Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 14 de 14
Filter
Add more filters










Publication year range
1.
BMC Infect Dis ; 24(1): 401, 2024 Apr 15.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38622527

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: African giant pouched rats, trained by Anti-Persoonsmijnen Ontmijnende Product Ontwikkeling (APOPO), have demonstrated their ability to detect tuberculosis (TB) from sputum. We assessed rat-based case detection and compared the mycobacterium bacillary load (MTB-load) in children versus adults. METHODS: From January-December 2022, samples were collected prospectively from 69 Directly Observed Therapy (DOT) facilities' presumed TB patients. Using an average of five rats, APOPO re-evaluated patients with bacteriologically negative (sputum-smear microscopy or Xpert MTB/RIF) results. Rat-positive samples were tested using concentrated smear light-emitting diode microscopy to confirm TB detection before treatment initiation. The rats' identification of pulmonary TB is based on smelling TB-specific volatile organic compounds (VOCs) in sputum. Using STATA, Chi-square for odds ratio and confidence interval was calculated and evaluated: (1) the yield of rat-based TB detection compared to that of the health facilities; (2) rat-based TB detection in children versus adults; and (3) rats' ability to detect TB across MTB-loads and between children and adults. RESULTS: From 35,766 patients, 5.3% (1900/35,766) were smear-positive and 94.7% (33,866/35,766) were smear or Xpert-negatives at DOTS facility. Of those with negative results, 2029 TB cases were detected using rats, contributing to 52% (2029/3929 of total TB identified), which otherwise would have been missed. Compared to DOT facilities, rats were six-fold more likely to detect TB among Acid Fast Bacilli (AFB) 1+/scanty [90% (1829/2029) versus 60% (1139/1900), odds ratio, OR = 6.11, 95% confidence interval, CI: 5.14-7.26]; twice more likely to identify TB cases among children [71% (91/129) versus 51% (1795/3542), OR = 2.3, 95% CI: 1.59-3.42]; and twice more likely to identify TB cases among children with AFB 1+/scanty than adults with the same MTB-load [5% (86/1703) versus 3% (28/1067), OR = 2.0, 95% CI: 1.28-3.03]. CONCLUSIONS: Rats contributed over half of the TB cases identified in program settings, and children, especially those with a lower MTB-load, were more likely to be diagnosed with TB by rats. The chemical signatures, VOCs, were only available for adults, and further research describing the characteristics of VOCs in children versus adults may pave the way to enhance TB diagnosis in children.


Subject(s)
Mycobacterium tuberculosis , Tuberculosis, Pulmonary , Tuberculosis , Adult , Child , Humans , Rats , Animals , Tanzania , Sensitivity and Specificity , Tuberculosis/diagnosis , Tuberculosis, Pulmonary/diagnosis , Tuberculosis, Pulmonary/microbiology , Sputum/microbiology
2.
Online J Public Health Inform ; 16: e50771, 2024 Apr 16.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38625737

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: Technological advancement has led to the growth and rapid increase of tuberculosis (TB) medical data generated from different health care areas, including diagnosis. Prioritizing better adoption and acceptance of innovative diagnostic technology to reduce the spread of TB significantly benefits developing countries. Trained TB-detection rats are used in Tanzania and Ethiopia for operational research to complement other TB diagnostic tools. This technology has increased new TB case detection owing to its speed, cost-effectiveness, and sensitivity. OBJECTIVE: During the TB detection process, rats produce vast amounts of data, providing an opportunity to identify interesting patterns that influence TB detection performance. This study aimed to develop models that predict if the rat will hit (indicate the presence of TB within) the sample or not using machine learning (ML) techniques. The goal was to improve the diagnostic accuracy and performance of TB detection involving rats. METHODS: APOPO (Anti-Persoonsmijnen Ontmijnende Product Ontwikkeling) Center in Morogoro provided data for this study from 2012 to 2019, and 366,441 observations were used to build predictive models using ML techniques, including decision tree, random forest, naïve Bayes, support vector machine, and k-nearest neighbor, by incorporating a variety of variables, such as the diagnostic results from partner health clinics using methods endorsed by the World Health Organization (WHO). RESULTS: The support vector machine technique yielded the highest accuracy of 83.39% for prediction compared to other ML techniques used. Furthermore, this study found that the inclusion of variables related to whether the sample contained TB or not increased the performance accuracy of the predictive model. CONCLUSIONS: The inclusion of variables related to the diagnostic results of TB samples may improve the detection performance of the trained rats. The study results may be of importance to TB-detection rat trainers and TB decision-makers as the results may prompt them to take action to maintain the usefulness of the technology and increase the TB detection performance of trained rats.

3.
BMC Vet Res ; 19(1): 226, 2023 Oct 30.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37904151

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: Brucellosis is a contagious zoonosis caused by bacteria of the genus Brucella. While the disease has been eradicated in most developed countries, it remains endemic in sub-Saharan Africa where access to reliable diagnostics is limited. African giant pouched rats (Cricetomys ansorgei) have been trained to detect the scent of Mycobacterium tuberculosis to increase case detection in sub-Saharan Africa. Given the similar diagnostic challenges facing brucellosis and tuberculosis, we explored the feasibility of training African giant pouched rats to detect Brucella. RESULTS: After 3 months of training, rats reliably identified cultured Brucella, achieving an average sensitivity of 93.56% (SD = 0.650) and specificity of 97.65% (SD = 0.016). Rats readily generalized to novel, younger Brucella cultures that presumably generated a weaker volatile signal and correctly identified at least one out of three fecal samples spiked with Brucella culture during a final test of feasibility. DISCUSSION: To our knowledge, these experiments are the first to demonstrate Brucella emits a unique odor profile that scent detection animals can be trained to identify. Importantly, cultured E. coli samples were included throughout training and test to ensure the rats learned to specifically identify Brucella bacteria rather than any bacteria in comparison to bacteria-free culture medium. E. coli controls therefore served a crucial function in determining to what extent Brucella abortus emits a unique odor signature. Further research is needed to determine if a Brucella-specific volatile signature is present within clinical samples. If confirmed, the present results suggest trained rats could serve as a valuable, novel method for the detection of Brucella infection.


Subject(s)
Brucellosis , Rats , Animals , Odorants , Brucella abortus , Escherichia coli , Muridae , Brucellosis/diagnosis , Brucellosis/veterinary
5.
Behav Processes ; 174: 104085, 2020 May.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32068141

ABSTRACT

Since 1997, APOPO, a non-profit organization based in Tanzania, has deployed African giant pouched rats (Cricetomys ansorgei) to detect landmines in post-conflict areas. More recent research suggests the pouched rats can also be trained to detect tuberculosis in human sputum samples. Despite proven success on both tasks, the potential impact of each rat is limited by the required training time and constraint to a single target odor. The aim of this project was to establish a technique to rapidly train pouched rats to detect multiple odor targets. Eight pouched rats were trained to detect five unrelated target odors in Experiment 1. In addition to training duration, we measured maintenance of all odor targets. In Experiment 2, we examined response persistence under conditions of extinction. Experiment 3 investigated whether refresher sessions before tests would maintain detection accuracy. The animals mastered all odor targets in significantly fewer sessions than APOPO's operational rats require to master a single target odor. Importantly, rats demonstrated strong discrimination between targets and non-targets despite the potential for interference and forgetting, suggesting pouched rats can be trained to detect at least five targets simultaneously. These results have the potential to increase the impact of each detection rat by both decreasing training time and expanding operational versatility, e.g., a single rat could be trained to detect multiple diseases.


Subject(s)
Discrimination Learning , Olfactory Perception , Animals , Conditioning, Operant , Extinction, Psychological , Female , Muridae , Odorants , Rats , Time Factors
6.
J Appl Behav Anal ; 52(3): 682-700, 2019 07.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31016719

ABSTRACT

Animal trainers working in scent detection programs are responsible for arranging training contingencies as well as for observing and recording animal behavior. We provided behavioral skills training (BST) to animal trainers working with scent detection rats to improve the treatment integrity of scent-detection research sessions. We evaluated the trainers' behavior at baseline and during the sequential introduction of each component of BST (instructions, modeling, and feedback). We observed incremental improvements in treatment integrity with the introduction of each BST component. Posttraining probes revealed that these improvements were sustained at least 3 weeks post-BST. As the trainers' behavior was modified during BST, we observed decrements in measures of rat performance. We discuss the nature of these interactions and their implications for the use of BST in scent detection research and in situations in which intervention with one party produces concomitant effects on the behavior of another.


Subject(s)
Discrimination Learning , Smell , Teacher Training/methods , Animals , Behavior, Animal , Female , Humans , Male , Rats
7.
Behav Processes ; 154: 73-80, 2018 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29274761

ABSTRACT

We've shown that pigeons can integrate separately acquired spatial maps into a cognitive map. Integration requires an element shared between maps. In two experiments using a spatial-search task in pigeons, we test spatial combination rules when no shared element was present during training. In all three experiments, pigeons first learned individual landmark-target maps. In subsequent tests involving combinations of landmarks, we found evidence that landmarks collaborate in guiding spatial choice at test (Experiment 1). In Experiment 2, pigeons were trained on two landmarks with different proximities to the target. On tests on a compound of both landmarks, pigeons showed stronger spatial control by the more proximal landmark, a performance overshadowing effect. Extinction of the proximal landmark shifted spatial control to the non-extinguished distal landmark. This reveals that the performance overshadowing effect was associative in nature, and not due to perceptual or spatial biases. This emphasis on spatial control during performance reflects the emphasis on performance processes that were a major focus in Ralph Miller's lab.


Subject(s)
Association Learning , Space Perception , Animals , Bias , Columbidae , Extinction, Psychological , Male
8.
J Neurosci ; 37(11): 3085-3101, 2017 03 15.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28188216

ABSTRACT

Fear- and stress-induced activity in the amygdala has been hypothesized to influence sensory brain regions through the influence of the amygdala on neuromodulatory centers. To directly examine this relationship, we used optical imaging to observe odor-evoked activity in populations of olfactory bulb inhibitory interneurons and of synaptic terminals of olfactory sensory neurons (the primary sensory neurons of the olfactory system, which provide the initial olfactory input to the brain) during pharmacological inactivation of amygdala and locus coeruleus (LC) in mice. Although the amygdala does not directly project to the olfactory bulb, joint pharmacological inactivation of the central, basolateral, and lateral nuclei of the amygdala nonetheless strongly suppressed odor-evoked activity in GABAergic inhibitory interneuron populations in the OB. This suppression was prevented by inactivation of LC or pretreatment of the olfactory bulb with a broad-spectrum noradrenergic receptor antagonist. Visualization of synaptic output from olfactory sensory neuron terminals into the olfactory bulb of the brain revealed that amygdalar inactivation preferentially strengthened the odor-evoked synaptic output of weakly activated populations of sensory afferents from the nose, thus demonstrating a change in sensory gating potentially mediated by local inhibition of olfactory sensory neuron terminals. We conclude that amygdalar activity influences olfactory processing as early as the primary sensory input to the brain by modulating norepinephrine release from the locus coeruleus into the olfactory bulb. These findings show that the amygdala and LC state actively determines which sensory signals are selected for processing in sensory brain regions. Similar local circuitry operates in the olfactory, visual, and auditory systems, suggesting a potentially shared mechanism across modalities.SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT The affective state is increasingly understood to influence early neural processing of sensory stimuli, not just the behavioral response to those stimuli. The present study elucidates one circuit by which the amygdala, a critical structure for emotional learning, valence coding, and stress, can shape sensory input to the brain and early sensory processing through its connections to the locus coeruleus. One function of this interaction appears to be sensory gating, because inactivating the central, basolateral, and lateral nuclei of the amygdala selectively strengthened the weakest olfactory inputs to the brain. This linkage of amygdalar and LC output to primary sensory signaling may have implications for affective disorders that include sensory dysfunctions like hypervigilance, attentional bias, and impaired sensory gating.


Subject(s)
Amygdala/physiology , Locus Coeruleus/physiology , Nerve Net/physiology , Olfactory Bulb/physiology , Olfactory Perception/physiology , Sensory Gating/physiology , Animals , Female , Male , Mice , Mice, Inbred C57BL , Neural Pathways/physiology
9.
J Exp Psychol Anim Learn Cogn ; 42(2): 200-11, 2016 Apr.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26881900

ABSTRACT

Mental imagery involves the perceptual-like experience of an event that is not physically present, or detected by the senses. Fast and Blaisdell (2011) reported that rats use the representation of an associatively retrieved event to guide behavior in ambiguous situations. Rats were reinforced for lever-pressing during 1 of 2 lights but not both lights. They were then tested with 1 light illuminated while the second light was either covered by an opaque shield (ambiguous) or uncovered and unlit (explicitly absent). Rats lever-pressed less when the second light was covered compared with unlit, suggesting that a representation of the ambiguously absent light guided their behavior. However, Dwyer and Burgess (2011) offered an alternative mechanism in which the explicit absence of a cue gains associative value during training. Covering the light at test could effectively remove these associative properties, resulting in a generalization decrement of behavior. The current experiments were designed to test contrasting predictions made by these 2 accounts. Experiment 1 empirically established that generalization decrement can occur when an element of a compound cue is presented alone at test, but this decrement is attenuated, rather than enhanced, when the absent element is covered. Experiment 2 utilized a conditioned inhibition procedure to demonstrate that rat behavior during cue ambiguity is driven by an associatively retrieved representation rather than by generalization decrement. Collectively, the results argue against a purely nonrepresentational associative account of behavior and support a role for associatively retrieved representations in rats.


Subject(s)
Conditioning, Psychological/physiology , Cues , Imagination , Inhibition, Psychological , Uncertainty , Acoustic Stimulation , Analysis of Variance , Animals , Female , Rats , Rats, Long-Evans
10.
Hippocampus ; 26(6): 804-15, 2016 06.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26703089

ABSTRACT

Identifying statistical patterns between environmental stimuli enables organisms to respond adaptively when cues are later observed. However, stimuli are often obscured from detection, necessitating behavior under conditions of ambiguity. Considerable evidence indicates decisions under ambiguity rely on inference processes that draw on past experiences to generate predictions under novel conditions. Despite the high demand for this process and the observation that it deteriorates disproportionately with age, the underlying mechanisms remain unknown. We developed a rodent model of decision-making during ambiguity to examine features of experience that contribute to inference. Rats learned either a simple (positive patterning) or complex (negative patterning) instrumental discrimination between the illumination of one or two lights. During test, only one light was lit while the other relevant light was blocked from physical detection (covered by an opaque shield, rendering its status ambiguous). We found experience with the complex negative patterning discrimination was necessary for rats to behave sensitively to the ambiguous test situation. These rats behaved as if they inferred the presence of the hidden light, responding differently than when the light was explicitly absent (uncovered and unlit). Differential expression profiles of the immediate early gene cFos indicated hippocampal involvement in the inference process while localized microinfusions of the muscarinic antagonist, scopolamine, into the dorsal hippocampus caused rats to behave as if only one light was present. That is, blocking cholinergic modulation prevented the rat from inferring the presence of the hidden light. Collectively, these results suggest cholinergic modulation mediates recruitment of hippocampal processes related to past experiences and transfer of these processes to make decisions during ambiguous situations. Our results correspond with correlations observed between human brain function and inference abilities, suggesting our experiments may inform interventions to alleviate or prevent cognitive dysfunction. © 2015 Wiley Periodicals, Inc.


Subject(s)
Acetylcholine/metabolism , Decision Making/physiology , Hippocampus/metabolism , Learning/physiology , Animals , Catheters, Indwelling , Decision Making/drug effects , Discrimination, Psychological/drug effects , Discrimination, Psychological/physiology , Female , Hippocampus/drug effects , Immunohistochemistry , Learning/drug effects , Muscarinic Antagonists/pharmacology , Neuropsychological Tests , Proto-Oncogene Proteins c-fos/metabolism , Rats, Long-Evans , Scopolamine/pharmacology , Visual Perception/drug effects , Visual Perception/physiology
11.
Physiol Behav ; 128: 220-5, 2014 Apr 10.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24548685

ABSTRACT

INTRODUCTION: Purified high-fat diet (HFD) feeding causes deleterious metabolic and cognitive effects when compared with unrefined low-fat diets in rodent models. These effects are often attributed to the diet's high content of fat, while less attention has been paid to other mechanisms associated with the diet's highly refined state. Although the effects of HFD feeding on cognition have been explored, little is known about the impact of refined vs. unrefined food on cognition. We tested the hypothesis that a refined low-fat diet (LFD) increases body weight and adversely affects cognition relative to an unrefined diet. MATERIALS AND METHODS: Rats were allowed ad libitum access to unrefined rodent chow (CON, Lab Diets 5001) or a purified low-fat diet (REF, Research Diets D12450B) for 6 months, and body weight and performance on an instrumental lever pressing task were recorded. RESULTS: After six months on their respective diets, group REF gained significantly more weight than group CON. REF rats made significantly fewer lever presses and exhibited dramatically lower breaking points than CON rats for sucrose and water reinforcement, indicating a chronic reduction of motivation for instrumental performance. Switching the rats' diet for 9 days had no effect on these measures. CONCLUSIONS: Diet-induced obesity produces a substantial deficit in motivated behavior in rats, independent of dietary fat content. This holds implications for an association between obesity and motivation. Specifically, behavioral traits comorbid with obesity, such as depression and fatigue, may be effects of obesity rather than contributing causes. To the degree that refined foods contribute to obesity, as demonstrated in our study, they may play a significant contributing role to other behavioral and cognitive disorders.


Subject(s)
Dietary Fats/pharmacology , Motivation/drug effects , Obesity/etiology , Psychomotor Performance/drug effects , Animals , Cognition/drug effects , Cognition/physiology , Conditioning, Operant/drug effects , Conditioning, Operant/physiology , Dietary Fats/administration & dosage , Female , Food Quality , Motivation/physiology , Obesity/physiopathology , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Rats , Rats, Long-Evans
12.
Behav Processes ; 91(2): 133-40, 2012 Oct.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22766352

ABSTRACT

There is much interest in studying animal personalities but considerable debate as to how to define and evaluate them. We assessed the utility of one proposed framework while studying personality in terrestrial hermit crabs (Coenobita clypeatus). We recorded the latency of individuals to emerge from their shells over multiple trials in four unique manipulations. We used the specific testing situations within these manipulations to define two temperament categories (shyness-boldness and exploration-avoidance). Our results identified individual behavioral consistency (i.e., personality) across repeated trials of the same situations, within both categories. Additionally, we found correlations between behaviors across contexts (traits) that suggested that the crabs had behavioral syndromes. While we found some correlations between behaviors that are supposed to measure the same temperament trait, these correlations were not inevitable. Furthermore, a principal component analysis (PCA) of our data revealed new relationships between behaviors and provided the foundation for an alternate interpretation: measured behaviors may be situation-specific, and may not reflect general personality traits at all. These results suggest that more attention must be placed on how we infer personalities from standardized methods, and that we must be careful to not force our data to fit our frameworks.


Subject(s)
Anomura/physiology , Behavior, Animal/physiology , Personality/physiology , Animals , Data Interpretation, Statistical , Electroshock , Environment , Exploratory Behavior/physiology , Individuality , Photic Stimulation , Predatory Behavior , Temperament
13.
Psychon Bull Rev ; 18(6): 1230-7, 2011 Dec.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21968926

ABSTRACT

In the present study, we investigated response decisions made under conditions of incomplete information in rats. In Experiment 1, rats were trained on either a positive patterning (PP; A-, B-, AB+) or a negative patterning (NP; A+, B+, AB-) instrumental lever-press discrimination. Subjects that had learned an NP discrimination responded less to Cue A when Cue B was covered at test. The cover did not, however, affect test responses to Cue A in the PP condition. In Experiment 2, rats received concurrent training on both PP and NP discriminations. After concurrent training, responses to Cue A were different with B covered versus uncovered for both NP and PP discriminations. We discuss possible accounts for why exposure to a nonlinearly soluble discrimination (NP) may have affected sensitivity to cue ambiguity produced by the cover. These results have interesting implications for representational processes engaged in problem solving.


Subject(s)
Discrimination Learning , Rats/psychology , Uncertainty , Animals , Conditioning, Operant , Cues , Discrimination, Psychological , Female , Rats, Long-Evans/psychology
14.
Behav Processes ; 88(1): 7-11, 2011 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21756986

ABSTRACT

Responses to innocuous stimuli often habituate with repeated stimulation, but the mechanisms involved in dishabituation are less well studied. Chan et al. (2010b) found that hermit crabs were quicker to perform an anti-predator withdrawal response in the presence of a short-duration white noise relative to a longer noise stimulus. In two experiments, we examined whether this effect could be explicable in terms of a non-associative learning process. We delivered repeated presentations of a simulated visual predator to hermit crabs, which initially caused the crabs to withdraw into their shells. After a number of trials, the visual stimulus lost the ability to elicit the withdrawal response. We then presented the crabs with an auditory stimulus prior to an additional presentation of the visual predator. In Experiment 1, the presentation of a 10-s, 89-dB SPL noise produced no significant dishabituation of the response. In Experiment 2 we increased the duration (50s) and intensity (95dB) of the noise, and found that the crabs recovered their withdrawal response to the visual predator. This finding illustrates dishabituation of an antipredator response and suggests two distinct processes-distraction and sensitization-are influenced by the same stimulus parameters, and interact to modulate the strength of the anti-predator response.


Subject(s)
Acoustic Stimulation/psychology , Anomura , Behavior, Animal , Escape Reaction/physiology , Habituation, Psychophysiologic , Animals , Anomura/physiology , Auditory Perception/physiology , Behavior, Animal/physiology , Predatory Behavior/physiology , Visual Perception/physiology
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...