Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 9 de 9
Filter
Add more filters










Database
Language
Publication year range
1.
Child Dev ; 88(6): 2043-2059, 2017 Nov.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28124795

ABSTRACT

Visual information influences speech perception in both infants and adults. It is still unknown whether lexical representations are multisensory. To address this question, we exposed 18-month-old infants (n = 32) and adults (n = 32) to new word-object pairings: Participants either heard the acoustic form of the words or saw the talking face in silence. They were then tested on recognition in the same or the other modality. Both 18-month-old infants and adults learned the lexical mappings when the words were presented auditorily and recognized the mapping at test when the word was presented in either modality, but only adults learned new words in a visual-only presentation. These results suggest developmental changes in the sensory format of lexical representations.


Subject(s)
Child Development/physiology , Facial Recognition/physiology , Language , Learning/physiology , Speech Perception/physiology , Adult , Female , Humans , Infant , Male , Young Adult
2.
Behav Brain Res ; 231(2): 343-55, 2012 Jun 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22326374

ABSTRACT

Reaching-to-eat (skilled reaching) is a natural behaviour that involves reaching for, grasping and withdrawing a target to be placed into the mouth for eating. It is an action performed daily by adults and is among the first complex behaviours to develop in infants. During development, visually guided reaching becomes increasingly refined to the point that grasping of small objects with precision grips of the digits occurs at about one year of age. Integration of the hand, upper-limbs, and whole body are required for successful reaching, but the ontogeny of this integration has not been described. The present longitudinal study used Laban Movement Analysis, a behavioural descriptive method, to investigate the developmental progression of the use and integration of axial, proximal, and distal movements performed during visually guided reaching. Four infants (from 7 to 40 weeks age) were presented with graspable objects (toys or food items). The first prereaching stage was associated with activation of mouth, limb, and hand movements to a visually presented target. Next, reaching attempts consisted of first, the advancement of the head with an opening mouth and then with the head, trunk and opening mouth. Eventually, the axial movements gave way to the refined action of one upper-limb supported by axial adjustments. These findings are discussed in relation to the biological objective of reaching, the evolutionary origins of reaching, and the decomposition of reaching after neurological injury.


Subject(s)
Child Development/physiology , Consummatory Behavior/physiology , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Upper Extremity/physiology , Arm/physiology , Biomechanical Phenomena , Data Interpretation, Statistical , Feeding Behavior , Female , Hand/physiology , Hand Strength/physiology , Head Movements/physiology , Humans , Individuality , Infant , Longitudinal Studies , Male , Mouth/physiology , Movement/physiology , Sex Characteristics , Upper Extremity/growth & development , Video Recording
3.
Behav Neurosci ; 124(6): 851-67, 2010 Dec.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21133536

ABSTRACT

Reaching movements of the arm and hand become automatic early in development and are used throughout one's life span. Studies on skilled reaching have focused on the kinematic aspects and have advanced our knowledge of the individual motor components of reaching. It has also been shown that motor behaviors are organized in terms of ethologically relevant actions, rather than by motor components. Thus, it is important to analyze how the motor components of reaching are performed within the overall action as a whole. The objective of the present study was to examine the motor components of reaching-to-eat within the context of the overall behavior in stroke participants. Results show that reaching-to-eat involves the whole body to produce isolated actions of the limb and changes after stroke in three fundamental ways: abnormal use of nonkinematic aspects of movement, body-limb disintegration, and a disruption in the temporal aspect of the phases of reaching-to-eat. The movements within the behavior can reorganize, possibly a reflection of dynamic interactions between behavioral compensation and neuroplasticity, while the overall performance of the behavior remains the same. Such subtle flexibility may be part of the process of recovery.


Subject(s)
Arm/physiology , Eating/physiology , Motor Activity/physiology , Movement/physiology , Stroke/physiopathology , Aged , Analysis of Variance , Biomechanical Phenomena/physiology , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Recovery of Function/physiology , Video Recording
4.
Behav Brain Res ; 188(2): 281-90, 2008 Apr 09.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18155782

ABSTRACT

Trauma or stroke to motor cortex (MtCx) results in motor impairments that include movements of the contralateral forelimb in reaching for food that is to be placed in the mouth for eating (skilled reaching). In the rat, post-lesion recovery of success is incomplete and achieved using compensatory movements. A striking and puzzling feature of post-lesion performance is an increase in the numbers of reaching attempts. Whereas successful movements, whether normal or compensatory, have been extensively described, there has been no previous analysis of the movements comprising reach attempts, especially those that are unsuccessful. Here, rats pretrained in a single pellet reaching task received MtCx stroke via pial removal contralateral to the preferred-for-reaching forelimb. They then received daily physical rehabilitation and assessment in reaching. In addition to conventional end-point measures of performance, reaching behavior was evaluated by a new measure, gestures, derived from Laban Movement Analysis. Gestural analysis describes all non-weight bearing limb movements and so can document movements not explicitly directed to, or successful in, grasping food. In the acute post-stroke period, MtCx rats made few gestures, but thereafter gesture number escalated with recovery time, and eventually exceeded preoperative levels. Gestures were frequently repetitive and included combinations not used prior to stroke. The escalation in gestures number with recovery training suggests that excessive and inappropriate gestures may represent motor habits that substitute for, and compete with, successful movements. This description of "learned baduse" furthers the understanding of MtCx contributions to skilled movements and could potentially contribute to the modification of rehabilitative strategies for the treatment of stroke.


Subject(s)
Forelimb/physiopathology , Gestures , Learning/physiology , Motor Cortex/pathology , Motor Skills/physiology , Recovery of Function/physiology , Stroke , Analysis of Variance , Animals , Behavior, Animal , Disease Models, Animal , Feeding Behavior/physiology , Female , Functional Laterality/physiology , Movement/physiology , Rats , Rats, Long-Evans , Restraint, Physical/methods , Stroke/pathology , Stroke/physiopathology , Time Factors
5.
J Neurosci Methods ; 158(1): 137-49, 2006 Nov 15.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16766042

ABSTRACT

The purpose of this study was to adapt a universal language for human movement, Laban Movement Analysis (LMA), to capture the kinematic and non-kinematic aspects of movement in a reach-for-food task by subjects whose movements had been affected by stroke. Two control subjects, one stroke subject with internal capsule damage, and one subject with right posterior parietal stroke were video recorded while performing the reaching task. The movements of limb advancement, grasping the food, and limb withdrawal to place the food in the mouth, were notated using LMA. A scale, the Expressive Reaching Scale (ERS), was derived from the notation. All subjects completed the task; however, the stroke subjects displayed abnormalities in both the kinematic and non-kinematic aspects of movements during reaching with either limb. The most extensive impairments were in the contralateral-to-stroke limb and were most severe in the subject with internal capsule damage. The ERS rating scale may be a useful diagnosis and assessment tool.


Subject(s)
Hand Strength/physiology , Movement/physiology , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Stroke/physiopathology , Aged , Biomechanical Phenomena , Case-Control Studies , Functional Laterality , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Physical Exertion/physiology , Space Perception/physiology , Video Recording/methods , Weights and Measures
6.
Arch Sex Behav ; 35(2): 117-29, 2006 Apr.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16752116

ABSTRACT

Mounting is generally considered to be a male-typical behavior. Female Japanese macaques, in certain populations, are unusual, in that they routinely mount other females. In this study, we examined to what extent female Japanese macaques mount same-sex partners in a male-typical manner. We compared the mount postures males and females adopt and their rate of pelvic thrusting. In addition, we employed a modified form of Laban Movement Analysis (LMA) to compare patterns of pelvic movement during mounts. LMA is a universal language for movement that describes quantitative features of movement, such as changes in the relation of the body segments, as well as qualitative features, such as the style of movements. Our results indicate that female Japanese macaques do not mount in a male-typical manner. Females exhibited a much greater variety of mount postures than did males. Some of the most common types of mount postures employed by females were never exhibited by males. Females performed fewer pelvic thrusts per mount than males, but they executed more pelvic movements per mount, as well as, greater variety and complexity of movement. In addition, the qualitative style of pelvic mounting that females employed differed, in general, from that of males. We argue that these sex differences in mounting can be explained by the fact that both sexes sought sexual reward via genital stimulation during mounting, but they did so in different ways owing to the constraints imposed by their genital architecture. This study raises the larger question as to what constitutes a male-typical or female-typical behavior.


Subject(s)
Copulation , Macaca , Pair Bond , Posture , Animals , Female , Male , Sexual Behavior, Animal
7.
Behav Brain Res ; 149(1): 69-76, 2004 Feb 04.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-14739011

ABSTRACT

With the onset of puberty, play fighting in rats decreases in frequency and the tactics of attack and defense that are used are rougher. Previous studies have shown that the changes in the frequency of play and in the use of defensive tactics arise independently of social experience. Furthermore, while the former involves subcortical regulation, the latter depends on cortical mechanisms. In this study, the possible mechanisms regulating the developmental changes in the tactics of attack were examined. Two experiments were conducted using male rats. In the first study, rats reared in isolation from weaning were compared to rats reared in pairs, and were tested in the juvenile and early post-pubertal phases (30 and 60 days postnatally). In the second experiment, rats with their cortex removed shortly after birth were compared to sham-treated controls, and were tested in pairs at both the juvenile and early adult phases (30 and 90 days). Two measures of 'roughness,' derived from previous studies, were measured. Results showed that isolation-reared rats had the typical age-related changes in roughness of attack, whereas decorticated rats failed to show this age-related modulation, maintaining, or even exaggerating, the juvenile-typical pattern of attack. These findings suggest that social experience is not needed for this developmental change to occur, and that an intact cortex is needed to regulate this change in behavior.


Subject(s)
Aggression/physiology , Cerebral Cortex/physiology , Play and Playthings , Practice, Psychological , Sexual Maturation/physiology , Social Environment , Aging/physiology , Animals , Cerebral Cortex/growth & development , Cerebral Decortication , Male , Rats , Rats, Long-Evans
8.
Behav Brain Res ; 145(1-2): 221-32, 2003 Oct 17.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-14529819

ABSTRACT

Many rat strains are used for neurobiological studies of nervous system function and behavior. The most widely used strain for studies of the neural basis of movement is the out bred, pigmented Long-Evans strain, while the most widely used strains for the study of movement impairments in neurological disease are out bred albino rats, including Sprague-Dawley rats. Although previous research has indicated that there are strain differences in skilled movements displayed by different rat strains, there has been no explicit comparison of the Long-Evans and Sprague-Dawley strains. This was the purpose of the present study. The rats were video recorded as they learned to reach for single food pellets and the video records were subjected to frame-by-frame analysis. Component movements of reaching were scored using a system derived from Eshkol-Wachman Movement Notation (EWMN). The quality of movements was described using Laban Movement Analysis (LMA). Forelimb representations in motor cortex were defined electrophysiologically. Acquisition scores and success in reaching did not differ between the two strains, nor did the topographical representation of the forelimb in motor cortex. Long-Evans and Sprague-Dawley rats did differ in the movements used for reaching and on the quality of their movements. The movements of Sprague-Dawley rats resembled the movements of Long-Evans rats with motor system injury. That rat strains can show both quantitative and qualitative differences in movement is useful for the understanding of the genetic, neural, and behavioral organization of the motor system. The results are also relevant to the question of appropriateness of particular rat strains for studies of neurological diseases and the effects of albinism on motor behavior, and suggest that some of the most widely used rat strains for neurological investigations may be less than appropriate.


Subject(s)
Extremities/physiology , Motor Cortex/physiology , Movement/physiology , Rats, Long-Evans/physiology , Rats, Sprague-Dawley/physiology , Animals , Behavior, Animal , Brain Mapping , Electric Stimulation , Electrophysiology , Food Deprivation , Hand Strength , Male , Motor Cortex/anatomy & histology , Motor Skills , Neurobiology/methods , Psychomotor Performance , Rats , Selection, Genetic , Species Specificity , Time Factors , Video Recording
9.
Dev Psychobiol ; 42(1): 35-43, 2003 Jan.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-12471634

ABSTRACT

With increasing age, rats, when play fighting, become rougher. In part, this change can be accounted for by the increasing likelihood of using adult-typical fighting tactics. However, even when using the same tactics, adults appear rougher than juveniles in their play. In this study, videotaped sequences of play fighting in rats from the juvenile (30 days) to the post-pubertal (70 days) period were analyzed using Laban Movement Analysis (LMA). Movement qualities called Effort Factors in LMA captured the character of some of this change. Juveniles tended to use Indulging Efforts, whereas older rats tended to use Condensing Efforts. The latter are related to performing movements that are more controlled. This greater level of control was also evident in the way older rats maintained postural support during play fights. When standing over supine partners, juveniles are more likely to stand on the partner with all four paws, reducing their postural stability, and hence ability to control their partner's movements. Older rats are more likely to place their hind paws on the ground, thus providing a firmer anchor for movements with their upper bodies and forepaws. These age-related changes in behavior were found for both males and females. The findings lend support to a growing body of evidence that play fighting in the juvenile phase of rats is not just a more frequently occurring version of that present in adults, but rather, has unique organizational properties.


Subject(s)
Behavior, Animal , Movement/physiology , Social Behavior , Age Factors , Animals , Female , Male , Rats , Rats, Long-Evans , Sex Factors
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...