Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 20 de 45
Filter
1.
J Vis ; 24(7): 5, 2024 Jul 02.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38975946

ABSTRACT

Participants judged affective cooler/warmer gradients around a 12-step color circle. Each pair of adjacent colors was presented twice (left-right reversed), all in random order. Participants readily performed the task, but their settings do not correlate very well. Individual responses were compared with a small number of canonical templates. For a little less than one-half of the participants responses or judgements correlate with such a template. We find a warm pole (in the orange environment) and a cool pole (in the teal environment) connected with two tracks that tend to have one or more gaps or weak, even inverted links. We conclude that the common artistic cool-warm polarity is only weakly reflected in responses of our observers. If it does, the observers apparently use categorical warm and cool poles and may be uncertain in relating adjacent hue steps along the 12-step color circle.


Subject(s)
Color Perception , Photic Stimulation , Humans , Color Perception/physiology , Photic Stimulation/methods , Male , Adult , Female , Young Adult , Judgment/physiology
2.
J Vis ; 23(13): 8, 2023 Nov 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37971768

ABSTRACT

Still-life painters, especially of the so-called Golden Age (17th century) in the Netherlands, are famous for their masterful techniques of rendering reality. Their amazing abilities to depict different material properties of fruits and flowers are stunning. But how important are these careful arrangements of different objects for the perception of an individual item? Is the perceived color saturation of a single fruit influenced by its surrounding context? We selected fruits in still-life paintings as stimuli to investigate whether and how perceived saturations of fruits were affected by their original contexts. In our study, we focused especially on effects of five context properties: complementary colors, chromatic and luminance contrast, object overlap, and surround variance. Six fruit varieties depicted in high-quality digital reproductions of 48 classic and eight varieties in 64 more recent, modern still-life paintings were selected. In a single trial, eight images of fruits of the same variety appeared on a neutral gray background; half were single fruit cutouts, and the other half were the same fruits embedded in their circular contexts. Fifteen participants ranked all eight images according to perceived color saturations of the fruits. Saturation ratings showed a high agreement of 77%. Surrounding contexts led to an increase in perceived saturation of central fruits. This effect was mainly driven by object overlap, the presence of the central fruit type also in the context, and surround variance. Chroma contrast between fruits and contexts decreased saturation significantly. No significant context effects were found for complementary colors or luminance contrast. Our results show that in paintings, many of the cues that are usually experimentally isolated occur in interesting combinations and lead to an increase in perceived saturation that makes fruit objects more appealing and convincing.


Subject(s)
Color Perception , Fruit , Humans , Light , Color , Physical Phenomena
3.
Sci Rep ; 13(1): 13936, 2023 08 25.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37626103

ABSTRACT

Adolescents are at increased risk to develop substance use disorders and suffer from relapse throughout life. Targeted weakening of drug-associated memories has been shown to reduce relapse-like behavior in adult rats, however this process has been understudied in adolescents. We aimed to examine whether adolescent-formed, cocaine-associated memories could be manipulated via reconsolidation mechanisms. To accomplish this objective, we used an abbreviated operant cocaine self-administration paradigm (ABRV Coc-SA). Adult and adolescent rats received jugular catheterization surgery followed by ABRV Coc-SA in a distinct context for 2 h, 2×/day over 5 days. Extinction training (EXT) occurred in a second context for 2 h, 2×/day over 4 days. To retrieve cocaine-context memories, rats were exposed to the cocaine-paired context for 15 min, followed by subcutaneous injection of vehicle or the protein synthesis inhibitor cycloheximide (2.5 mg/kg). Two additional EXT sessions were conducted before a 2 h reinstatement test in the cocaine-paired context to assess cocaine-seeking behavior. We find that both adult and adolescent cocaine-exposed rats show similar levels of cocaine-seeking behavior regardless of post-reactivation treatment. Our results suggest that systemic treatment with the protein synthesis inhibitor cycloheximide does not impair reconsolidation of cocaine-context memories and subsequent relapse during adulthood or adolescence.


Subject(s)
Catheterization, Central Venous , Cocaine , Animals , Rats , Cycloheximide , Protein Synthesis Inhibitors , Administration, Cutaneous , Cocaine/pharmacology
4.
Neurobiol Learn Mem ; 199: 107722, 2023 03.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36639018

ABSTRACT

RATIONALE: Drug use during adolescence results in a life-long risk to develop substance-use disorders. Adolescent rats are sensitive to different drug-associated cues, compared to adults; however, the contribution of adolescent-formed context-drug-associations to elicit relapse-like behavior is underexplored. OBJECTIVES: The present study compared the effect of adolescent vs adult-formed context-drug associations to elicit time-dependent increases in cocaine-seeking behavior. This objective was accomplished using an abbreviated (ABRV) operant cocaine self-administration (Coc-SA), Extinction (EXT) paradigm, with cocaine-seeking tests occurring 1 day after training (T1, early relapse) or following 15 days of abstinence (T15, late relapse). METHODS: Adolescent and adult rats received ABRV Coc-SA in a distinct context (2 hr, 2x/day over 5 days) then EXT in a second context (2 hr, 2x/day over 4 days). Adolescent or adult cocaine-exposed rats were then tested (2 hr, non-rewarded) in either the previous EXT or Coc-paired contexts during early or late relapse. RESULTS & CONCLUSIONS: As previously reported, both adolescent and adult cocaine-exposed rats displayed similar magnitudes of cocaine intake and lever presses during Coc-SA, EXT, and early relapse. Independent analysis of adolescent and adult groups revealed differences in lever responding, specifically rats with cocaine exposure during adolescence showed time-dependent increases in lever responding during late relapse. These data suggest that cocaine-context associations formed during adolescence can elicit craving during adulthood and that these age-specific differences in contextual sensitivity may not be immediately observed at early relapse periods.


Subject(s)
Cocaine-Related Disorders , Cocaine , Rats , Animals , Male , Rats, Sprague-Dawley , Pharmaceutical Preparations , Drug-Seeking Behavior , Self Administration , Cues , Recurrence , Extinction, Psychological , Conditioning, Operant
5.
J Appl Biomech ; 37(6): 585-586, 2021 Dec 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34872069

ABSTRACT

As the first PhD graduate of the Biomechanics Laboratory at the Pennsylvania State University under the leadership of Dr. Richard C. Nelson, I reflect on my early experience in sport biomechanics there and its influence on some of my subsequent, and typically unpublished, research challenges.


Subject(s)
Sports , Biomechanical Phenomena , Humans
6.
Curr Biol ; 31(16): R991-R992, 2021 08 23.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34428418

ABSTRACT

Eye movements are an integral part of human visual perception. They allow us to have a small foveal region with exquisite acuity and at the same time a large visual field. For a long time, eye movements were regarded as machine-like behaviors in response to visual stimulation1, but over the past few decades it has been convincingly shown that expectations, intended actions, rewards and many other cognitive factors can have profound effects on the way we move our eyes2-4. In order to be useful, our oculomotor system must minimize delay with respect to the dynamic events in the visual scene. The ability to do so has been demonstrated in situations where we are in control of these events, for example when we are making a sandwich or tea5, and when we are active participants, for example when hitting a cricket ball6. But what about scenes with complex dynamics that we do not control or directly take part in, like a hockey game we are watching as a spectator? A semantic influence on gaze fixation location during viewing of tennis videos has been suggested before7. Here we use carefully annotated hockey videos to show that the brain is indeed able to exploit the semantic context of the game to anticipate the continuous motion of the puck, leading to eye movements that are fundamentally different than when following exactly the same motion without any context.


Subject(s)
Cues , Eye Movements , Hockey , Fixation, Ocular , Humans , Visual Perception
7.
J Vis ; 21(6): 11, 2021 06 07.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34144606

ABSTRACT

Saccadic eye movements modulate visual perception: they initiate and terminate high acuity vision at a certain location in space, but before and during their execution visual contrast sensitivity is strongly attenuated for 100 to 200 ms. Transient perisaccadic perceptual distortions are assumed to be an important mechanism to maintain visual stability. Little is known about age effects on saccadic suppression, even though for healthy adults other major age-related changes are well documented, like a decrease of visual contrast sensitivity for intermediate and high spatial frequencies or an increase of saccade latencies. Here, we tested saccadic suppression of luminance and isoluminant chromatic flashes in 100 participants from eight to 78 years. To estimate the effect of saccadic suppression on contrast sensitivity, we used a two-alternative forced choice (2AFC) design and an adaptive staircase procedure to modulate the luminance or chromatic contrast of a flashed detection target during fixation and 15 ms after saccade onset. The target was a single horizontal luminance or chromatic line flashed 2° above or below the fixation or saccade target. Compared to fixation, average perisaccadic contrast sensitivity decreased significantly by 66% for luminance and by 36% for color. A significant correlation was found for the strength of saccadic suppression of luminance and color. However, a small age effect was found only for the strength of saccadic suppression of luminance, which increased from 64% to 70% from young to old age. We conclude that saccadic suppression for luminance and color is present in most participants independent of their age and that mechanisms of suppression stay relatively stable during healthy aging.


Subject(s)
Saccades , Visual Perception , Adult , Contrast Sensitivity , Humans , Photic Stimulation , Vision, Ocular
9.
J Vis ; 20(8): 26, 2020 08 03.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32845961

ABSTRACT

Research on eye movements has primarily been performed in two distinct ways: (1) under highly controlled conditions using simple stimuli such as dots on a uniform background, or (2) under free-viewing conditions with complex images, real-world movies, or even with observers moving around in the world. Although both approaches offer important insights, the generalizability among eye movement behaviors observed under these different conditions is unclear. Here, we compared eye movement responses to video clips showing moving objects within their natural context with responses to simple Gaussian blobs on a blank screen. Importantly, for both conditions, the targets moved along the same trajectories at the same speed. We measured standard oculometric measures for both stimulus complexities, as well as the effect of the relative angle between saccades and pursuit, and compared them across conditions. In general, eye movement responses were qualitatively similar, especially with respect to pursuit gain. For both types of stimuli, the accuracy of saccades and subsequent pursuit was highest when both eye movements were collinear. We also found interesting differences; for example, latencies of initial saccades to moving Gaussian blob targets were significantly faster compared to saccades to moving objects in video scenes, whereas pursuit accuracy was significantly higher in video scenes. These findings suggest a lower processing demand for simple target conditions during saccade preparation and an advantage for tracking behavior in natural scenes due to higher predictability provided by the context information.


Subject(s)
Motion Perception/physiology , Motion Pictures , Oculomotor Nerve/physiology , Pursuit, Smooth/physiology , Saccades/physiology , Adult , Female , Humans , Male , Normal Distribution , Video Recording , Young Adult
10.
Psychopharmacology (Berl) ; 237(9): 2823-2833, 2020 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32601989

ABSTRACT

RATIONALE: Drug use during adolescence results in a lifelong risk to develop substance-use disorders. Adolescent rats are less reactive to cocaine-associated cues compared with adults; however, the contribution of adolescent-formed, context-drug-associations to elicit relapse-like behavior is underexplored. Although it is known that social isolation can impact drug-seeking behavior, the effects of housing conditions on context-induced, cocaine-seeking during adolescence vs adulthood are unknown. OBJECTIVES: The present study compared the effect of adolescent vs adult-formed context-drug associations under different housing conditions (pair vs single) on cocaine-seeking behavior during adolescence or adulthood. This objective was accomplished using operant cocaine self-administration (Coc-SA) under a standard, non-abbreviated (Non-ABRV) or modified abbreviated (ABRV) paradigm. METHODS: In experiment 1, adolescent and adult rats received Non-ABRV Coc-SA in a distinct context (2 h, 1×/day, 10 days), and extinction training (EXT) in a second context (1 h, 1×/day, 8 days) with reinstatement test (TEST) during adulthood in the cocaine-paired context. In experiments 2 and 3, rats received all behavioral phases during adolescence or adulthood: ABRV Coc-SA (2 h, 2×/day, 5 days), EXT (1 h, 4×/day, 2 days) with TEST in a cocaine-paired or novel, unpaired context. All experiments included pair and single-housing conditions. RESULTS AND CONCLUSIONS: Age at cocaine exposure did not influence behavior in Non-ABRV or ABRV paradigms. Under Non-ABRV conditions, adolescent and adult single-housed rats had higher seeking behavior than pair housed. These data suggest that social isolation influences context-induced, cocaine-seeking regardless of age at drug exposure and provides a condensed, ABRV paradigm to investigate context-induced, cocaine-seeking behavior during adolescence.


Subject(s)
Cocaine-Related Disorders/psychology , Cocaine/administration & dosage , Cues , Drug-Seeking Behavior/drug effects , Extinction, Psychological/drug effects , Social Isolation/psychology , Age Factors , Animals , Dopamine Uptake Inhibitors/administration & dosage , Drug-Seeking Behavior/physiology , Extinction, Psychological/physiology , Male , Rats , Rats, Sprague-Dawley , Recurrence , Self Administration
11.
Iperception ; 10(5): 2041669519867973, 2019.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31565211

ABSTRACT

We investigated in "art-naïve" German and Chinese participants the perception of color and spatial balance in abstract art. For color perception, we asked participants (a) to adjust the color of a single element in 24 paintings according to their liking and (b) to indicate whether they preferred their version of the painting or the original. For spatial perception, we asked participants (a) to determine the "balance point" of an artwork and (b) to indicate their preferences for the original or left-right reversed orientation of previously seen and unfamiliar paintings. Results of the color experiments suggest that, even though the interactive task was of a rather open-ended nature, observers' color adjustments were not random but systematically influenced by each painting's color palette. Overall, participants liked their own color choices about as much as the original composition. Results of the spatial experiments reveal a remarkable consistency between participants in their balance point settings. The perceived lateral position of the balance point systematically affected the left-right orientation preference for a given painting. We conclude that "art-naïve" observers are sensitive to the composition of colors and spatial structures in abstract art and are influenced by their cultural backgrounds when experiencing abstract paintings.

12.
J Vis ; 19(7): 2, 2019 07 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31287856

ABSTRACT

To accurately foveate a moving target, the oculomotor system needs to estimate the position of the target at the saccade end, based on information about its position and ongoing movement, while accounting for neuronal delays and execution time of the saccade. We investigated human interceptive saccades and pursuit responses to moving targets defined by high and low luminance contrast or by chromatic contrast only (isoluminance). We used step-ramps with perpendicular directions between vertical target steps of 10 deg/s and horizontal ramps of 2.5 to 20 deg/s to separate errors with respect to the position step of the target in the vertical dimension, and errors related to target motion in the horizontal dimension. Interceptive saccades to targets of high and low luminance contrast landed close to the actual target positions, suggesting relatively accurate estimates of the amount of target displacement. Interceptive saccades to isoluminant targets were less accurate. They landed at positions the target had on average 100 ms before saccade onset. One account of this finding is that the integration of target motion is compromised for isoluminant targets moving in the periphery. In this case, the oculomotor system can use an accurate, but delayed position component, but cannot account for target movement. This deficit was also present for the postsaccadic pursuit speed. For the two luminance conditions, pursuit direction and speed were adjusted depending on the saccadic landing position. The rapid postsaccadic pursuit adjustments suggest shared position- and motion-related signals of target and eye for saccade and pursuit control.


Subject(s)
Motion Perception/physiology , Pursuit, Smooth/physiology , Saccades/physiology , Adult , Color , Contrast Sensitivity/physiology , Female , Humans , Male , Motion , Movement/physiology , Photic Stimulation , Space Perception/physiology
13.
Behav Neurosci ; 133(5): 489-495, 2019 Oct.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31058522

ABSTRACT

Cocaine-use disorders are characterized by repeated relapse to drug-seeking and drug-taking behavior following periods of abstinence. Former drug users display increased activation of the orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) in response to drug-related cues, and similar phenomena are also observed in rodent models of drug relapse. The lateral, but not medial, OFC functionally contributes to the maintenance of cue-drug associations; however, less is known about the role of the ventral OFC in this process. To examine the pattern of neuronal activation in OFC subregions in response to drug-associated cues, rats were trained to respond on a lever for a cocaine infusion paired with a complex cue (2-hr sessions, minimum 10 days). Cocaine self-administration was followed by extinction training, in which lever responses resulted in no consequences (2-hr sessions, minimum 7 days). During a 1-hr reinstatement test, drug-seeking behavior (i.e., responses on the drug-paired lever) was examined in the presence or absence of contingent drug-paired cues (Cue TEST vs. Ext TEST, respectively). Rats were overdosed with a ketamine + xylazine cocktail 30-min post session, and transcardially perfused with 4% paraformaldehyde. Cfos protein expression was utilized to measure potential changes in neural activation between the reinstatement test groups. An increase in the number of Cfos-Immunoreactive cells was observed in the ventral and lateral subregions of the OFC in the Cue TEST group. The present findings provide evidence that the ventral and lateral regions of the rat OFC display similar patterns of neuronal activation in response to cocaine-paired cues. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2019 APA, all rights reserved).


Subject(s)
Drug-Seeking Behavior/drug effects , Drug-Seeking Behavior/physiology , Prefrontal Cortex/drug effects , Animals , Behavior, Animal/drug effects , Cocaine/pharmacology , Conditioning, Operant/physiology , Cues , Extinction, Psychological/drug effects , Genes, fos/genetics , Male , Neurons/drug effects , Prefrontal Cortex/physiology , Rats , Rats, Sprague-Dawley , Self Administration , Transcriptome/genetics
14.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 115(9): 2240-2245, 2018 02 27.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29440494

ABSTRACT

Due to the foveal organization of our visual system we have to constantly move our eyes to gain precise information about our environment. Doing so massively alters the retinal input. This is problematic for the perception of moving objects, because physical motion and retinal motion become decoupled and the brain has to discount the eye movements to recover the speed of moving objects. Two different types of eye movements, pursuit and saccades, are combined for tracking. We investigated how the way we track moving targets can affect the perceived target speed. We found that the execution of corrective saccades during pursuit initiation modifies how fast the target is perceived compared with pure pursuit. When participants executed a forward (catch-up) saccade they perceived the target to be moving faster. When they executed a backward saccade they perceived the target to be moving more slowly. Variations in pursuit velocity without corrective saccades did not affect perceptual judgments. We present a model for these effects, assuming that the eye velocity signal for small corrective saccades gets integrated with the retinal velocity signal during pursuit. In our model, the execution of corrective saccades modulates the integration of these two signals by giving less weight to the retinal information around the time of corrective saccades.


Subject(s)
Motion Perception , Photic Stimulation , Saccades/physiology , Adult , Brain/physiology , Female , Fovea Centralis , Humans , Male , Motion , Normal Distribution , Reproducibility of Results , Retina/physiology , Young Adult
15.
Fam Community Health ; 41(2): 73-82, 2018.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29461355

ABSTRACT

This study explores how low-income rural Latino children and their mothers differ from their non-Latino white counterparts in terms of health, well-being, and health care access. A subsample of non-Latino white (n = 201) and Latino (n = 157) children and their mothers was drawn from the Rural Families Speak about Health Project, a multistate, cross-sectional data set developed through mixed purposive sampling methods. Findings suggest that Latino children's families were disadvantaged in terms of child health and access to health care, whereas non-Latino white children's families were disadvantaged in terms of child behavior problems and maternal health and depression.


Subject(s)
Health Services Accessibility/trends , Poverty/ethnology , Rural Population/trends , Child , Child, Preschool , Cross-Sectional Studies , Female , Hispanic or Latino , Humans , Male , White People
16.
J Eye Mov Res ; 11(4)2018 Nov 09.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33828708

ABSTRACT

Direct comparison of results of humans and monkeys is often complicated by differences in experimental conditions. We replicated in head unrestrained macaques experiments of a recent study comparing human directional precision during smooth pursuit eye movements (SPEM) and saccades to moving targets (Braun & Gegenfurtner, 2016). Directional precision of human SPEM follows an exponential decay function reaching optimal values of 1.5°-3° within 300 ms after target motion onset, whereas precision of initial saccades to moving targets is slightly better. As in humans, we found general agreement in the devel-opment of directional precision of SPEM over time and in the differences between direc-tional precision of initial saccades and SPEM initiation. However, monkeys showed over-all lower precision in SPEM compared to humans. This was most likely due to differences in experimental conditions, such as in the stabilization of the head, which was by a chin and a head rest in human subjects and unrestrained in monkeys.

17.
Vision Res ; 136: 57-69, 2017 07.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28614726

ABSTRACT

Visual sensitivity is dynamically modulated by eye movements. During saccadic eye movements, sensitivity is reduced selectively for low-spatial frequency luminance stimuli and largely unaffected for high-spatial frequency luminance and chromatic stimuli (Nature 371 (1994), 511-513). During smooth pursuit eye movements, sensitivity for low-spatial frequency luminance stimuli is moderately reduced while sensitivity for chromatic and high-spatial frequency luminance stimuli is even increased (Nature Neuroscience, 11 (2008), 1211-1216). Since these effects are at least partly of different polarity, we investigated the combined effects of saccades and smooth pursuit on visual sensitivity. For the time course of chromatic sensitivity, we found that detection rates increased slightly around pursuit onset. During saccades to static and moving targets, detection rates dropped briefly before the saccade and reached a minimum at saccade onset. This reduction of chromatic sensitivity was present whenever a saccade was executed and it was not modified by subsequent pursuit. We also measured contrast sensitivity for flashed high- and low-spatial frequency luminance and chromatic stimuli during saccades and pursuit. During saccades, the reduction of contrast sensitivity was strongest for low-spatial frequency luminance stimuli (about 90%). However, a significant reduction was also present for chromatic stimuli (about 58%). Chromatic sensitivity was increased during smooth pursuit (about 12%). These results suggest that the modulation of visual sensitivity during saccades and smooth pursuit is more complex than previously assumed.


Subject(s)
Color Perception/physiology , Light , Pursuit, Smooth/physiology , Saccades/physiology , Visual Perception/physiology , Adult , Female , Humans , Male , Photic Stimulation , Young Adult
18.
J Neurophysiol ; 118(3): 1762-1774, 2017 09 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28659462

ABSTRACT

Smooth pursuit and motion perception have mainly been investigated with stimuli moving along linear trajectories. Here we studied the quality of pursuit movements to curved motion trajectories in human observers and examined whether the pursuit responses would be sensitive enough to discriminate various degrees of curvature. In a two-interval forced-choice task subjects pursued a Gaussian blob moving along a curved trajectory and then indicated in which interval the curve was flatter. We also measured discrimination thresholds for the same curvatures during fixation. Motion curvature had some specific effects on smooth pursuit properties: trajectories with larger amounts of curvature elicited lower open-loop acceleration, lower pursuit gain, and larger catch-up saccades compared with less curved trajectories. Initially, target motion curvatures were underestimated; however, ∼300 ms after pursuit onset pursuit responses closely matched the actual curved trajectory. We calculated perceptual thresholds for curvature discrimination, which were on the order of 1.5 degrees of visual angle (°) for a 7.9° curvature standard. Oculometric sensitivity to curvature discrimination based on the whole pursuit trajectory was quite similar to perceptual performance. Oculometric thresholds based on smaller time windows were higher. Thus smooth pursuit can quite accurately follow moving targets with curved trajectories, but temporal integration over longer periods is necessary to reach perceptual thresholds for curvature discrimination.NEW & NOTEWORTHY Even though motion trajectories in the real world are frequently curved, most studies of smooth pursuit and motion perception have investigated linear motion. We show that pursuit initially underestimates the curvature of target motion and is able to reproduce the target curvature ∼300 ms after pursuit onset. Temporal integration of target motion over longer periods is necessary for pursuit to reach the level of precision found in perceptual discrimination of curvature.


Subject(s)
Discrimination, Psychological , Fixation, Ocular , Motion Perception , Pursuit, Smooth , Acceleration , Adult , Female , Humans , Male , Sensory Thresholds
19.
Perspect Psychiatr Care ; 53(4): 350-356, 2017 Oct.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27197547

ABSTRACT

PURPOSE: The purpose of this paper was to identify challenges of families caring for loved ones prior to inpatient psychiatric care, understand families' perceptions of support received from psychiatric health professionals, and identify ways for professionals to support families. CONCLUSIONS: Families experienced many challenges prior to hospital admission. There was a large gap between families' expectations and perceptions of actual support received by psychiatric health professionals. PRACTICE IMPLICATIONS: Nurses are well positioned to educate, advocate, and support overburdened families. Greater knowledge of families' challenges would enable nurses to anticipate and meet their needs.


Subject(s)
Hospitals, Psychiatric , Mental Disorders/therapy , Professional-Family Relations , Humans , Mental Disorders/nursing
20.
J Vis ; 16(13): 4, 2016 10 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27792805

ABSTRACT

Successful foveation of a dynamic target depends on good predictions of its movement direction and speed. We measured and compared the temporal dynamics of directional precision of both saccades and smooth pursuit and their interactions. We also compared the directional precision of both eye movements to psychophysical direction discrimination thresholds. Directional thresholds of pure pursuit responses improved rapidly and reached asymptotic values of 1.5°-3° within 300 ms after target motion onset, both for trained and untrained observers and irrespective of the speed of the stimuli. Psychophysical thresholds were in the same range. Directional thresholds for saccades in the ramp paradigm were just slightly higher, but these occurred significantly earlier in time at around 200 ms after target motion onset. At the equivalent time during pure pursuit initiation, thresholds were typically higher by 2°-3°. The rise in directional precision-or decrease in thresholds-over time was more pronounced for trials with longer latencies. As an effect, precision depended mainly on time since stimulus motion onset rather than pursuit onset. Directional precision for saccades to static targets was slightly better than to moving targets, at even shorter latencies. We conclude that directional precision is higher for the saccadic system at saccade onset than for the pursuit system, presumably due to additional position signals that are not available to the pursuit system at that point in time. The pursuit response improves rapidly due to refined sensory processing and motor planning. The combination of initial saccades and pursuit to track moving targets is a good strategy for the oculomotor system to reduce directional errors during the phase of initiation. The target speed has very little effects on the directional precision of both eye movements.


Subject(s)
Oculomotor Muscles/physiology , Pursuit, Smooth/physiology , Saccades/physiology , Adult , Female , Humans , Male , Motion Perception/physiology , Psychophysics , Young Adult
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...