Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 20 de 38
Filter
1.
Laryngoscope ; 113(11): 1977-82, 2003 Nov.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-14603059

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVES/HYPOTHESIS: Bacterial biofilms, bacteria surrounded by a protective glycocalyx, have been demonstrated on bioimplants placed within and outside of the head and neck region. The presence of the biofilm often makes decontamination of an infected implant impossible, requiring removal of the implant. Infections attributable to biofilm formation within the facial skeleton after reconstruction with implants may result in delayed union, fibrous union, malunion, nonunion, and malocclusion. These complications often require removal of the implant and secondary surgery. Although the incidence of infections necessitating implant removal is relatively low, the increased numbers of implants being placed make this a growing problem. Previous work in the authors laboratory has demonstrated a resistance to biofilm formation on different types of pressure-equalizing tubes. The hypothesis evaluated in the study is that such resistance to biofilm formation is due to the inability of bacteria to adhere to the tubes because of the material's smoothness or surface charge. STUDY DESIGN: A controlled observational study. METHODS: Scanning electron microscopy was used to evaluate the formation of biofilms in vitro for a common strain of Staphylococcus aureus on four implantable materials. The implantable materials included titanium and polylactide resorbable plates. RESULTS: Consistent with the authors' prior findings, they were able to produce bacterial biofilm reliably on a silicone pressure equalizing tube but were unable to demonstrate biofilm formation on the titanium or resorbable implants. CONCLUSION: The absence of biofilm formation on these implants can best be explained by the surface charge or polarity properties of these materials. These findings are consistent with the relatively low incidence of infections among patients receiving these implants in maxillofacial applications.


Subject(s)
Maxillofacial Prosthesis/microbiology , Prosthesis-Related Infections/microbiology , Staphylococcal Infections/microbiology , Staphylococcus aureus/physiology , Biofilms/growth & development , Humans , In Vitro Techniques , Spectrophotometry/methods , Staphylococcal Infections/immunology , Staphylococcus aureus/immunology , Titanium/immunology
3.
Hear Res ; 164(1-2): 215-30, 2002 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11950540

ABSTRACT

Peripheral auditory adaptation has been studied extensively in animal models, and multiple exponential components have been identified. This study explores the feasibility of estimating these component processes for human listeners with a peripheral model of adaptation. The processes were estimated from off-frequency masked detection data that probed temporal masking responses to a gated narrowband masker. The resulting response patterns reflected step-like onset and offset features with characteristically little evidence of confounding backward and forward masking. The model was implemented with linear combinations of exponential functions to represent the unadapted excitation response to gating the masker on and then off and the opposing effects of adaptation in each instance. The onset and offset of the temporal masking response were assumed to be approximately inverse operations and were modeled independently in this scheme. The unadapted excitation response at masker onset and the reversed excitation response at masker offset were each represented in the model by a single exponential function. The adaptation processes were modeled by three independent exponential functions, which were reversed at masker offset. Each adaptation component was subtractive and partially negated the unadapted excitation response to the dynamic masker. This scheme allowed for quantification of the response amplitude, action latency, and time constant for the unadapted excitation component and for each adaptation component. The results reveal that (1) the amplitudes of the unadapted excitation and reversed excitation components grow nonlinearly with masker level and mirror the 'compressive' input-output velocity response of the basilar membrane; (2) the time constants for the unadapted excitation and reversed excitation components are related inversely to masker intensity, which is compatible with neural synchrony increasing at masker onset (or offset) with increasing masker strength; (3) the composite strength of adaptation levels off at high masker levels; this 'saturation' response is consistent with a diminished contribution from peripheral neural adaptation processes at high sound levels; and (4) the response dynamics for two of the adaptation components correspond generally to those for the 'very rapid'/'rapid' processes and 'short-term' processes described in animal studies of peripheral neural adaptation. The action latency of a third adaptation component suggests the role of a second-order peripheral or central process. This modeling exercise (1) indicates that multiple adaptation processes, whatever their origins, contribute substantively to the form of the temporal masking response and (2) supports a sum-of-exponentials scheme for estimating properties of the component processes.


Subject(s)
Hearing/physiology , Adaptation, Physiological , Adult , Animals , Humans , Models, Biological , Perceptual Masking , Reflex, Vestibulo-Ocular/physiology
4.
J Speech Lang Hear Res ; 45(6): 1285-96, 2002 Dec.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-12546494

ABSTRACT

Classical temporal integration (TI) is often viewed as a frequency-dependent, energy-based detection process. Detection thresholds for brief sinusoidal increments in either a fixed-level or a random-level broadband pedestal are reported that refute this traditional perspective of TI, Instead, evidence is presented that indicates (a) detection of absolute energy is not necessary for the TI effect and (b) the frequency dependence of TI is consistent with variations across frequency in peripheral auditory tuning, rather than the integration process per se. When peripheral frequency selectivity is controlled, TI can be explained by a frequency-invariant integration process over at least the frequency range from 500 to 4,000 Hz. This process is characterized by threshold improvements of 8-9 dB per decade increase in duration for increment durations between 10 and 300 ms.


Subject(s)
Cues , Hearing/physiology , Adult , Audiometry, Pure-Tone/methods , Auditory Threshold/physiology , Humans , Models, Biological , Perceptual Masking/physiology
5.
J Acoust Soc Am ; 107(4): 2169-87, 2000 Apr.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10790043

ABSTRACT

The purpose of this report is to present new data that provide a novel perspective on temporal masking, different from that found in the classical auditory literature on this topic. Specifically, measurement conditions are presented that minimize rather than maximize temporal spread of masking for a gated (200-ms) narrow-band (405-Hz-wide) noise masker logarithmically centered at 2500 Hz. Masked detection thresholds were measured for brief sinusoids in a two-interval, forced-choice (21FC) task. Detection was measured at each of 43 temporal positions within the signal observation interval for the sinusoidal signal presented either preceding, during, or following the gating of the masker, which was centered temporally within each 500-ms observation interval. Results are presented for three listeners; first, for detection of a 1900-Hz signal across a range of masker component levels (0-70 dB SPL) and, second, for masked detection as a function of signal frequency (fs = 500-5000 Hz) for a fixed masker component level (40 dB SPL). For signals presented off-frequency from the masker, and at low-to-moderate masker levels, the resulting temporal masking functions are characterized by sharp temporal edges. The sharpness of the edges is accentuated by complex patterns of temporal overshoot and undershoot, corresponding with diminished and enhanced detection, respectively, at both masker onset and offset. This information about the onset and offset timing of the gated masker is faithfully represented in the temporal masking functions over the full decade range of signal frequencies (except for fs=2500 Hz presented at the center frequency of the masker). The precise representation of the timing information is remarkable considering that the temporal envelope characteristics of the gated masker are evident in the remote masking response at least two octaves below the frequencies of the masker at a cochlear place where little or no masker activity would be expected. This general enhancement of the temporal edges of the masking response is reminiscent of spectral edge enhancement by lateral suppression/inhibition.


Subject(s)
Auditory Perception/physiology , Perceptual Masking/physiology , Time Perception/physiology , Acoustic Stimulation/methods , Adult , Humans , Middle Aged
6.
J Vestib Res ; 10(1): 25-39, 2000.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10798831

ABSTRACT

A protocol is described for measuring responses to a broad-band (1-2 Hz) caloric step stimulus from which the vestibular ocular reflex (VOR) and adaptation time constants can be estimated. This novel stimulation is the caloric equivalent to a rotatory step of head acceleration. In this protocol, the ear is irrigated continuously for 5 min with water at a constant temperature. During the initial 2-min period of irrigation the subject is seated and leaning forward in a nonstimulable position (horizontal canals in a horizontal plane). This irrigation phase establishes a steady-state thermal gradient across the horizontal canal, effectively eliminating thermal dynamic properties of the caloric transmission as a confounding factor. At the end of this phase, the subject is rapidly reclined to a stimulable position (horizontal canals in vertical plane) that elicits the VOR nystagmus response to an on-step of force on the cupula. Consistent with adaptation processes, the VOR response first increases and then declines gradually over the 2-min period that the step of force is maintained. Four minutes after the onset of irrigation, the subject is rapidly returned to the nonstimulable position (off-step), which is then maintained for a final 1 min. The response after the off-step, which releases the force on the cupula, reveals reversed after-nystagmus due to adaptation. Five subjects provided caloric step responses for 26 caloric temperature conditions spanning the range from 28.4 to 43.0 degrees C. The resulting responses were fitted with an adaptation model similar to models applied to rotatory acceleration step responses. Estimates of the model parameters for robust caloric stimulation, including time constants for the VOR (18.3 sec) and for vestibular adaptation (153.2 sec), are considered in relation to corresponding values reported in the literature for rotatory and caloric vestibular stimulation. The results suggest that caloric step stimulation can be used successfully to probe VOR dynamics.


Subject(s)
Caloric Tests/methods , Reflex, Vestibulo-Ocular/physiology , Adult , Humans , Nystagmus, Physiologic , Vestibule, Labyrinth/physiology
7.
Am J Audiol ; 9(2): 69-74, 2000 Dec.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11200194

ABSTRACT

The University of Maryland Tinnitus & Hyperacusis Center in Baltimore was the first center in the United States dedicated to the evaluation and treatment of tinnitus and hyperacusis patients implementing an habituation-based protocol that has become known internationally as Tinnitus Retraining Therapy (TRT). A crucial feature of the model is the postulate that a number of systems in the brain are involved in the emergence of tinnitus. The cochlea and auditory periphery play only a secondary role. To facilitate the goal of habituation of the tinnitus signal, TRT implements both directive counseling to neutralize the negative emotional associations toward the tinnitus, and sound therapy to interfere with the signal. As an outgrowth of the work with tinnitus, the evaluation and treatment of hyperacusis has emerged as an increasingly important part of our program. This report describes the unique facility, staff, and services of the Center as we celebrate a decade of research and clinical management dedicated to the scientific understanding of tinnitus and hyperacusis.


Subject(s)
Hyperacusis/therapy , Tinnitus/therapy , Acoustic Stimulation , Counseling , Habituation, Psychophysiologic , Hospitals, University , Humans , Hyperacusis/diagnosis , Hyperacusis/etiology , Maryland , Patient Care Team , Tinnitus/diagnosis , Tinnitus/etiology
8.
J Acoust Soc Am ; 106(1): 313-26, 1999 Jul.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10420624

ABSTRACT

The purpose of this study was to compare detection of increments and decrements occurring over limited regions of time and frequency within a 500-ms broadband (0-6000 Hz) noise. Three listeners tracked detection thresholds adaptively in a two-interval, two-alternative forced-choice task. Thresholds were measured for both increments and decrements in level [delta L = 10 log10(1 + delta N0/N0) dB, where N0 is the spectral power density of the noise] as a function of signal duration (T = 30-500 ms) for a range of signal bandwidths (W = 62-6000 Hz) that were logarithmically centered around 2500 Hz. Listeners were forced to rely on temporal- and spectral-profile cues for detection due to randomization of overall presentation level from interval to interval, which rendered overall energy an inconsistent cue. Increments were detectable for all combinations of W and T, whereas decrements were not consistently detectable for W < 500 Hz. Narrow-band decrements were not detectable due to spread of excitation from the spectral edges of the noise into the decrements. Increment and decrement thresholds were similar for W > or = 1000 Hz. Temporal- and spectral-integration effects were observed for both increments and decrements. The exceptions were for random-level conditions in which the signal matched the bandwidth or duration of the standard. A multicue decision process is described qualitatively to explain how the combination of temporal- and spectral-profile cues can produce temporal- and spectral-integration effects in the absence of overall-energy cues.


Subject(s)
Auditory Perception/physiology , Noise , Recruitment Detection, Audiologic , Time Perception/physiology , Adult , Auditory Threshold/physiology , Humans , Psychoacoustics , Time Factors
9.
J Acoust Soc Am ; 104(2 Pt 1): 984-98, 1998 Aug.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9714918

ABSTRACT

Detection thresholds were measured for silent temporal gaps within combinations of two, three, or four sinusoidal markers (i.e., combinations of one or two pre-gap markers with one or two post-gap markers). The markers were selected from the frequency range 2000-3100 Hz. Sinusoidal frequencies F1 and F4 were used as pre-gap markers, while F2 and F3 served as post-gap markers. Temporal gap detection (TGD) thresholds were measured from sets of three normal-hearing adults who tracked 70.7% correct detection thresholds adaptively across blocks of 50 two-interval, two-alternative, forced-choice trials. For symmetric marker conditions, where pre- and post-gap markers were equivalent in frequency (e.g., F1 = F2 or F1 = F2 and F3 = F4), TGD thresholds were < 10 ms. However, for asymmetric marker frequency alignments across the silent gap, including stimulus configurations where only three markers were presented on a trial (e.g., F1 = F2, F2 not equal to F3, no F4), performance was highly variable and was dramatically disrupted by the presentation of a second post-gap marker. The multiple-marker results reveal that TGD depends greatly on the number of markers presented, both in terms of the marker temporal position before and after the silent gap signal and the marker frequency alignment (symmetry) across the gap. These results, which cannot be predicted from models of the auditory periphery, may reflect perceptual mechanisms that are important in grouping and organizing auditory images.


Subject(s)
Signal Detection, Psychological/physiology , Time Perception/physiology , Adult , Auditory Threshold , Humans , Middle Aged
10.
J Acoust Soc Am ; 103(6): 3554-60, 1998 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9637037

ABSTRACT

Monaurally measured temporal gap detection (TGD) thresholds characteristically increase as the frequency difference is increased over a range of about half an octave to an octave between two sinusoids that mark the onset and offset of the silent gap. For greater sinusoidal frequency separations, the TGD thresholds often become asymptotic. This pattern probably reflects two different processes. The first process likely reflects within-channel processing within a single auditory filter or channel. The second process is less certain, but may reflect between-channel processing of the silent gap stimulus across two or more independent frequency channels. To evaluate the hypothesis that asymptotic monaural gap detection can be explained by a simple between-channel process, TGD thresholds were measured as a function of frequency separation between a pregap sinusoid presented to the left ear (channel 1) and a postgap sinusoid, of higher frequency, presented to the right ear (channel 2). The rationale for dichotic presentation of the sinusoidal markers and gap signal followed from the fact that the gap detection task must be performed between two independent channels by combining the outputs from each channel (ear) and recovering the gap information centrally. The resulting TGD thresholds for pregap sinusoids from 250 to 4000 Hz were relatively invariant and increased only slightly with increasing marker frequency separation. The average TGD thresholds for four listeners were in the range of 30 to 40 ms, which corresponded closely with their asymptotic TGD thresholds for the same set of stimulus conditions measured monaurally. This correspondence of the two data sets supports an across-frequency, between-channel process for asymptotic monaural gap detection at marker frequency separations greater than about half an octave.


Subject(s)
Auditory Perception/physiology , Auditory Threshold , Adult , Dichotic Listening Tests , Humans , Middle Aged
11.
J Am Acad Audiol ; 7(2): 125-9, 1996 Apr.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8652865

ABSTRACT

An adaptive, maximum likelihood (ML) procedure was assessed as an automated tool for estimating audiometric pure-tone thresholds in the clinic under computer control. Pure-tone air-conduction thresholds were measured from 101 workmen who received annual hearing rechecks as part of their employee hearing conservation program. A pure-tone threshold was measured bilaterally for each of the standard audiometric frequencies in a 15-trial block to yield 60 percent correct detection with the ML procedure. The workmen were tested on a modified "yes-no" task. On a trial, the signal was presented in a visually cued 200-msec observation interval. Each workman then had 1000 msec to make a "yes" response. If the workman did not respond during the 1000-msec response period, then the computer assumed a "no" response. After either the "yes" or "no" response, the computer adjusted the signal level for the next trial. The thresholds measured by ML procedure compared favorably with thresholds measured from the same listeners by conventional (CONV) audiometry. The efficiency of the ML procedure was also compared in terms of the time necessary for an experienced audiologist to instruct the listener and perform CONV audiometry. CONV audiometry (3-4 minutes per listener) required about half of the time needed for the ML procedure (6-7 minutes per listener). The relatively longer time associated with measuring an audiogram with the ML procedure was due primarily to more trials being used to estimate threshold.


Subject(s)
Audiometry, Pure-Tone , Auditory Perception , Diagnosis, Computer-Assisted , Hearing , Adult , Audiometry, Speech , Humans , Male
12.
J Speech Hear Res ; 39(1): 4-18, 1996 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8820695

ABSTRACT

Categorical perception was evaluated for a nine-token voice onset time (VOT) continuum with endpoint tokens /feil/-/veil/. The synthetic speech continuum was presented in a random-level noise masker at different signal-to-noise ratios (SNR = 0, + 6, +12 dB) and overall presentation levels (50 and 70 dB HL). Overall labelling performance deteriorated as the SNR was reduced. Labelling results for the +12-dB-SNR condition reflected a category boundary at 87 ms for listeners with normal hearing sensitivity. The companion two-step discrimination function revealed better-than-chance performance between pairs of tokens labelled fail, chance performance between pairs of tokens labelled vail, and a slight performance peak at the labelling boundary between fail and vail. Listeners with high-frequency audiometric deficits produced labelling results for the +12-dB-SNR condition that were similar to normal functions measured for the 0-dB-SNR condition. These listeners were unable to discriminate two-step differences in voicing duration, but they produced a normal temporal labelling boundary. To try to understand the noncategorical discrimination data, a psychoacoustic analog for the speech continuum was evaluated. Relative onset time (ROT) difference limens (DLs) were measured as a function of the temporal onset delay of a low-frequency sawtooth waveform relative to the onset of a high-frequency noise burst. The ROT cue was used only when absolute stimulus duration could not be relied upon as a consistent cue, under conditions where a large range of random overall duration was presented to the listener. The ROT DLs were relatively invariant over a range of standard delays from 50 to 110 ms. The average DL was about 30 ms, which is consistent with the small performance peak in the synthetic speech discrimination function.


Subject(s)
Noise , Phonetics , Speech Perception , Humans , Psychoacoustics , Sound Spectrography , Speech Discrimination Tests , Time Factors
13.
J Acoust Soc Am ; 96(1): 102-14, 1994 Jul.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8064013

ABSTRACT

Masked detection thresholds were measured at a center frequency of 2500 Hz for a range of noise signal bandwidths (W = 62 to 6000 Hz) and durations (T = 10 to 480 ms) approximating that found acoustically in speech. The signals were presented in an uncorrelated 480-ms, 6000-Hz-wide masker. The masker was presented: (1) at a constant spectrum level (53 dB SPL/Hz) or (2) with the overall level varied randomly over a 50-dB range from interval to interval of a trial. Performance was disrupted in the random-level masker only for the condition where the signal and uncorrelated masker were gated on and off simultaneously and were matched spectrally. Time constants (tau) estimated from temporal integration functions fit to the masked detection threshold data were related inversely to W for W broader than the critical bandwidth. Sensitivity to the noise signals was evaluated in the context of an optimum-detection model (Green, 1960). The results did not follow the prediction of a constant bandwidth-duration (WT) product, but may be understood in terms of cues available to the listener from the relative combination of signal and masker parameters. At least three cues for detection were identified in these experiments: (1) a relative timing cue, (2) a spectral shape cue, and (3) a traditional energy cue compared across observation intervals. The relative timing cue and spectral shape cue together contributed as much as a 10- to 12-dB advantage relative to detection based on the traditional energy cue alone. A new multi-cue detection model for predicting the masked detection thresholds was derived. Predictions from the new model were highly correlated (r = 0.95) with the empirically measured masked detection thresholds.


Subject(s)
Auditory Threshold , Noise , Perceptual Masking , Adult , Auditory Perception , Humans , Time Factors
14.
J Acoust Soc Am ; 93(2): 1023-7, 1993 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8445113

ABSTRACT

Temporal gap detection thresholds were measured in narrow-band noise-burst markers having acoustic characteristics representative of isolated steady-state second-formant (F2) properties for/p,t,k/paired separately with/i,ae,u,o/. The results revealed that gap detection threshold increased systematically as the difference was increased between the simulated stop and vowel F2 frequencies. A strong positive correlation (r = 0.87) between gap detection threshold and linear marker center frequency difference was highly significant (p < 0.001). Differences in other stimulus features had little influence on gap detection performance. Implications for speech perception are discussed.


Subject(s)
Auditory Perception , Phonetics , Acoustic Stimulation , Acoustics , Adult , Auditory Threshold , Female , Humans , Male , Noise , Speech Acoustics
15.
Acta Otolaryngol Suppl ; 498: 19-29, 1992.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-1462772

ABSTRACT

Management of the patient's level of arousal is one of the most important variables in obtaining consistent and strong caloric responses. The patient may suppress the caloric response and/or exacerbate beat-to-beat variability if some type of mental alerting (MA) task is not used to focus the patient's attention from his or her dizziness. An experiment was undertaken to evaluate simple MA tasks in terms of associated caloric response strength and variability. Warm caloric responses were measured while each of 10 normal subjects performed eight different MA tasks. The mental exercises included two math tasks, two quizzing tasks, two hand-motor tasks, and two alphabet tasks. One of the tasks in each complementary pair required the subject to interact with the examiner throughout the caloric response. Minimal or no interaction was required for the companion task. The relative ordering among the eight MA tasks was compared in terms of total sum of ranks, summed across 15 performance measures taken from caloric response indices. The highest-ranked altering task was an exercise requiring subjects to name or list local cities, states in the U.S.A., colors, etc. The lowest-ranked tasks were backward counting exercises and reflexive quizzing, which are the traditional tasks used in the clinic.


Subject(s)
Arousal/physiology , Caloric Tests/methods , Evaluation Studies as Topic , Humans
16.
Acta Otolaryngol Suppl ; 498: 4-18, 1992.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-1462773

ABSTRACT

A mathematical model for describing the caloric response over time offers many important advantages over the commercially-available qualitatively-fitted curves that are now used by the clinician for evaluating caloric results. In this report advances in the development of a nonlinear least-squares mathematical model are discussed and the roles and derivations of fitting parameters and curve-derived indices are outlined. This model provides a rigorous and objective description of the caloric response in its entirety with four continuous parameters. These fitting parameters make it possible to 1) describe individual caloric responses precisely and uniquely, 2) compare pairs of individual caloric responses or groups of caloric responses statistically, 3) extract information not previously available, 4) quantify variability within the caloric response, and 5) model physical properties of the caloric stimulus and physiological variables affecting the caloric response. Results from this model are compared with the results from our earlier models and with traditional multiparameter caloric results.


Subject(s)
Caloric Tests , Models, Biological , Humans , Mathematics , Time Factors
17.
J Acoust Soc Am ; 91(1): 293-305, 1992 Jan.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-1737878

ABSTRACT

The purpose of this study was to compare the role of frequency selectivity in measures of auditory and vibrotactile temporal resolution. In the first experiment, temporal modulation transfer functions for a sinusoidally amplitude modulated (SAM) 250-Hz carrier revealed auditory modulation thresholds significantly lower than corresponding vibrotactile modulation thresholds at SAM frequencies greater than or equal to 100 Hz. In the second experiment, auditory and vibrotactile gap detection thresholds were measured by presenting silent gaps bounded by markers of the same or different frequency. The marker frequency F1 = 250 Hz preceded the silent gap and marker frequencies after the silent gap included F2 = 250, 255, 263, 310, and 325 Hz. Auditory gap detection thresholds were lower than corresponding vibrotactile thresholds for F2 markers less than or equal to 263 Hz, but were greater than the corresponding vibrotactile gap detection thresholds for F2 markers greater than or equal to 310 Hz. When the auditory gap detection thresholds were transformed into filter attenuation values, the results were modeled well by a constant-percentage (10%) bandwidth filter centered on F1. The vibrotactile gap detection thresholds, however, were independent of marker frequency separation. In a third experiment, auditory and vibrotactile rate difference limens (RDLs) were measured for a 250-Hz carrier at SAM rates less than or equal to 100 Hz. Auditory RDLs were lower than corresponding vibrotactile RDLs for standard rates greater than 10 Hz. Combination tones may have confounded auditory performance for standard rates of 80 and 100 Hz. The results from these experiments revealed that frequency selectivity influences auditory measures of temporal resolution, but there was no evidence of frequency selectivity affecting vibrotactile temporal resolution.


Subject(s)
Auditory Perception/physiology , Pitch Perception/physiology , Sensory Thresholds/physiology , Time Perception/physiology , Touch/physiology , Vibration , Adult , Auditory Threshold/physiology , Female , Humans , Male
18.
J Acoust Soc Am ; 89(2): 830-7, 1991 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-2016432

ABSTRACT

Gap detection thresholds were measured by forced-choice procedure for conditions where the duration of a silent gap was varied adaptively between pairs of sinusoidal markers of the same or different frequency. Frequencies of the first sinusoid in a pair of markers ranged from F1 = 500 to 4000 Hz. Second-sinusoid marker frequencies F2 included F1 = F2, and usually frequencies 2%, 5%, 24%, and 50% higher than F1. In preliminary studies the role of presentation level (E/N0) on gap detection was considered. Preliminary data revealed confounding extraneous factors arising from gating transients and from overall stimulus (i.e., markers + gap) and/or masker duration cues. In the main experiments, the contributions of these extraneous cues were evaluated with experimental designs aimed at identifying and minimizing the confounding roles of these cues in gap detection. For conditions where extraneous gating transient cues were minimized (by presenting the sinusoidal markers in a continuous noise masker with random onset phase for the second sinusoid in every pair of markers) and overall stimulus duration cues were diminished (by randomizing the duration of each marker independently), gap detection thresholds increased from 5 to 90 ms as the frequency separation between F1 and F2 was increased by half an octave. When the gap detection thresholds were treated as filter attenuation values by normalizing and converting the data into decibels, the data were closely fit by the roex filter model. On average, the listeners' performances were modeled well by a constant-percentage (7%) bandwidth filter centered on F1.


Subject(s)
Attention , Loudness Perception , Pitch Perception , Auditory Threshold , Humans , Psychoacoustics
19.
J Speech Hear Res ; 33(3): 530-9, 1990 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-2232771

ABSTRACT

At present, the most popular auditory filter shape model is one with a rounded peak and exponentially decaying filter skirts (Patterson & Moore, 1986). Unfortunately, the complex nature of this "roex" filter model may, in some instances, have hindered the application of the auditory filter shape in clinical measurements of frequency selectivity. Moreover, some of the assumptions of the roex filter model may be violated at high sound-pressure levels (SPLs) and this limitation has also been a factor when considering the roex auditory filter shape in the clinic. Our purpose is to introduce a simplified method that is adequate for obtaining clinically useful estimates of triangular-shaped auditory filters. Although the triangular-shaped filter model faces the same problems as the roex model at high SPLs, the calculations and assumptions underlying the former are far less complicated. The triangular filter model also retains many of the qualitative properties and advantages afforded by roex-fitted auditory filter shapes. In this report, we review the basic concepts underlying auditory filter shape estimates and describe our methods for measuring and fitting the triangular-shaped filter model. We then present normative triangular filter shapes and compare these estimates with auditory filter shapes fitted by other means. Finally, we present selected examples of triangular filter shapes fitted to the masked thresholds of hearing-impaired patients. For the most part, the triangular-shaped filter model offers the clinician a satisfactory compromise for obtaining estimates of auditory filter shape and frequency selectivity at moderately intense and high SPLs.


Subject(s)
Ear/anatomy & histology , Hearing Disorders/physiopathology , Hearing Tests/instrumentation , Adult , Auditory Threshold , Humans , Least-Squares Analysis , Noise , Perceptual Masking , Physical Phenomena , Physics
20.
J Speech Hear Res ; 32(3): 681-7, 1989 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-2779211

ABSTRACT

We describe preliminary attempts to fit a mathematical function to the slow-component eye velocity (SCV) over the time course of caloric-induced nystagmus. Initially, we consider a Weibull equation with three parameters. These parameters are estimated by a least-squares procedure to fit digitized SCV data. We present examples of SCV data and fitted curves to show how adjustments in the parameters of the model affect the fitted curve. The best fitting parameters are presented for curves fit to 120 warm caloric responses. The fitting parameters and the efficacy of the fitted curves are compared before and after the SCV data were smoothed to reduce response variability. We also consider a more flexible four-parameter Weibull equation that, for 98% of the smoothed caloric responses, yields fits that describe the data more precisely than a line through the mean. Finally, we consider advantages and problems in fitting the Weibull function to caloric data.


Subject(s)
Models, Theoretical , Nystagmus, Physiologic , Adult , Caloric Tests , Eye Movements , Humans
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...