Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 6 de 6
Filter
Add more filters










Database
Language
Publication year range
1.
Br J Psychol ; 115(2): 275-305, 2024 May.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38041610

ABSTRACT

As people age, they tend to spend more time indoors, and the colours in their surroundings may significantly impact their mood and overall well-being. However, there is a lack of empirical evidence to provide informed guidance on colour choices, irrespective of age group. To work towards informed choices, we investigated whether the associations between colours and emotions observed in younger individuals also apply to older adults. We recruited 7393 participants, aged between 16 and 88 years and coming from 31 countries. Each participant associated 12 colour terms with 20 emotion concepts and rated the intensity of each associated emotion. Different age groups exhibited highly similar patterns of colour-emotion associations (average similarity coefficient of .97), with subtle yet meaningful age-related differences. Adolescents associated the greatest number but the least positively biased emotions with colours. Older participants associated a smaller number but more intense and more positive emotions with all colour terms, displaying a positivity effect. Age also predicted arousal and power biases, varying by colour. Findings suggest parallels in colour-emotion associations between younger and older adults, with subtle but significant age-related variations. Future studies should next assess whether colour-emotion associations reflect what people actually feel when exposed to colour.


Subject(s)
Affect , Emotions , Adolescent , Humans , Aged , Young Adult , Adult , Middle Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Color , Color Perception , Arousal
2.
Vision (Basel) ; 7(4)2023 Oct 23.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37873896

ABSTRACT

(1) Background: Saccadic eye movements are rapid eye movements aimed to position the object image on the central retina, ensuring high-resolution data sampling across the visual field. Although saccadic eye movements are studied extensively, different experimental settings applied across different studies have left an open question of whether and how stimulus parameters can affect the saccadic performance. The current study aims to explore the effect of stimulus contrast and spatial position on saccadic eye movement latency, peak velocity and accuracy measurements. (2) Methods: Saccadic eye movement targets of different contrast levels were presented at four different spatial positions. The eye movements were recorded with a Tobii Pro Fusion video-oculograph (250 Hz). (3) Results: The results demonstrate a significant effect of stimulus spatial position on the latency and peak velocity measurements at a medium grey background, 30 cd/m2 (negative and positive stimulus polarity), light grey background, 90 cd/m2 (negative polarity), and black background, 3 cd/m2 (positive polarity). A significant effect of the stimulus spatial position was observed on the accuracy measurements when the saccadic eye movement stimuli were presented on a medium grey background (negative polarity) and on a black background. No significant effect of stimulus contrast was observed on the peak velocity measurements under all conditions. A significant stimulus contrast effect on latency and accuracy was observed only on a light grey background. (4) Conclusions: The best saccadic eye movement performance (lowest latency, highest peak velocity and accuracy measurements) can be observed when the saccades are oriented to the right and left from the central fixation point. Furthermore, when presenting the stimulus on a light grey background, a very low contrast stimuli should be considered carefully.

3.
J Opt Soc Am A Opt Image Sci Vis ; 40(3): A85-A90, 2023 Mar 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37133012

ABSTRACT

Color arrangement tests such as the D15 test can be used to detect congenital and acquired color vision defects. However, the D15 test cannot be used as the only test to assess color vision because of its relatively low sensitivity in less severe cases of color vision deficiency. In this study, we attempted to determine D15 cap arrangements for red/green anomalous trichromats with varying degrees of severity of color vision deficiency. The color coordinates of D15 test caps corresponding to a particular type and severity of color vision deficiency were determined using the model proposed by Yaguchi et al. [J. Opt. Soc. Am. A35, B278 (2018)JOAOD60740-323210.1364/JOSAA.35.00B278]. The arrangement of the color caps was modeled by assuming that individuals with color vision deficiency would arrange the D15 test caps judging by color differences perceived by them. The proposed simulation correctly predicts the increase in severity of color vision deficiency with spectral reduction between the L- and M-cone photopigments. The type of color vision deficiency is correctly predicted with few exceptions in protanomalous trichromats.

5.
Ann Dyslexia ; 65(2): 69-83, 2015 Jul.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25911276

ABSTRACT

This study includes an evaluation, according to age, of the reading and global motion perception developmental trajectories of 2027 school age children in typical stages of development. Reading is assessed using the reading rate score test, for which all of the student participants, regardless of age, received the same passage of text of a medium difficulty reading level. The coherent motion perception threshold is determined according to the adaptive psychophysical protocol based on a four-alternative, forced-choice procedure. Three different dot velocities: 2, 5, and 8 deg/s were used for both assemblies of coherent or randomly moving dots. Reading rate score test results exhibit a wide dispersion across all age groups, so much so that the outlier data overlap, for both the 8 and 18-year-old student-participant age groups. Latvian children's reading fluency developmental trajectories reach maturation at 12-13 years of age. After the age of 13, reading rate scores increase slowly; however, the linear regression slope is different from zero and positive: F(1, 827) = 45.3; p < 0.0001. One hundred eighty-one student-participants having results below the 10th percentile were classified as weak readers in our study group. The reading fluency developmental trajectory of this particular group of student-participants does not exhibit any statistically significant saturation until the age of 18 years old. Coherent motion detection thresholds decrease with age and do not reach saturation. Tests with slower moving dots (2 deg/s) yield results that exhibit significant differences between strong and weak readers.


Subject(s)
Child Development , Motion Perception , Pattern Recognition, Visual , Reading , Adolescent , Age Factors , Child , Female , Humans , Male , Psychophysics , Sensory Thresholds
6.
Front Psychol ; 5: 1376, 2014.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25505891

ABSTRACT

We produced a set of pseudoisochromatic plates for determining individual color-difference thresholds to assess test performance and test properties, and analyzed the results. We report a high test validity and classification ability for the deficiency type and severity level [comparable to that of the fourth edition of the Hardy-Rand-Rittler (HRR) test]. We discuss changes of the acceptable chromatic shifts from the protan and deutan confusion lines along the CIE xy diagram, and the high correlation of individual color-difference thresholds and the red-green discrimination index. Color vision was tested using an Oculus HMC anomaloscope, a Farnsworth D15, and an HRR test on 273 schoolchildren, and 57 other subjects with previously diagnosed red-green color-vision deficiency.

SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...