Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 20 de 31
Filter
Add more filters











Publication year range
1.
eNeuro ; 6(2)2019.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30923736

ABSTRACT

Creating three-dimensional (3D) representations of the world from two-dimensional retinal images is fundamental to visually guided behaviors including reaching and grasping. A critical component of this process is determining the 3D orientation of objects. Previous studies have shown that neurons in the caudal intraparietal area (CIP) of the macaque monkey represent 3D planar surface orientation (i.e., slant and tilt). Here we compare the responses of neurons in areas V3A (which is implicated in 3D visual processing and precedes CIP in the visual hierarchy) and CIP to 3D-oriented planar surfaces. We then examine whether activity in these areas correlates with perception during a fine slant discrimination task in which the monkeys report if the top of a surface is slanted toward or away from them. Although we find that V3A and CIP neurons show similar sensitivity to planar surface orientation, significant choice-related activity during the slant discrimination task is rare in V3A but prominent in CIP. These results implicate both V3A and CIP in the representation of 3D surface orientation, and suggest a functional dissociation between the areas based on slant-related choice signals.


Subject(s)
Parietal Lobe/physiology , Space Perception/physiology , Visual Perception/physiology , Animals , Choice Behavior/physiology , Macaca mulatta , Male , Neurons/physiology , Orientation/physiology
2.
Behav Processes ; 123: 84-9, 2016 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26344580

ABSTRACT

Three pigeons were trained to remember arrays of 2-6 colored squares and detect which of two squares had changed color to test their visual short-term memory. Procedures (e.g., stimuli, displays, viewing times, delays) were similar to those used to test monkeys and humans. Following extensive training, pigeons performed slightly better than similarly trained monkeys, but both animal species were considerably less accurate than humans with the same array sizes (2, 4 and 6 items). Pigeons and monkeys showed calculated memory capacities of one item or less, whereas humans showed a memory capacity of 2.5 items. Despite the differences in calculated memory capacities, the pigeons' memory results, like those from monkeys and humans, were all well characterized by an inverse power-law function fit to d' values for the five display sizes. This characterization provides a simple, straightforward summary of the fundamental processing of visual short-term memory (how visual short-term memory declines with memory load) that emphasizes species similarities based upon similar functional relationships. By closely matching pigeon testing parameters to those of monkeys and humans, these similar functional relationships suggest similar underlying processes of visual short-term memory in pigeons, monkeys and humans.


Subject(s)
Columbidae/physiology , Haplorhini/physiology , Memory, Short-Term/physiology , Animals , Discrimination Learning/physiology , Humans , Photic Stimulation/methods , Visual Perception/physiology
3.
J Exp Psychol Anim Learn Cogn ; 41(1): 32-8, 2015 Jan.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25706544

ABSTRACT

Two adult rhesus monkeys were trained to detect which item in an array of memory items had changed using the same stimuli, viewing times, and delays as used with humans. Although the monkeys were extensively trained, they were less accurate than humans with the same array sizes (2, 4, & 6 items), with both stimulus types (colored squares, clip art), and showed calculated memory capacities of about 1 item (or less). Nevertheless, the memory results from both monkeys and humans for both stimulus types were well characterized by the inverse power-law of display size. This characterization provides a simple and straightforward summary of a fundamental process of visual short-term memory (STM; how VSTM declines with memory load) that emphasizes species similarities based upon similar functional relationships. By more closely matching monkey testing parameters to those of humans, the similar functional relationships strengthen the evidence suggesting similar processes underlying monkey and human VSTM.


Subject(s)
Macaca mulatta/physiology , Memory, Short-Term/physiology , Visual Perception/physiology , Animals , Humans , Male , Photic Stimulation , Signal Detection, Psychological/physiology
6.
Front Behav Neurosci ; 7: 105, 2013.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23966916

ABSTRACT

Content-specific sub-systems of visual working memory (VWM) have been explored in many neuroimaging studies with inconsistent findings and procedures across experiments. The present study employed functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) and a change detection task using a high number of trials and matched stimulus displays across object and location change (what vs. where) conditions. Furthermore, individual task periods were studied independently across conditions to identify differences corresponding to each task period. Importantly, this combination of task controls has not previously been described in the fMRI literature. Composite results revealed differential frontoparietal activation during each task period. A separation of object and location conditions yielded a distributed system of dorsal and ventral streams during the encoding of information corresponding to bilateral inferior parietal lobule (IPL) and lingual gyrus activation, respectively. Differential activity was also shown during the maintenance of information in middle frontal structures bilaterally for objects and the right IPL and left insula for locations. Together, these results reflect a domain-specific dissociation spanning several cortices and task periods. Furthermore, differential activations suggest a general caudal-rostral separation corresponding to object and location memory, respectively.

7.
Anim Cogn ; 16(5): 839-44, 2013 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23754273

ABSTRACT

Change detection is commonly used to assess capacity (number of objects) of human visual short-term memory (VSTM). Comparisons with the performance of non-human animals completing similar tasks have shown similarities and differences in object-based VSTM, which is only one aspect ("what") of memory. Another important aspect of memory, which has received less attention, is spatial short-term memory for "where" an object is in space. In this article, we show for the first time that a monkey and pigeons can be accurately trained to identify location changes, much as humans do, in change detection tasks similar to those used to test object capacity of VSTM. The subject's task was to identify (touch/peck) an item that changed location across a brief delay. Both the monkey and pigeons showed transfer to delays longer than the training delay, to greater and smaller distance changes than in training, and to novel colors. These results are the first to demonstrate location-change detection in any non-human species and encourage comparative investigations into the nature of spatial and visual short-term memory.


Subject(s)
Columbidae , Macaca mulatta/psychology , Memory, Short-Term , Animals , Generalization, Psychological , Male , Time Factors
8.
Behav Processes ; 93: 25-30, 2013 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23159348

ABSTRACT

Seven adult human participants were tested in change detection tasks for object and location memory with large and small sets of four different stimulus types. Blocked tests demonstrated that participants performed similarly in separate object and location tests with matched parameters and displays. In mixed tests, participants were informed that they would be tested with either object changes or location changes; surprisingly, they were nearly as accurate remembering both objects and locations as when either was tested alone. By contrast, in the large-set condition, performance was lower than baseline on surprise probe test trials in which participants were tested (on 13% of trials) with the change type opposite to the present block (e.g., location probe trials during the object change block). These probe-test results were further supported by the reduction in probe-baseline differences when tested with small sets (6) of these item types. Small sets required remembering locations and objects to resolve object-location confounds. Together these results show that humans can remember both objects and locations with little loss of accuracy when instructed to do so, but do not learn these contextual associations without instruction.


Subject(s)
Discrimination Learning , Memory, Short-Term , Pattern Recognition, Visual , Space Perception , Adult , Female , Humans , Male , Reaction Time
9.
Behav Processes ; 93: 31-8, 2013 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23098992

ABSTRACT

The study of visual memory has repeatedly shown qualitatively similar visual short-term memory (VSTM) systems between human and many nonhuman species. In studies of human VSTM using change detection, increasing visual object complexity has an inverse effect on accuracy. In the current study, we assessed the functional relationship between visual object complexity and memory performance in visual change detection in pigeons and humans. Visual object complexity was quantified for each object type within each species using visual target search. Change detection performance was inversely related to object complexity in both species, suggesting that pigeon VSTM, like human VSTM, is limited by visual object complexity. Human participants were able to use a verbal-labeling strategy to mitigate some of the effect of visual object complexity, suggesting a qualitative difference in how the two species may solve certain visual discriminations. Considering the visual complexity of novel objects may also help explain previous failures to transfer relational rules to novel visual objects.


Subject(s)
Color Perception , Discrimination, Psychological , Memory, Short-Term , Transfer, Psychology , Visual Perception , Adolescent , Animals , Columbidae , Female , Humans , Male , Regression Analysis , Young Adult
10.
J Comp Psychol ; 126(3): 203-12, 2012 Aug.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22428982

ABSTRACT

Two monkeys (Macaca mulatta) learned a color change-detection task where two colored circles (selected from a 4-color set) were presented on a 4 × 4 invisible matrix. Following a delay, the correct response was to touch the changed colored circle. The monkeys' learning, color transfer, and delay transfer were compared to a similar experiment with pigeons. Monkeys, like pigeons (Columba livia), showed full transfer to four novel colors, and to delays as long as 6.4 s, suggesting they remembered the colors as opposed to perceptual based attentional capture process that may work at very short delays. The monkeys and pigeons were further tested to compare transfer with other dimensions. Monkeys transferred to shape and location changes, unlike the pigeons, but neither species transferred to size changes. Thus, monkeys were less restricted in their domain to detect change than pigeons, but both species learned the basic task and appear suitable for comparative studies of visual short-term memory.


Subject(s)
Columbidae , Discrimination Learning , Macaca mulatta/psychology , Animals , Color Perception , Male , Memory, Short-Term , Photic Stimulation
11.
Neuroimage ; 2011 Oct 14.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22019875

ABSTRACT

This article has been withdrawn at the request of the authors. The Publisher apologizes for any inconvenience this may cause. The full Elsevier Policy on Article Withdrawal can be found at http://www.elsevier.com/locate/withdrawalpolicy. This article has been withdrawn at the request of the editor. The Publisher apologizes for any inconvenience this may cause. The full Elsevier Policy on Article Withdrawal can be found at http://www.elsevier.com/locate/withdrawalpolicy.

12.
Curr Biol ; 21(11): 975-9, 2011 Jun 07.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21596568

ABSTRACT

Change detection is a popular task to study visual short-term memory (STM) in humans [1-4]. Much of this work suggests that STM has a fixed capacity of 4 ± 1 items [1-6]. Here we report the first comparison of change-detection memory between humans and a species closely related to humans, the rhesus monkey. Monkeys and humans were tested in nearly identical procedures with overlapping display sizes. Although the monkeys' STM was well fit by a one-item fixed-capacity memory model, other monkey memory tests with four-item lists have shown performance impossible to obtain with a one-item capacity [7]. We suggest that this contradiction can be resolved using a continuous-resource approach more closely tied to the neural basis of memory [8, 9]. In this view, items have a noisy memory representation whose noise level depends on display size as a result of the distributed allocation of a continuous resource. In accord with this theory, we show that performance depends on the perceptual distance between items before and after the change, and d' depends on display size in an approximately power-law fashion. Our results open the door to combining the power of psychophysics, computation, and physiology to better understand the neural basis of STM.


Subject(s)
Macaca mulatta/psychology , Memory, Short-Term , Visual Perception/physiology , Animals , Humans , Macaca mulatta/physiology , Psychophysics
13.
Psychon Bull Rev ; 17(2): 243-9, 2010 Apr.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20382927

ABSTRACT

Six pigeons were trained in a change detection task with four colors. They were shown two colored circles on a sample array, followed by a test array with the color of one circle changed. The pigeons learned to choose the changed color and transferred their performance to four unfamiliar colors, suggesting that they had learned a generalized concept of color change. They also transferred performance to test delays several times their 50-msec training delay without prior delay training. The accurate delay performance of several seconds suggests that their change detection was memory based, as opposed to a perceptual attentional capture process. These experiments are the first to show that an animal species (pigeons, in this case) can learn a change detection task identical to ones used to test human memory, thereby providing the possibility of directly comparing short-term memory processing across species.


Subject(s)
Discrimination Learning , Memory , Animals , Attention , Color Perception , Columbidae , Memory, Short-Term , Photic Stimulation , Time Factors , Transfer, Psychology , Visual Perception
14.
Learn Behav ; 37(2): 204-13, 2009 May.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19380897

ABSTRACT

Three pigeons were trained in a three-item simultaneous same/different task. Three of six stimulus combinations were not trained (untrained set) and were tested later. Following acquisition, the subjects were tested with novel stimuli, the untrained set, training-stimulus inversions, and object shape and color manipulations. There was no novel-stimulus transfer--that is, no abstract-concept learning. Two pigeons showed partial transfer to untrained pairs and good transfer to stimulus inversions, suggesting that they had learned the relationship between the stimuli. Lack of transfer by the third pigeon suggests item-specific learning. The somewhat surprising finding of relational learning by 2 pigeons with only six training pairs suggests restricted-domain relational learning that was controlled more by color than by shape features. Individual differences of item-specific learning by 1 pigeon and relational learning by 2 others demonstrate that this task can be learned in different ways and that relational learning can occur in the absence of novel-stimulus transfer.


Subject(s)
Concept Formation , Discrimination Learning , Generalization, Psychological , Recognition, Psychology , Transfer, Psychology , Animals , Columbidae
15.
Exp Brain Res ; 195(1): 135-43, 2009 May.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19305983

ABSTRACT

Certain sounds, such as fingernails screeching down a chalkboard, have a strong association with somatosensory percepts. In order to assess the influences of audition on somatosensory perception, three experiments measured how task-irrelevant auditory stimuli alter detection rates for near-threshold somatosensory stimuli. In Experiment 1, we showed that a simultaneous auditory stimulus increases sensitivity, but not response biases, to the detection of an electrical cutaneous stimulus delivered to the hand. Experiment 2 demonstrated that this enhancement of somatosensory perception is spatially specific--only monaural sounds on the same side increased detection. Experiment 3 revealed that the effects of audition on touch are also frequency dependent--only sounds with the same frequency as the vibrotactile frequency enhanced tactile detection. These results indicate that auditory information influences touch perception in highly systematic ways and suggest that similar coding mechanisms may underlie the processing of information from these different sensory modalities.


Subject(s)
Auditory Perception/physiology , Reaction Time/physiology , Sound , Touch Perception/physiology , Touch/physiology , Acoustic Stimulation/methods , Adolescent , Analysis of Variance , Female , Functional Laterality , Humans , Male , Psychoacoustics , Sensory Thresholds/physiology , Young Adult
16.
Int J Radiat Biol ; 81(6): 445-58, 2005 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16308915

ABSTRACT

Ionizing radiation has been reported to promote accelerated or premature senescence in both normal and tumour cell lines. The current studies were designed to characterize the accelerated senescence response to radiation in the breast tumour cell in terms of its dependence on functional p53 and its relationship to telomerase activity, telomere lengths, expression of human telomerase reverse transcriptase (hTERT, the catalytic subunit of telomerase) and human telomerase RNA (hTR, the RNA subunit of telomerase), as well as the induction of cytogenetic aberrations. Studies were performed in p53 wild-type MCF-7 cells, MCF-7/E6 cells with attenuated p53 function, MDA-MB231 cells with mutant p53 and MCF-7/hTERT cells with constitutive expression of hTERT. Telomerase activity was measured by the telomeric repeat amplification protocol (TRAP assay), telomere lengths by the terminal restriction fragment (TRF) assay, hTR and hTERT expression by reverse transcriptase-polymerase chain reaction (RT-PCR), senescence by beta-galactosidase staining, and apoptosis by TdT-mediated d-UTP-X nick-end labelling (TUNEL assay). Widespread and extensive expression of beta-galactosidase, a marker of cellular senescence, was evident in MCF-7 breast tumour cells following exposure to 10 Gy of ionizing radiation. Radiation did not suppress expression of either hTERT or hTR, alter telomerase activity or induce telomere shortening. Senescence arrest was also observed in irradiated MCF-7/hTERT cells, which have elongated telomeres due to the ectopic expression of the catalytic component of telomerase. In contrast to MCF-7 cells, irradiated MDA-MB231 breast tumour cells and MCF-7/E6 cells failed to senesce and instead demonstrated a delayed apoptotic cell death. Irradiation produced chromosome end associated abnormalities, including end-to-end fusions (an indicator of telomere dysfunction) in MCF-7 cells, MCF-7/hTERT cells, as well as in MCF-7/E6 cells. When cells were maintained in culture following irradiation, proliferative recovery was evident exclusively after senescence while the cell lines which responded to radiation by apoptosis continued to decline in cell number. Accelerated senescence in response to ionizing radiation is p53 dependent and associated with telomer dysfunction but is unrelated to changes in telomerase activity or telomere lengths, expression of hTERT and hTR. In the absence of functional p53, cells are unable to arrest for an extended period, resulting in apoptotic cell death while accelerated senescence in cells expressing p53 is succeeded by proliferative recovery.


Subject(s)
Breast Neoplasms/radiotherapy , Cellular Senescence/radiation effects , Tumor Suppressor Protein p53/physiology , Apoptosis/radiation effects , Breast Neoplasms/genetics , Breast Neoplasms/pathology , Cell Cycle/radiation effects , Cell Line, Tumor , Chromosome Aberrations , DNA-Binding Proteins/metabolism , Female , Humans , RNA/analysis , Telomerase/analysis , Telomerase/metabolism , Telomere
17.
Cancer Res ; 61(12): 4791-6, 2001 Jun 15.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11406554

ABSTRACT

Telomerase activity has been detected in >85% of all malignant human cancers, including 90% of prostate carcinomas. Using a well-characterized experimental prostate cancer system, we have found that telomerase activity is notably increased (>10-fold) during tumorigenic conversion. Expression profiles of the telomerase components (hTR and hTERT) revealed no substantive changes, which suggests a nontranscriptional mechanism for increased activity. Because the hsp90 chaperone complex functionally associates with telomerase, we investigated that relationship and found that along with telomerase activity, a number of hsp90-related chaperones are markedly elevated during transformation, as well as in advanced prostate carcinomas. Using the nontumorigenic cell protein extract as the source of telomerase, addition of purified chaperone components enhanced reconstitution of telomerase activity, which suggests a novel mechanism of increased telomerase assembly via a hsp90 chaperoning process during prostate cancer progression.


Subject(s)
HSP90 Heat-Shock Proteins/metabolism , Prostatic Neoplasms/metabolism , Telomerase/metabolism , Animals , Cell Transformation, Neoplastic/metabolism , DNA-Binding Proteins , Disease Progression , HSP90 Heat-Shock Proteins/biosynthesis , Humans , Intramolecular Oxidoreductases , Male , Mice , Mice, Nude , Molecular Chaperones/biosynthesis , Molecular Chaperones/metabolism , Phosphoproteins/biosynthesis , Phosphoproteins/metabolism , Prostaglandin-E Synthases , Prostatic Neoplasms/enzymology , Prostatic Neoplasms/pathology , RNA/metabolism , Telomerase/biosynthesis , Templates, Genetic
18.
J Biol Chem ; 276(19): 15571-4, 2001 May 11.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11274138

ABSTRACT

The ribonucleoprotein telomerase holoenzyme is minimally composed of a catalytic subunit, hTERT, and its associated template RNA component, hTR. We have previously found two additional components of the telomerase holoenzyme, the chaperones p23 and heat shock protein (hsp) 90, both of which are required for efficient telomerase assembly in vitro and in vivo. Both hsp90 and p23 bind specifically to hTERT and influence its proper assembly with the template RNA, hTR. We report here that the hsp70 chaperone also associates with hTERT in the absence of hTR and dissociates when telomerase is folded into its active state, similar to what occurs with other chaperone targets. Our data also indicate that hsp90 and p23 remain associated with functional telomerase complexes, which differs from other hsp90-folded enzymes that require only a transient hsp90.p23 binding. Our data suggest that components of the hsp90 chaperone complex, while required for telomerase assembly, remain associated with active enzyme, which may ultimately provide critical insight into the biochemical properties of telomerase assembly.


Subject(s)
HSP70 Heat-Shock Proteins/metabolism , HSP90 Heat-Shock Proteins/metabolism , Heat-Shock Proteins/metabolism , RNA , Telomerase/metabolism , Binding Sites , Catalytic Domain , DNA-Binding Proteins , Drosophila Proteins , HSP70 Heat-Shock Proteins/chemistry , HSP90 Heat-Shock Proteins/chemistry , Heat-Shock Proteins/chemistry , Humans , Kinetics , Models, Molecular , Protein Conformation , Recombinant Proteins/chemistry , Recombinant Proteins/metabolism , Telomerase/chemistry , Templates, Genetic
19.
Arch Pathol Lab Med ; 125(1): 146-51, 2001 Jan.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11151069

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: The proto-oncogene c-kit encodes a tyrosine kinase receptor (CD117) with a molecular weight of 145 kd. Previous studies, predominantly utilizing immunohistochemistry, have led to contradictory findings regarding the expression of CD117 in the endometrium. To help resolve this issue, we analyzed a series of benign and malignant endometrial tissues using both immunohistochemistry and Western blot analysis. OBJECTIVE: To examine the expression of CD117 in benign and malignant human endometrial tissues. METHODS: The expression of CD117 in 35 benign endometrial tissues (7 hyperplastic, 14 proliferative, 14 secretory) and 10 endometrioid carcinomas was investigated by immunohistochemistry (clone K45 monoclonal antibody). Immunoprecipitation (clone K69 monoclonal antibody) followed by Western blotting (clone K45 monoclonal antibody and clone 1.D9.3D6 monoclonal antibody) was performed to confirm CD117 expression. RESULTS: Fifty-seven percent of the hyperplasias, 93% of proliferative endometria, and 79% of secretory endometria immunostained positively for CD117. In benign endometria, epithelial staining tended to be more intense in the hyperplastic and proliferative endometria as compared to the secretory endometria, whereas endometrial stromal cells were not immunoreactive. Of the 10 frozen endometrial tissues analyzed by immunohistochemistry, 4 of 9 endometrioid carcinomas and a single case of an endometrioid polyp developing in association with a carcinoma expressed CD117. Immunoprecipitation followed by Western blot analysis confirmed expression of full-length CD117 in an endometrial polyp and carcinoma, and revealed a correlation between levels of immunoprecipitated CD117 and immunohistochemical staining intensity. CONCLUSIONS: Benign and malignant endometrial tissues express CD117. Our data suggest (a) a possible relationship between estrogen and CD117 expression in benign endometrium and (b) potential involvement of this growth factor receptor in endometrial carcinogenesis.


Subject(s)
Endometrial Neoplasms/metabolism , Endometrium/metabolism , Proto-Oncogene Proteins c-kit/metabolism , Blotting, Western , Endometrial Neoplasms/genetics , Endometrial Neoplasms/immunology , Endometrial Neoplasms/pathology , Endometrium/anatomy & histology , Endometrium/immunology , Epithelium/anatomy & histology , Epithelium/immunology , Epithelium/metabolism , Female , Gene Expression , Humans , Immunohistochemistry , Precipitin Tests , Proto-Oncogene Mas , Proto-Oncogene Proteins c-kit/genetics
20.
Mol Carcinog ; 28(1): 1-4, 2000 May.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10820482

ABSTRACT

Introduction of telomerase into normal cells provides telomere maintenance and an extended cellular life span, establishing the critical role of telomere attrition in cellular senescence. Additional data surrounding this observation suggest that expression of telomerase renders these "mortal" cells genomically stable with decreased frequencies of mutation, ultimately leading to continued proliferation without signs of changes typically associated with progression to a cancer-like phenotype. Interestingly, oncogenic insult after exogenous telomerase expression does not result in cellular transformation, yet addition of an oncogene first followed by telomerase does transform cells. Taken together, these results imply that order of addition is important for telomerase-mediated genomic protection and that telomerase expression is critical for the transformation process. The hypothesis proposed here is that telomerase, via its function in telomere stabilization, is capable of protecting cells from acquiring the required mutations and genomic instability necessary for malignant transformation, suggesting that telomerase is not an oncogene but may act as a novel class of tumor suppressor.


Subject(s)
Cell Transformation, Neoplastic , Genes, Tumor Suppressor , Telomerase , Telomere , Animals , Enzyme Stability , Humans
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL