Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 20 de 21
Filter
Add more filters










Publication year range
1.
Parkinsonism Relat Disord ; 78: 165-177, 2020 09.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32927414

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: In Parkinson's disease (PD), impulsive-compulsive behaviors (ICBs) may develop as side-effect of dopaminergic medications. Abnormal incentive-driven decision-making, which is supported by the cognitive control and motivation interaction, may represent an ICBs signature. This systematic review explored whether structural and/or functional brain differences between PD patients with vs without ICBs encompass incentive-driven decision-making networks. METHODS: Structural and functional neuroimaging studies comparing PD patients with and without ICBs, either de novo or medicated, were included. RESULTS: Thirty articles were identified. No consistent evidence of structural alteration both in de novo and medicated PD patients were found. Differences in connectivity within the default mode, the salience and the central executive networks predate ICBs development and remain stable once ICBs are fully developed. Medicated PD patients with ICBs show increased metabolism and cerebral blood flow in orbitofrontal and cingulate cortices, ventral striatum, amygdala, insula, temporal and supramarginal gyri. Abnormal ventral striatum connectivity with anterior cingulate cortex and limbic structures was reported in PD patients with ICBs. DISCUSSION: Functional brain signatures of ICBs in PD encompass areas involved in cognitive control and motivational encoding networks of the incentive-driven decision-making. Functional alterations predating ICBs may be related to abnormal synaptic plasticity in these networks.


Subject(s)
Decision Making , Disruptive, Impulse Control, and Conduct Disorders , Executive Function , Impulsive Behavior , Motivation , Nerve Net , Neuroimaging , Parkinson Disease , Decision Making/physiology , Disruptive, Impulse Control, and Conduct Disorders/diagnostic imaging , Disruptive, Impulse Control, and Conduct Disorders/etiology , Disruptive, Impulse Control, and Conduct Disorders/pathology , Disruptive, Impulse Control, and Conduct Disorders/physiopathology , Executive Function/physiology , Humans , Impulsive Behavior/physiology , Motivation/physiology , Nerve Net/diagnostic imaging , Nerve Net/pathology , Nerve Net/physiopathology , Parkinson Disease/complications , Parkinson Disease/diagnostic imaging , Parkinson Disease/pathology , Parkinson Disease/physiopathology
2.
Front Neurol ; 9: 1018, 2018.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30568628

ABSTRACT

Background: Around 30% Parkinson's disease (PD) patients develop impulse control disorders (ICDs) to D2/3 dopamine agonists and, to a lesser extent, levodopa. We aim to investigate striatal dopaminergic function in PD patients with and without ICD. Methods: PubMed, Science Direct, EBSCO, and ISI Web of Science databases were searched (from inception to March 7, 2018) to identify PET or SPECT studies reporting striatal dopaminergic function in PD patients with ICD (ICD+) compared to those without ICD (ICD-). Studies which included drug naïve patients, explored non-pharmacological procedures (e.g., deep brain stimulation), and those using brain blood perfusion or non-dopaminergic markers were excluded. Standardized mean difference (SDM) was used and random-effect models were applied. Separate meta-analyses were performed for dopamine transporter level, dopamine release, and dopamine receptors availability in the putamen, caudate, dorsal, and ventral striatum. Results: A total of 238 studies were title and abstract screened, of which 19 full-texts were assessed. Nine studies (ICD+: N = 117; ICD-: N = 175 patients) were included in the analysis. ICD+ showed a significant reduction of dopamine transporter binding in the putamen (SDM = -0.46; 95% CI: -0.80, -0.11; Z = 2.61; p = 0.009), caudate (SDM = -0.38; 95% CI: -0.73, -0.04; Z = 2.18; p = 0.03) and dorsal striatum (SDM = -0.45; 95% CI: -0.77, -0.13; Z = 2.76; p = 0.006), and increased dopamine release to reward-related stimuli/gambling tasks in the ventral striatum (SDM = -1.04; 95% CI: -1.73, -0.35; Z = 2.95; p = 0.003). Dopamine receptors availability did not differ between groups. Heterogeneity was low for dopamine transporter in the dorsal striatum (I 2 = 0%), putamen (I 2 = 0%) and caudate (I 2 = 0%), and pre-synaptic dopamine release in the dorsal (I 2 = 0%) and ventral striatum (I 2 = 0%); heterogeneity was high for dopamine transporter levels in the ventral striatum (I 2 = 80%), and for dopamine receptors availability in the ventral (I 2 = 89%) and dorsal (I 2 = 86%) striatum, putamen (I 2 = 93%), and caudate (I 2 = 71%). Conclusions: ICD+ patients show lower dopaminergic transporter levels in the dorsal striatum and increased dopamine release in the ventral striatum when engaged in reward-related stimuli/gambling tasks. This dopaminergic imbalance might represent a biological substrate for ICD in PD. Adequately powered longitudinal studies with drug naïve patients are needed to understand whether these changes may represent biomarkers of premorbid vulnerability to ICD.

3.
Front Neurol ; 9: 654, 2018.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30233478

ABSTRACT

Background: In Parkinson's disease (PD), impulse control disorders (ICDs) develop as side-effect of dopaminergic replacement therapy (DRT). Cognitive, affective, and motivational correlates of ICD in medicated PD patients are debated. Here, we systematically reviewed and meta-analyzed the evidence for an association between ICD in PD and cognitive, affective, and motivational abnormalities. Methods: A systematic review and meta-analysis was performed on PubMed, Science Direct, ISI Web of Science, Cochrane, EBSCO for studies published between 1-1-2000 and 8-3-2017 comparing cognitive, affective, and motivational measures in PD patients with ICD (ICD+) vs. those without ICD (ICD-). Exclusion criteria were conditions other than PD, substance and/or alcohol abuse, dementia, drug naïve patients, cognition assessed by self-report tools. Standardized mean difference (SMD) was used, and random-effect model applied. Results: 10,200 studies were screened (title, abstract), 79 full-texts were assessed, and 25 were included (ICD+: 625 patients; ICD-: 938). Compared to ICD-, ICD+ showed worse performance reward-related decision-making (0.42 [0.02, 0.82], p = 0.04) and set-shifting tasks (SMD = -0.49 [95% CI -0.78, -0.21], p = 0.0008). ICD in PD was also related to higher self-reported rate of depression (0.35 [0.16, 0.54], p = 0.0004), anxiety (0.43 [0.18, 0.68], p = 0.0007), anhedonia (0.26 [0.01, 0.50], p = 0.04), and impulsivity (0.79 [0.50, 1.09], p < 0.00001). Heterogeneity was low to moderate, except for depression (I2 = 61%) and anxiety (I2 = 58%). Conclusions: ICD in PD is associated with worse set-shifting and reward-related decision-making, and increased depression, anxiety, anhedonia, and impulsivity. This is an important area for further studies as ICDs have negative impact on the quality of life of patients and their caregivers.

4.
J Neural Transm (Vienna) ; 125(2): 131-143, 2018 02.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29119257

ABSTRACT

Impulse control disorders (ICDs) in Parkinson's disease (PD) are considered dopaminergic treatment side effects. Cognitive and affective factors may increase the risk of ICD in PD. The aim is to investigate risky decision-making and associated cognitive processes in PD patients with ICDs within a four-stage conceptual framework. Relationship between ICDs and affective factors was explored. Thirteen PD patients with ICD (ICD+), 12 PD patients without ICD (ICD-), and 17 healthy controls were recruited. Overall risky decision-making and negative feedback effect were examined with the Balloon Analogue Risk Task (BART). A cognitive battery dissected decision-making processes according to the four-stage conceptual framework. Affective and motivational factors were measured. ANOVA showed no effect of group on overall risky decision-making. However, there was a group × feedback interaction [F (2, 39) = 3.31, p = 0.047]. ICD+, unlike ICD- and healthy controls, failed to reduce risky behaviour following negative feedback. A main effect of group was found for anxiety and depression [F(2, 38) = 8.31, p = 0.001], with higher symptoms in ICD+ vs. healthy controls. Groups did not differ in cognitive outcomes or affective and motivational metrics. ICD+ may show relatively preserved cognitive function, but reduced sensitivity to negative feedback during risky decision-making and higher symptoms of depression and anxiety.


Subject(s)
Decision Making/physiology , Disruptive, Impulse Control, and Conduct Disorders/etiology , Parkinson Disease/complications , Parkinson Disease/psychology , Affective Symptoms/etiology , Aged , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Motivation/physiology
5.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28694990

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: The aim was to assess the feasibility of a single-centre, single-blind, randomized, crossover design to explore the effects of two slow-release dopamine agonists, ropinirole and pramipexole, on cued recall in Parkinson's disease. As the design required a switch from the prescribed agonist (pramipexole-to-ropinirole, or ropinirole-to-pramipexole), the primary objectives were to (a) examine the efficacy of processes and procedures used to manage symptoms during the washout period and (b) to use cued recall estimates to inform a power calculation for a definitive trial. Secondary objectives were to assess consent and missing data rates, acceptability of clinical support for the OFF sessions, experience of the OFF sessions and of agonist switching, barriers-to-participation for patients and informal caregivers. METHODS: Patients were randomized in a 1:1 ratio to two treatment arms and stabilized on each agonist for 6 weeks. The arms differed only in the sequence in which the agonists were administered. Cued recall was assessed ON medication and, following a washout period resulting in 93.75% agonist elimination, OFF medication. RESULTS: A total of 220 patients were screened: 145 were excluded and 75 invitations to participate were sent to eligible patients. Fifty-three patients declined, 22 consented and 16 completed the study. There were no serious adverse events, and rates of non-serious adverse events were equivalent between the agonists. Using the largest standard deviation (SD) of the ON-OFF difference cued recall score (inflated by ~25% to give a conservative estimate of the SD in a definitive trial) and assuming an effect of at least 10% of the observed range of OFF medication cued recall scores for either agonist to be clinically important, a main trial requires a sample size of just under 150 patients. The consent and missing data rates were 29 and 27% respectively. The washout period and the preparation for the OFF sessions were acceptable, and the sessions were manageable. The experience of switching was also manageable. Barriers to participation included concerns about disease stability, side effects, research process, carer workload and accessibility of the information sheet. CONCLUSIONS: This study presented challenges to recruitment both in design and execution, and while it was a major aim of the study to assess this, evaluation of these challenges provided the opportunity to explore how they could be overcome for future studies. TRIAL REGISTRATION: EudraCT 2012-000801-64.

6.
Cortex ; 83: 62-77, 2016 10.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27494375

ABSTRACT

Anosognosia for hemiplegia (AHP) is informative about the neurocognitive basis of motor awareness. However, it is frequently associated with concomitant symptoms, such as hemispatial neglect and disturbances in the sense of body ownership (DSO). Although double dissociations between these symptoms have been reported, there is ongoing debate about whether they are manifestations of independent abnormalities, or a single neurocognitive deficit. We aimed to investigate the specificity of lesions associated with AHP by surpassing four, existing methodological limitations: (a) recruit a relatively large sample of patients (total N = 70) in a multi-centre study; (b) identify lesions associated with AHP in grey and white matter using voxel-based methods; (c) take into account the duration of AHP and concomitant neglect symptoms; and (d) compare lesions against a control hemiplegic group, patients suffering from AHP and DSO, and a few, rare patients with selective DSO. Results indicated that acute AHP is associated with a wide network, mainly including: (1) the Rolandic operculum, (2) the insula and (3) the superior temporal gyri. Subcortically, damage mainly involved the basal ganglia and white matter, mostly the superior corona radiate, arcuate fasciculus and the part of the ventral, superior longitudinal fasciculus. Persistent symptoms were linked with wider damage involving fronto-temporal cortex and long white matter tracts. A shift in the latero-medial direction (mainly involving the basal ganglia and surrounding white matter) emerged when DSO was taken accounted for. These results suggest that while bodily awareness is processed by areas widely distributed across the brain, intact subcortical structures and white matter tracts may be necessary to support basic feelings of owning and controlling contralateral body parts. An accurate and 'up-to-date' awareness of our motor abilities, however, may rely also on intact processing in cortical areas which presumably allow higher-order inferences about the current state of the body.


Subject(s)
Agnosia/diagnostic imaging , Brain/diagnostic imaging , Hemiplegia/diagnostic imaging , Stroke/diagnostic imaging , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Agnosia/complications , Agnosia/physiopathology , Awareness/physiology , Brain/physiopathology , Female , Hemiplegia/etiology , Hemiplegia/physiopathology , Humans , Image Processing, Computer-Assisted , Male , Middle Aged , Stroke/complications , Stroke/physiopathology
7.
Neuropsychology ; 30(2): 213-24, 2016 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26192538

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVE: According to a still-controversial view of recognition, projections between the perirhinal cortex and the medial subdivision of the mediodorsal thalamic nucleus (mMDT) support the mnemonic processes underlying familiarity, whereas a separate extended hippocampal system is critical for the recollection of episodic details during recognition. METHOD: In this study, we examined item recognition, familiarity, and recollection for faces and words in a patient (OG) with a right-sided lesion centered on the mMDT, which encroached on the central medial midline nucleus and may have resulted in partial disconnection of the mammillothalamic tract. On the basis of OG's neuropathology, the dual-process signal-detection (DPSD) high-threshold theory and the material-specific hypothesis of long-term memory together predicted a material-specific impairment in familiarity for novel facial memoranda, with a lesser decline in recollection of novel faces at short retention intervals. No abnormalities in either familiarity- or recollection-driven recognition of verbal memoranda were expected. RESULTS: Comparing the performance of OG and that of a group of 10 age-, sex-, and IQ-matched healthy controls, the remember-know procedure revealed the dissociations predicted by the material-specific and DPSD hypotheses: With recognition of previously novel faces, OG showed a deficit in familiarity-driven recognition that was significantly greater than the insignificant reduction in his recollection. All components of his word recognition were, however, preserved. CONCLUSION: A memory profile, marked by a dissociation between familiarity and recollection, fits naturally with the DPSD model and is incompatible with the idea that these kinds of memories reflect different degrees of trace strength.


Subject(s)
Memory, Long-Term , Mental Recall , Recognition, Psychology , Thalamus/pathology , Adult , Cerebral Cortex/pathology , Face , Female , Hippocampus/pathology , Humans , Male , Memory, Episodic , Models, Psychological
8.
Cortex ; 71: 85-101, 2015 Oct.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26188680

ABSTRACT

Medicated, non-dementing mild-to-moderate Parkinson's disease (PD) patients usually show recall/recollection impairments but have only occasionally shown familiarity impairments. We aimed to assess two explanations of this pattern of impairment. Recollection typically improves when effortful planning of encoding and retrieval processing is engaged. This depends on prefrontally-dependent executive processes, which are often disrupted in PD. Relative to an unguided encoding and retrieval of words condition (C1), giving suitable guidance at encoding alone (C2) or at encoding and retrieval (C3) should, if executive processes are disrupted, improve PD recollection more than control recollection and perhaps raise it to normal levels. Familiarity, being a relatively automatic kind of memory, whether impaired or intact, should be unaffected by guidance. According to the second explanation, PD deficits are amnesia-like and caused by medial temporal lobe dysfunction and although poorer recollection, which is caused by hippocampal disruption, may be improved by guidance, it should not improve more than control recollection. Familiarity impairment will also occur if the perirhinal cortex is disrupted, but will be unimproved by guidance. Without guidance, recollection/recall was impaired in thirty PD patients relative to twenty-two healthy controls and remained relatively equally impaired when full guidance was provided (C1 vs C3), both groups improving to broadly the same extent. Although impaired, and markedly less so than recollection, familiarity was not improved by guidance in both groups. The patients showed elevated rates of subclinical depressive symptoms, which weakly correlated with recall/recollection in all three conditions. PD executive function was also deficient and correlated with unguided/C1 recollection only. Our results are consistent with a major cause of the patients' recall/recollection impairments being hippocampal disruption, probably exacerbated by subclinical depressive symptoms. However, the results do not exclude a lesser prefrontal cortex contribution because patient executive functions were impaired and correlated solely with unguided overall recollection.


Subject(s)
Amnesia/psychology , Cues , Mental Recall , Parkinson Disease/psychology , Aged , Amnesia/etiology , Amnesia/physiopathology , Depression/psychology , Executive Function , Female , Hippocampus/physiopathology , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Neuropsychological Tests , Parkinson Disease/complications , Parkinson Disease/physiopathology , Photic Stimulation , Prefrontal Cortex/physiopathology , Psychomotor Performance , Recognition, Psychology , Temporal Lobe/physiopathology
9.
Neurocase ; 21(4): 471-8, 2015.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24898941

ABSTRACT

Anarchic hand syndrome (AHS) is characterized by goal-directed movements performed without volitional control (agency). Different AHS subtypes have been identified; however, few studies have examined the posterior subtype. We report a case of AHS following right-hemisphere parietal damage, with left-sided somatosensory and proprioceptive impairment. Agency was examined for nonanarchic (volitional) movements performed using the anarchic hand. The patient experienced abnormal agency for movements whether motor intention and visual feedback were congruent or incongruent, but not when intention was absent (passive movement). Findings suggest a general disturbance of veridical motor awareness and agency in this case of parietal AHS.


Subject(s)
Alien Limb Phenomenon/psychology , Awareness , Brain Injuries/complications , Parietal Lobe/pathology , Volition , Adult , Alien Limb Phenomenon/etiology , Alien Limb Phenomenon/pathology , Brain Injuries/pathology , Female , Humans , Movement , Psychomotor Performance , Somatosensory Disorders/etiology , Young Adult
10.
J Neuropsychol ; 7(2): 284-305, 2013 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23815454

ABSTRACT

A selective deficit in the recollection of episodic details is frequently reported in Parkinson's disease (PD). Previous explanations implicate dopamine dysregulation in prefrontal structures on which strategic memory processes rely. However, neuroimaging advancements suggest dopaminergic dysregulation of hippocampally dependent memory processes. Accordingly, dopamine agonists, which target D3 receptors in the hippocampus, may impair hippocampal functioning, causing a more pronounced recollection decline. Recognition memory (RM), familiarity, and recollection were examined in 21 patients with mild-to-moderate PD (Hoehn and Yahr mean: 2.67). Patients were subdivided into two subgroups according to dopamine agonist (pramipexole [PPX] or ropinirole [RPR]), and completed matched versions of an RM test in a medicated and unmedicated condition (termed ON and OFF, respectively). Ten demographically matched healthy volunteers (HVs) also completed both RM tasks in two separate sessions. The PD group (PPX and RPR subgroups combined) showed impairments in RM and recollection, but spared familiarity. When subdivided by dopamine agonist, the PPX subgroup's ON-medication recollection performance was significantly lower than that of both the HVs and RPR subgroup. There was no evidence of decline in OFF-medication recollection or familiarity in either the PPX or RPR subgroups. Recollection in both PD subgroups correlated positively with a composite measure of recall, but not prefrontally dependent measures of cognitive control. These findings suggest that mild-to-moderate PD patients may show relatively preserved recollection and familiarity, but that recollection is selectively disrupted by PPX, but not RPR and that this effect may depend on disrupted hippocampal function rather than impaired pre-frontally dependent executive functions.


Subject(s)
Benzothiazoles/adverse effects , Dopamine Agonists/adverse effects , Indoles/adverse effects , Memory Disorders/chemically induced , Mental Recall/drug effects , Parkinson Disease/psychology , Aged , Attention/drug effects , Case-Control Studies , Female , Humans , Male , Memory Disorders/complications , Memory Disorders/psychology , Memory, Short-Term/drug effects , Middle Aged , Parkinson Disease/complications , Parkinson Disease/drug therapy , Pramipexole , Recognition, Psychology/drug effects
11.
J Neuropsychol ; 6(1): 119-40, 2012 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22257705

ABSTRACT

Neuropsychological findings suggest material-specific lateralization of the medial temporal lobe's role in long-term memory, with greater left-sided involvement in verbal memory, and greater right-sided involvement in visual memory. Whether material-specific lateralization of long-term memory also extends to the anteromedial thalamus remains uncertain. We report two patients with unilateral right (OG) and left (SM) mediodorsal thalamic pathology plus probable correspondingly lateralized damage of the mammillo-thalamic tract. The lesions were mapped using high-resolution structural magnetic resonance imaging and schematically reconstructed. Mean absolute volume estimates for the mammillary bodies, hippocampus, perirhinal cortex, and ventricles are also presented. Estimates of visual and verbal recall and item recognition memory were obtained using the Doors and People, the Rey Complex Figure Test, and the Logical Memory subtests of the Wechsler Memory Scales. Each patient's performance was compared to a group of healthy volunteers matched for demographic characteristics, premorbid IQ, and current levels of functioning. A striking double dissociation was evident in material-specific long-term memory, with OG showing significant impairments in visual memory but not verbal memory, and SM showing the opposite profile of preserved visual memory and significantly impaired verbal memory. These impairments affected both recall and item recognition. The reported double dissociation provides the strongest evidence yet that material-specific lateralization of long-term memory also extends to the anteromedial thalamus. The findings are also discussed in relation to proposals that distinct anatomical regions within the medial temporal lobe, anteromedial thalamus, and associated tracts make qualitatively different contributions to recall and item recognition.


Subject(s)
Mammillary Bodies/physiopathology , Memory Disorders/physiopathology , Memory, Long-Term/physiology , Thalamus/physiopathology , Adult , Aged , Atrophy/pathology , Atrophy/physiopathology , Case-Control Studies , Cerebral Cortex/pathology , Female , Functional Laterality/physiology , Hippocampus/pathology , Humans , Lateral Ventricles/pathology , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Male , Mammillary Bodies/pathology , Memory Disorders/pathology , Memory Disorders/psychology , Middle Aged , Neural Pathways/pathology , Neural Pathways/physiopathology , Neuropsychological Tests/statistics & numerical data , Thalamus/pathology
13.
Neuropsychologia ; 48(5): 1367-75, 2010 Apr.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20036678

ABSTRACT

The effect of disease severity and dopaminergic medication on the assessment of familiarity and the recollection of episodic details during recognition in nondementing idiopathic Parkinson's is uncertain. Some studies have reported familiarity as deficient in mild Parkinson's yet others have found it intact even in moderate Parkinson's. Recollection has been found to be both preserved and deficient in mild and moderate Parkinson's. The extent to which these conflicting findings are explained by disease severity or dopaminergic medication or a combination of the two is uncertain, as all studies assessed patients in a medicated state, and disease severity has not always been consistently reported. Twelve patients with mild Parkinson's and 11 with moderate Parkinson's (medicated Hoehn and Yahr mean: 2.1 and 3.2, respectively), completed matched versions of a yes/no recognition memory test in a medicated and unmedicated condition (termed ON and OFF, respectively). Twenty-one matched healthy volunteers also completed both memory tasks in 2 separate sessions (termed Blue and Green, respectively). In the ON/Green condition, the moderate Parkinson's recollection performance was significantly poorer than the healthy volunteers and mild Parkinson's. By contrast, recognition memory and familiarity measures in both Parkinson's group were relatively spared. In the OFF/Blue condition, the moderate Parkinson's recollection was impaired, but only in relation to the healthy volunteer set. There were no significant differences in recollection performance between the mild and moderate Parkinson's groups. Again, recognition memory and familiarity measures in both Parkinson's group were relatively spared. Further analyses showed the moderate patients' recollection rates to be significantly poorer ON-medication compared to OFF. These findings are discussed in relation to the staging of disease progression on medial temporal areas which separately support recollection and familiarity, and the putative effects the different classes of dopaminergic drugs may have on these areas.


Subject(s)
Dopamine Agonists/therapeutic use , Memory Disorders/epidemiology , Parkinson Disease , Recognition, Psychology , Aged , Female , Humans , Male , Memory Disorders/diagnosis , Middle Aged , Neuropsychological Tests , Parkinson Disease/diagnosis , Parkinson Disease/drug therapy , Parkinson Disease/epidemiology , Severity of Illness Index
14.
Psychiatry Res ; 175(3): 205-10, 2010 Feb 28.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20034676

ABSTRACT

Neuropsychological research suggests that recognition memory (RM) and recall memory are impaired in patients with a major depressive disorder or a dysphoric mood state. This study examines the proposal that abnormalities in recollection (a form of recall) result from a breakdown in frontal strategic memory processes involved in encoding and retrieval, and executive functions linked to reality monitoring, planning, problem-solving, reasoning and decision-making. We investigated two predictions arising from this theory. Firstly, patients diagnosed with a major depressive disorder (MDD) will display a dissociation between (deficient) recollection and (preserved) familiarity. Secondly, if recollection impairments are indicative of a breakdown in prefrontal strategic memory processes which are dependent, at least in part, on executive processes, then an explicit correlational approach predicts that recollection will be positively associated with the severity of executive dysfunction in MDD patients. The remember/know paradigm was used to investigate RM for words and neutral faces in 16 MDD patients and 16 healthy volunteers, matched for age, gender and estimates of premorbid IQ. Measures of executive function included working memory, reasoning and decision-making. Applying the Dual Process Signal Detection interpretation of the remember/know data, the MDD group displayed significant impairments in RM and recollection rates for both verbal and neutral facial memoranda. In contrast, familiarity-aware rates were preserved. There was no evidence of executive dysfunction in the patient group, and little evidence that recollection rates correlated with executive function. Furthermore, a single process signal detection approach suggested that the MDD patients displayed a reduction in sensitivity for RM and remember rates but not know responses. The criteria for detecting studied from unstudied items, and remembering from knowing, were the same in both patient and healthy control groups. Taken together, these findings are consistent with the view that MDD is marked by a decline in RM, which is underpinned by an impairment in recollection rather than familiarity processes. The extent to which the recollection deficiencies arise from disruption of strategic memory and executive processes requires further investigation.


Subject(s)
Depressive Disorder, Major/complications , Memory Disorders/etiology , Mental Recall/physiology , Adult , Antidepressive Agents/pharmacology , Antidepressive Agents/therapeutic use , Depressive Disorder, Major/drug therapy , Executive Function/drug effects , Executive Function/physiology , Female , Humans , Male , Mental Recall/drug effects , Middle Aged , Multivariate Analysis , Neuropsychological Tests , Recognition, Psychology/drug effects , Recognition, Psychology/physiology , Statistics as Topic
15.
Cogn Behav Neurol ; 22(2): 117-21, 2009 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19506428

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVE: To test the hypothesis that anosognosia for dyskinesias in Parkinson disease (PD) results from a failure to detect discrepancies between intended and actual movement. BACKGROUND: PD patients often complain of drug-induced dyskinesias (involuntary movements) less than their carers. This remarkable unawareness is an example of anosognosia (ie, unawareness of deficits associated with an illness). A better understanding of anosognosia for dyskinesias in PD is important to understand the impact of the illness and side effects of treatment. METHODS: The ability to detect a discrepancy between intended movement and visual feedback about actual movement was investigated in 6 PD patients with anosognosia for dyskinesias, 11 nonanosognosic PD controls with dyskinesias, and 22 healthy volunteers, using a mirror to reverse the expected visual consequences of an executed movement. RESULTS: Nonanosognosic PD patients and healthy volunteers rated mirror-reversed movement as significantly stranger than normal movement (P=0.024 and <0.001, respectively), whereas PD patients with anosognosia for dyskinesias did not (P=0.375). CONCLUSIONS: The findings support our proposal, in that PD patients with anosognosia for dyskinesias do not report mirror-reversed movement (in which intentions and visual feedback conflict) as feeling distinct from normal movement.


Subject(s)
Dyskinesia, Drug-Induced/psychology , Parkinson Disease/psychology , Aged , Antiparkinson Agents/adverse effects , Awareness , Feedback, Psychological , Female , Humans , Levodopa/adverse effects , Male , Neuropsychological Tests , Photic Stimulation , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Self Concept
16.
Conscious Cogn ; 18(2): 458-70, 2009 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19195910

ABSTRACT

Anosognosia for hemiplegia (AHP) is a lack of awareness about paralysis following stroke. Recent explanations use a 'forward model' of movement to suggest that AHP patients fail to register discrepancies between internally- and externally-generated sensory information. We predicted that this failure would impair the ability to recall from memory whether information is internally- or externally-generated (i.e., reality monitor). Two experiments examined this prediction. Experiment 1 demonstrated that AHP patients exhibit a reality monitoring deficit for non-motor information (i.e., perceived vs. imagined drawings), whilst hemiplegic controls without anosognosia (nonAHP) perform like age-matched healthy volunteers (HVs). Experiment 2 explored if this deficit occurs when AHP patients discriminate performed, imagined, or observed movement. Results showed impaired reality monitoring for movements in AHP and nonAHP patients relative to HVs. Findings suggest that reality monitoring processes not directly related to movement, together with a failure to reality monitor movements, contribute to the pathogenesis of AHP.


Subject(s)
Agnosia/psychology , Awareness , Hemiplegia/psychology , Kinesthesis , Reality Testing , Aged , Attention , Body Image , Culture , Discrimination, Psychological , Female , Functional Laterality , Humans , Imagination , Judgment , Male , Mental Recall , Middle Aged , Pattern Recognition, Visual , Psychomotor Performance , Stroke/psychology
17.
Neuropsychologia ; 47(2): 481-8, 2009 Jan.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18973767

ABSTRACT

Anosognosia for hemiplegia (AHP) is characterised by poor insight or underestimation of hemiplegia after brain injury. Recent explanations of AHP have used an established 'forward model', which proposes that normal motor awareness involves comparing the predicted and actual sensory consequences of movements. These accounts propose that AHP patients may be able to form representations of their intended movements (i.e., motor representations), but fail to register discrepancy between intended and actual movements. A prediction arising from this proposal is that AHP patients are able to generate motor representations involving their hemiplegic limb(s). Our study provides the first direct examination of this prediction in patients with AHP. We used an existing 'grip selection task', which investigates motor representations by comparing how patients would grasp an object and how they actually grasp the same object. Eight right hemisphere stroke patients with AHP, 10 control patients (non-AHP), and 22 age-matched healthy volunteers (HVs) completed the task. Results showed that HVs outperformed both AHP and non-AHP patients in their motor representations for the hemiplegic limb; however, the performance of AHP and non-AHP patients did not differ significantly. Motor representations for the intact limb were lower than normal in AHP patients, whereas performance in non-AHP patients was midway between the AHP and HV groups. Findings suggested that the ability to form motor representations lie on a continuum, but that impaired motor representations for the paralysed limb cannot account for AHP. Distorted motor representations, in combination with other deficits, might contribute to the pathogenesis of AHP.


Subject(s)
Cognition Disorders/psychology , Hemiplegia/psychology , Imagination/physiology , Movement/physiology , Self Concept , Aged , Cognition Disorders/diagnosis , Data Interpretation, Statistical , Female , Hand Strength , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Models, Psychological , Neuropsychological Tests , Photic Stimulation , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Upper Extremity/physiology , Verbal Behavior/physiology
18.
Q J Exp Psychol (Hove) ; 62(1): 33-40, 2009 Jan.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18622809

ABSTRACT

Patients with a diagnosis of Parkinson's disease and age- and IQ-matched controls estimated the duration of short 500-Hz tones (325-1,225 ms), on trials where the tone was either preceded by 3 s of 5-Hz clicks, or presented without clicks. The click manipulation had been shown in earlier studies with student participants to make verbal estimates longer. Patients were tested both on and off their dopaminergic medication, and controls were also tested in two sessions. Verbal estimates were markedly and significantly longer on trials with clicks than on those without clicks for both the patients and the controls, but there were no significant performance differences between patients or controls, nor between the on and off medication sessions in the patients. The study shows that a manipulation of subjective time, which has had small but consistent effects in student participants, also affects timing in patients and adds to a growing body of evidence that timing in patients with Parkinson's disease may in many cases have the same characteristics as those of neurologically intact control groups.


Subject(s)
Auditory Perception/physiology , Parkinson Disease/physiopathology , Time Perception/physiology , Acoustic Stimulation/methods , Adult , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Case-Control Studies , Humans , Middle Aged , Neuropsychological Tests , Reaction Time , Time Factors , Young Adult
19.
J Neuropsychol ; 1(2): 131-47, 2007 09.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19331014

ABSTRACT

There is conflicting evidence about whether Parkinson's disease (PD) is associated with deficiencies in recognition memory (RM) and the processes which underlie it, namely recollection (a form of recall) and familiarity (a feeling of memory in the absence of recall). The aims of the current study were to examine forced-choice verbal RM (assessed with the Warrington Recognition memory Test), yes-no RM, recollection, familiarity and executive functioning in 17 patients with moderate PD and 17 healthy volunteers matched for age and premorbid intelligence. We predicted that patients with moderate PD would display a significant recollection deficit on the yes-no RM test, because their strategic memory processing that depends on executive functioning and is necessary for efficient encoding and/or retrieval, was disrupted. In contrast, familiarity memory, which is not dependent on these processes, and forced-choice RM (which is largely dependent on familiarity) should show higher levels of preservation. We also predicted that recollection should be correlated with severity of executive dysfunction. Our findings revealed that the PD patients were as likely to accurately discriminate between targets and distractors as the healthy volunteers on both RM tests. However, the PD patients were significantly less reliant on recollection-driven recognition decisions on the yes-no RM test when compared with the healthy control group. The patients also displayed executive function deficits, but these were not correlated with recollection. The extent to which the PD patients' reliance on familiarity at the expense of recollection is explained by impairments in strategic memory processes/executive function and/or medial temporal lobe retrieval processes needs further exploration.


Subject(s)
Antiparkinson Agents/therapeutic use , Cognition Disorders/diagnosis , Mental Recall , Neuropsychological Tests/statistics & numerical data , Parkinson Disease/diagnosis , Recognition, Psychology , Verbal Learning , Aged , Attention/drug effects , Cognition Disorders/drug therapy , Female , Humans , Judgment , Male , Memory, Short-Term/drug effects , Mental Recall/drug effects , Middle Aged , Parkinson Disease/drug therapy , Recognition, Psychology/drug effects , Verbal Learning/drug effects
20.
Neuropsychologia ; 44(6): 931-8, 2006.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16253293

ABSTRACT

In an earlier study we disputed the claim that the mediodorsal thalamic nucleus is critical for familiarity. We reported patient (QX) who showed a severe deficit in conscious recollection, and behavioural problems (disinhibition, emotional lability) with relative sparing of familiarity-aware memory following a left mediodorsal thalamic lesion. More recent MR imaging has revealed that QX's lesions are more extensive than previously reported and involve both dorsolateral thalamic nuclei, and whilst there is evidence of left mediodorsal thalamic damage, it is not the main focus of damage. This paper reports a full analysis of QX's thalamic pathology alongside a more detailed investigation of his recognition memory, using yes/no and forced-choice procedures, and executive function. The results revealed impairments in yes/no recognition and conscious recollection rates of famous, artist and unknown names. In addition to the previously noted behavioural disinhibition and emotional lability, a deficit in spontaneous planning ability was evident on the Zoo Map Test (subtest of the Bahavioural Assessment of the Dysexecutive Syndrome). Forced-choice recognition, familiarity estimates and remote memory showed higher levels of preservation. The findings indicate that the dorsolateral thalamus is part of the extended hippocampal circuit which is causally critical only for recall and conscious recollection of complex associations rather than for the more automatic processes linked with novelty detection.


Subject(s)
Brain Injuries/pathology , Mental Recall/physiology , Recognition, Psychology/physiology , Thalamus/physiopathology , Choice Behavior/physiology , Functional Laterality , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Male , Neuropsychological Tests/statistics & numerical data , Thalamus/pathology
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...