Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 7 de 7
Filter
Add more filters











Database
Language
Publication year range
1.
Obes Res Clin Pract ; 13(5): 435-439, 2019.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31551167

ABSTRACT

Media exposes children to weight biased messaging by presenting overweight characters negatively. Although bias against human figures and human characters has been examined, children's bias against non-human animated characters is unstudied. Children's (N=60; 4-6 years old) weight bias against human and non-human characters was measured. Children saw characters of different weights (thin, overweight), genders (girl, boy), and stimuli type (human-line drawing, human-photo, non-human cartoon), and rated them using positive and negative characteristics. ANOVAs and t-tests examined weight bias based on mean ratings of each character. Overweight figures were rated more negatively than non-overweight figures overall, regardless of gender or type of stimulus. Further, mean ratings of the non-human cartoon were significantly less positive than ratings of both the line drawings and photographs of human figures. However, there was no interaction of stimulus type and weight status, suggesting that bias is expressed equally against human and non-human overweight figures. Results indicated that children's negative weight bias extends to non-human cartoon figures. Implications for children's media are discussed.


Subject(s)
Bias , Body Weight , Cartoons as Topic , Human Body , Child , Child, Preschool , Communications Media , Female , Humans , Male , Overweight
2.
Pediatr Obes ; 11(4): 258-63, 2016 08.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26227415

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVE: This study sought to better understand factors influencing mothers' perceptions about healthy feeding. Additionally, a social consensus intervention was used to try to change mothers' likelihood to serve healthy foods. METHODS: Mothers saw photographs of healthy and unhealthy snacks and meals and rated them on several factors that were hypothesized to predict the likelihood of serving the foods. Then, mothers were randomly assigned to the experimental or control condition. Those in the experimental condition received social consensus information about these foods, whereas the control condition did not. The change in perceptions in the control and experimental conditions was examined. RESULTS: Mothers rated healthy meals and snacks less favourably than unhealthy meals and snacks on factors including cost, preparation time and difficulty, and child preference. Furthermore, mothers' perceptions of foods as liked by their child, easy to prepare and 'kid-friendly' predicted higher likelihood to serve those foods. Finally, mothers were more likely to describe the healthy snack as kid-friendly and indicated being more likely to serve it after receiving favourable social norm information about the food. CONCLUSIONS: The use of social consensus interventions to change parents' perceptions of healthy snacks may be one means of increasing the quality of children's diets.


Subject(s)
Diet, Healthy , Feeding Behavior , Health Knowledge, Attitudes, Practice , Maternal Behavior/psychology , Social Perception , Adult , Child , Child, Preschool , Female , Humans , Male , Mothers/psychology , Surveys and Questionnaires
3.
Eat Weight Disord ; 19(2): 137-44, 2014 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24338490

ABSTRACT

Recent debate has considered the validity of self-reports and laboratory-based behavioral measures of emotional eating. This paper reviews the literature on self-reported emotional eating and actual eating behavior (i.e., examines the concurrent validity). As detailed in the review, the literature suggests mixed findings on the correspondence between these self-reports and actual eating behavior. Based on this, we cite characteristics of studies that support the concurrent validity of the DEBQ and address possible reasons for the lack of concurrent validity in other studies, as well as concerns about the measurement of emotional eating in the laboratory. Two reasons for the lack of concurrent validity of self-report emotional eating scales identified in this review include (1) methodological/experimental design flaws and (2) the variability of emotional eating based on participant characteristics. We argue that further research on emotional eating needs to address factors related to self-reports of emotional eating and objective emotional eating behavior (e.g., negative affect, inaccurate recall of eating behaviors, sample differences, and laboratory design). We conclude with recommendations for future research on emotional eating.


Subject(s)
Eating/psychology , Emotions , Feeding Behavior/psychology , Feeding and Eating Disorders/diagnosis , Surveys and Questionnaires , Feeding and Eating Disorders/psychology , Humans , Netherlands , Reproducibility of Results
4.
Eat Weight Disord ; 13(4): e91-5, 2008 Dec.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19169069

ABSTRACT

Although overweight children and adolescents experience a number of immediate difficulties, little research has investigated the long-term psychological consequences for individuals who were overweight as children or adolescents despite their weight status as young adults. The goal of this study was to examine the relations between individuals' retrospective reports of their weight and height during elementary and high school, and their past and current weight concerns. It was expected that individuals who recall being overweight as a child or adolescent would have more weight concerns than their peers who recall being normal weight, even after controlling for current body size. Participants were 174 females and 61 males, ranging in age from 18-20 (M=18.66 yr, SD=0.63). For females, perceived elementary school weight in comparison to their peers significantly predicted current weight concerns, p<0.05. For males, perceived elementary school height in comparison to their peers significantly predicted a history of weight concerns, p<0.05. Retrospective height and weight in high school did not impact current weight concerns for either males or females.


Subject(s)
Body Height , Body Image , Body Weight , Adolescent , Child , Female , Humans , Male , Mental Recall , Peer Group , Self Concept , Young Adult
5.
Dev Psychol ; 34(3): 426-34, 1998 May.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9597353

ABSTRACT

In 3 experiments, the authors examined the sensitivity of infants to the unity of a partly occluded moving rod undergoing translation, rotation, or oscillation. Four-month-old infants were sensitive to the unity of the partly occluded rod when it translated, but not when it rotated, behind an occluder. Six-month-old infants perceived the rotating rod as continuing behind the occluder, but they did not perceive the unity of a rod that oscillated back and forth behind the occluder. Finally, 6-month-old infants showed an ambiguous response to a rotating rod when the shape of the occluder was changed from rectangular to round. These findings suggest that all types of common motion are not equivalent for specifying infants' perceptions of occluded objects. Additional factors should be considered that take into account the information specified by different types of motion and by different conditions at the intersection of the occluder and the object.


Subject(s)
Depth Perception , Discrimination Learning , Orientation , Pattern Recognition, Visual , Perceptual Closure , Psychology, Child , Attention , Concept Formation , Female , Humans , Infant , Male , Motion Perception , Perceptual Masking , Psychophysics
6.
Psychol Aging ; 12(3): 489-502, 1997 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9308096

ABSTRACT

Two aspects of perceived control, locus of control (LOC) and perceived competence (COM), command significant attention in personality and aging research. Mainly, these concepts are regarded as stable variables of considerable promise for predicting a range of outcomes. The authors concentrate on week-to-week within-person variability in self-reported LOC and COM. Using data collected over 7 months, the authors first demonstrates that the responses of a panel of older participants are structurally consistent with dominant conceptions of perceived control and that the responses maintain an underlying structure over the frequently repeated protocol. They next show that the within-person variation over weekly measurements is coherent information rather than "noise" and that individual differences in magnitude of week-to-week variability are a relatively stable attribute that predicts mortality status 5 years later. Implications of the findings for both methodological and substantive concerns are discussed.


Subject(s)
Adaptation, Psychological , Aging/psychology , Geriatric Assessment , Internal-External Control , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Female , Humans , Individuality , Male , Neuropsychological Tests/statistics & numerical data , Personality , Psychometrics , Reproducibility of Results , Self Concept
7.
Dev Psychol ; 33(5): 764-70, 1997 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9300209

ABSTRACT

Two-year-old children were required to use nonliteral words or actions to complete scenarios in which a doll acted as the agent of a series of make-believe events. An experimenter encoded the doll's imaginary intentions either verbally or through pretend actions. No gender differences were observed, but older 2-year-olds performed reliably better than younger 2-year-olds. In addition, episodes that required children to enact the conclusion to events that began with doll-directed actions proved difficult to comprehend. Discussion focuses on the implications of children's understanding of independent agency for joint sociodramatic play and on the problem of intersubjectivity in children's understanding of an adult's nonliteral intentions.


Subject(s)
Child Behavior , Child Development , Imagination , Internal-External Control , Play and Playthings/psychology , Psychology, Child , Social Behavior , Social Perception , Verbal Behavior , Age Factors , Analysis of Variance , Child, Preschool , Cross-Sectional Studies , Female , Humans , Male , Nonverbal Communication
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL