to visual experience (PNA), while M pathway development is more influenced by variables unrelated to visual experience (GL). Other variables, including prenatal environment, are also discussed.


Subject(s)
Age Factors , Birth Order , Contrast Sensitivity/physiology , Gender Identity , Gestational Age , Analysis of Variance , Color Perception/physiology , Female , Humans , Infant , Light , Male , Regression Analysis
19.
Biol Psychiatry ; 66(10): 950-7, 2009 Nov 15.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19765688

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: Previous studies have documented atypicalities in face/object processing in children and adults with autism spectrum disorders (ASDs). To investigate whether such atypicalities may reflect a genetically mediated risk factor present early in development, we measured face/object processing in 10-month-old high-risk infants who carry some of the genes associated with ASD because they have an older sibling diagnosed with the disorder. METHODS: We employed event-related potentials (ERPs) to measure cortical responses to pictures of faces and objects, the objects being toys. Latencies and amplitudes of four ERP components (P100, N290, P400, and Nc) were compared between 20 high-risk infants and 20 low-risk control subjects (infants with no family history of ASD). RESULTS: Responses to faces versus objects differed between high- and low-risk infants for the latencies of the N290 and P400. Differences were driven by faster responses to faces than objects in low-risk, but not high-risk, infants (P400) and, conversely, faster responses to objects than faces in high-risk, but not low-risk, infants (N290). Object responses were also faster in high-risk than low-risk infants (both N290 and P400). Left versus right hemisphere responses also differed between high- and low-risk infants for the amplitudes of the P100, N290, and P400; collapsed across faces/objects, low-risk, but not high-risk, infants exhibited hemisphere asymmetries. CONCLUSIONS: Genetic risk for ASD is associated with atypical face versus object processing and an atypical lack of hemispheric asymmetry early in life. These atypicalities might contribute to development of the disorder.


Subject(s)
Autistic Disorder/physiopathology , Brain/physiopathology , Face , Functional Laterality/physiology , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Analysis of Variance , Autistic Disorder/pathology , Brain Mapping , Cerebral Cortex/physiopathology , Child , Child, Preschool , Electroencephalography/methods , Evoked Potentials/physiology , Female , Fourier Analysis , Humans , Infant , Male , Neuropsychological Tests , Photic Stimulation/methods , Reaction Time/physiology , Risk
20.
Optom Vis Sci ; 86(6): E583-8, 2009 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19417708

ABSTRACT

Early in postnatal development, the brain produces exuberant connections, some of which are later retracted, a process that is thought to play a role in the formation of functionally segregated modules in the brain. In the case of visual development, retraction between visual areas might underlie the known psychophysical and neural segregation of processing for different aspects of vision (e.g., color, motion, form, depth) known to exist in adults. This review covers the psychophysical evidence for increasing dissociation between visual modules over the course of development, and provides insight into the possible functions of this developmental alteration.


Subject(s)
Child Development , Vision, Ocular/physiology , Child, Preschool , Color Perception/physiology , Form Perception/physiology , Humans , Infant , Motion Perception/physiology , Psychophysics
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...