Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 20 de 20
Filter
1.
J Appl Behav Anal ; 57(2): 426-443, 2024 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38438321

ABSTRACT

The functional analysis approach described by Iwata et al. (1982/1994) has been used widely to determine the variables evoking and maintaining challenging behavior. However, one potential concern with conducting functional analyses is that repeated exposure to contingencies may induce a novel functional relation. To examine the likelihood of these potential iatrogenic effects, we evaluated social test conditions of the functional analysis for 116 participants and searched for patterns of responding indicative of acquisition. Patterns suggestive of acquisition occurred in 13.70% of tangible reinforcement conditions; however, the prevalence was only slightly lower in the attention condition (8.75%). Much lower prevalence was observed for the escape condition (2.13%). When grouped by quotient score, a pattern of acquisition was just as likely to be observed in the attention condition as in the tangible condition. Additionally, patterns indicative of acquisition were no more likely to be observed with participants who emitted automatically reinforced challenging behavior.


Subject(s)
Child Behavior Disorders , Reinforcement, Psychology , Humans , Child , Behavior Therapy , Attention , Probability
2.
J Appl Behav Anal ; 57(2): 490-501, 2024 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38239100

ABSTRACT

Renewal is a relapse phenomenon that refers to the recurrence of a previously reduced behavior following a change in stimulus conditions. Muething et al. (2022) examined the phenomenology of renewal among individuals with automatically maintained challenging behavior treated at an outpatient clinic. We replicated their findings by retrospectively examining renewal across various topographies of automatically maintained behavior treated at an inpatient hospital, and we extended their work by also examining differences across subtypes of automatically maintained self-injurious behavior. The prevalence of renewal was comparable to that observed by Muething et al., supporting the notion that automatically maintained challenging behavior is susceptible to relapse phenomena. Furthermore, renewal was twice as likely to occur for individuals with Subtype 2 versus Subtype 1 self-injurious behavior, providing additional evidence of behavioral differentiation between subtypes. Our findings suggest that even after apparent stability in treatment, practitioners should remain vigilant for the recurrence of automatically maintained behavior during generalization.


Subject(s)
Reinforcement, Psychology , Self-Injurious Behavior , Humans , Retrospective Studies , Self-Injurious Behavior/therapy , Generalization, Psychological , Recurrence
3.
J Appl Behav Anal ; 56(4): 869-883, 2023.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37464543

ABSTRACT

Augmenting competing stimulus assessments by embedding tactics to increase stimulus engagement and disrupt challenging behavior can improve outcomes of treatments for automatically maintained self-injurious behavior, even for treatment-resistant subtypes. This study replicated and extended research on augmented competing stimulus assessments by reporting outcomes for 16 consecutively encountered cases with automatically maintained self-injurious behavior (19 total applications); six participants had treatment-responsive Subtype 1 and 10 had treatment-resistant Subtypes 2 or 3. Fewer high-competition stimuli were identified for individuals with treatment-resistant subtypes. When response-promotion and disruption tactics were applied and withdrawn, outcomes improved for six of eight applications. At least one high-competition stimulus was still effective when maintenance probes were conducted for a subset of participants. Ultimately, at least one high-competition stimulus was identified for each participant. Although augmenting tactics may not be necessary for all individuals, these findings provide additional support for their efficacy with treatment-resistant self-injurious behavior.

4.
J Appl Behav Anal ; 56(3): 575-592, 2023 06.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37249167

ABSTRACT

Subtypes of automatically maintained self-injurious behavior (SIB) have been defined based on response patterns observed during the functional analysis, which are thought to reflect each subtype's distinct mechanisms. Current practice for identifying subtypes involves using structured criteria to identify whether SIB is automatically maintained, followed by the application of additional criteria to identify the subtype. We describe a series of studies directed at simplifying these methods to facilitate wider application of the subtyping model in research and practice. In Studies 1 and 2, we demonstrate the accuracy of modified criteria using the level of differentiation of SIB across the play and no-interaction conditions at distinguishing between two subtypes (i.e., Subtypes 1 and 2). We then demonstrate visual analysis (without structured criteria) can accurately identify SIB as automatically maintained in Study 3, which can be used in combination with level of differentiation to enable application of the subtyping model in practice.


Subject(s)
Self-Injurious Behavior , Humans , Self-Injurious Behavior/diagnosis
5.
J Appl Behav Anal ; 54(4): 1566-1585, 2021 09.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34341998

ABSTRACT

Contingent positive reinforcement has proven more effective in treating escape-maintained problem behavior than contingent negative reinforcement, particularly when problem behavior continues to produce escape. However, this research has overwhelmingly used distributed-reinforcement arrangements, where tasks and reinforcer access are interspersed throughout the work period. An alternative to interspersal involves allowing the individual to accumulate and then receive a larger quantity of reinforcement once work requirements are completed; this is known as an accumulated-reinforcement arrangement. The current study examined the efficacy of, and preference for positive (food) and negative (break) reinforcement contingencies delivered in accumulated and distributed arrangements in the treatment of escape-maintained problem behavior. In Experiment 1, accumulated break was preferred for 4 of 5 participants and accumulated food was preferred for 3 of 5. In Experiment 2, accumulated break was similarly effective to distributed break for 3 of 5 participants and accumulated and distributed food were equally effective for 4 participants.


Subject(s)
Problem Behavior , Behavior Therapy , Food , Humans , Reinforcement Schedule , Reinforcement, Psychology
6.
Behav Anal Pract ; 14(4): 883-892, 2021 Dec.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34150186

ABSTRACT

Since the arrival of the novel coronavirus, recommendations for public masking have emerged to decrease infection rates. For a variety of reasons, tolerating wearing a mask is challenging for many individuals with intellectual and developmental disabilities (IDDs). Therefore, we evaluated behavioral strategies to promote compliance with wearing a mask with six hospitalized individuals diagnosed with IDDs. One participant was compliant with wearing the mask for extended durations during baseline while engaging in various activities (e.g., academics, leisure). For the other five individuals, engagement in activities alone was ineffective. Blocking mask removal, reinforcement for mask wearing, and noncontingent access to preferred activities or competing stimuli were then evaluated using a changing-criterion design in which the duration participants were required to tolerate the mask gradually increased. Increases in compliance with mask wearing were achieved with all participants; however, the terminal duration was attained for only four of the five individuals. SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION: The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1007/s40617-021-00583-7.

7.
J Appl Behav Anal ; 53(4): 2172-2185, 2020 09.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32588920

ABSTRACT

Competing stimulus assessments (CSAs) are designed to identify stimuli that, when made freely available, reduce problem behavior. Although CSAs have demonstrated utility, identifying competing stimuli can be difficult for some individuals. The current study describes outcomes from an augmented CSA (A-CSA) for 6 consecutively encountered cases with treatment-resistant subtypes of automatically maintained problem behavior. When test stimuli were made freely available, only between 0 and 1 effective competing stimuli were identified for each case. Prompting and response blocking were temporarily employed in succession to promote engagement with stimuli and disrupt problem behavior. When those procedures were withdrawn and stimuli made freely available, the number of effective competing stimuli increased in all 6 cases. Findings suggest that procedures designed to promote engagement and disrupt problem behavior may allow the A-CSA to be a platform not only for identifying competing stimuli, but also for actively establishing competing stimuli.


Subject(s)
Applied Behavior Analysis , Autism Spectrum Disorder/psychology , Problem Behavior , Adolescent , Child , Female , Humans , Male , Reinforcement, Psychology , Young Adult
8.
J Autism Dev Disord ; 49(9): 3543-3556, 2019 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31124029

ABSTRACT

We examined autism spectrum disorder (ASD) risk in a large national sample of 203 individuals with Down syndrome, 6-25 years old, to determine the association of ASD risk with age, sex, IQ, adaptive behaviors, and maladaptive behaviors. We used a two-pronged approach by (1) considering ASD symptomatology continuously across the sample of individuals with DS and examining associations with each characteristic, and (2) dichotomizing our sample into high and low ASD risk groups and comparing groups on each characteristic. The pattern of results was largely similar across both types of analyses. ASD symptomatology/risk was negatively associated with IQ and adaptive behaviors and positively associated with certain types of maladaptive behaviors. Clinical implications for screening and therapeutic purposes are discussed.


Subject(s)
Autism Spectrum Disorder/epidemiology , Down Syndrome/epidemiology , Adaptation, Psychological , Adolescent , Adult , Child , Down Syndrome/psychology , Female , Humans , Intelligence , Male
9.
Am J Intellect Dev Disabil ; 123(6): 514-528, 2018 11.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30421968

ABSTRACT

The cause of the high degree of variability in cognition and behavior among individuals with Down syndrome (DS) is unknown. We hypothesized that birth defects requiring surgery in the first years of life (congenital heart defects and gastrointestinal defects) might affect an individual's level of function. We used data from the first 234 individuals, age 6-25 years, enrolled in the Down Syndrome Cognition Project (DSCP) to test this hypothesis. Data were drawn from medical records, parent interviews, and a cognitive and behavior assessment battery. Results did not support our hypothesis. That is, we found no evidence that either birth defect was associated with poorer outcomes, adjusting for gender, race/ethnicity, and socioeconomic status. Implications for study design and measurement are discussed.


Subject(s)
Behavioral Symptoms/epidemiology , Cognitive Dysfunction/epidemiology , Digestive System Abnormalities/epidemiology , Down Syndrome/epidemiology , Heart Defects, Congenital/epidemiology , Adolescent , Adult , Child , Female , Humans , Male , Young Adult
10.
J Appl Behav Anal ; 51(2): 276-282, 2018 04.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29536535

ABSTRACT

Predictions made under low response requirements inherent in most preference assessments (PAs) do not guarantee the utility of stimuli in treatments. We examined whether PA rank would predict how well stimuli supported compliance for children with escape-maintained problem behavior by examining the relation between PA rank and demand elasticity across five fixed-ratio values. Three patterns were observed: All stimuli were selected equally across values, higher ranked stimuli were selected more at higher values, and something other than the highest ranked stimulus was selected more at higher values (i.e., mixed correspondence).


Subject(s)
Food Preferences/psychology , Problem Behavior/psychology , Choice Behavior , Female , Humans , Male , Reinforcement Schedule , Reinforcement, Psychology
11.
J Exp Anal Behav ; 109(1): 253-264, 2018 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29380436

ABSTRACT

Behavioral momentum theory is a quantitative framework used to characterize the persistence of behavior during response disruptors as a function of baseline stimulus-reinforcer relations. Results of several investigations have shown that alternative reinforcement can increase the resistance to change of a target response during extinction. In the present study, concomitant variable-interval fixed-time schedules of reinforcement for problem behavior were employed to simulate naturalistic situations involving the superimposition of response-independent reinforcers on a baseline schedule of reinforcement for problem behavior, as in the common use of noncontingent reinforcement treatments. Resistance to change of problem behavior was assessed during postsession periods of extinction by comparing response rates in extinction following sessions with and without additional reinforcer deliveries arranged by fixed-time schedules. For 2 out of 3 participants, problem behavior tended to be more resistant to extinction following periods in which additional fixed-time reinforcers were delivered. These results are discussed in terms of potential effects of noncontingent reinforcement on problem behavior when the intervention is discontinued or implemented without good treatment integrity.


Subject(s)
Behavior Therapy/methods , Child Behavior Disorders/therapy , Reinforcement, Psychology , Adolescent , Autism Spectrum Disorder/therapy , Child , Extinction, Psychological , Humans , Male , Psychological Theory , Treatment Outcome
12.
Autism ; 21(7): 881-895, 2017 10.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27368350

ABSTRACT

One of the key diagnostic criteria for autism spectrum disorder includes impairments in social interactions. This study compared the extent to which boys with high-functioning autism and typically developing boys "value" engaging in activities with a parent or alone. Two different assessments that can empirically determine the relative reinforcing value of social and non-social stimuli were employed: paired-choice preference assessments and progressive-ratio schedules. There were no significant differences between boys with high-functioning autism and typically developing boys on either measure. Moreover, there was a strong correspondence in performance across these two measures for participants in each group. These results suggest that the relative reinforcing value of engaging in activities with a primary caregiver is not diminished for children with autism spectrum disorder.


Subject(s)
Autistic Disorder/psychology , Parent-Child Relations , Reinforcement, Psychology , Social Behavior , Autism Spectrum Disorder/psychology , Case-Control Studies , Child , Humans , Interpersonal Relations , Male , Parents
13.
J Appl Behav Anal ; 50(2): 206-221, 2017 04.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27739068

ABSTRACT

The overjustification hypothesis suggests that extrinsic rewards undermine intrinsic motivation. Extrinsic rewards are common in strengthening behavior in persons with intellectual and developmental disabilities; we examined overjustification effects in this context. A literature search yielded 65 data sets permitting comparison of responding during an initial no-reinforcement phase to a subsequent no-reinforcement phase, separated by a reinforcement phase. We used effect sizes to compare response levels in these two no-reinforcement phases. Overall, the mean effect size did not differ from zero; levels in the second no-reinforcement phase were equally likely to be higher or lower than in the first. However, in contrast to the overjustification hypothesis, levels were higher in the second no-reinforcement phase when comparing the single no-reinforcement sessions immediately before and after reinforcement. Outcomes consistent with the overjustification hypothesis were somewhat more likely when the target behavior occurred at relatively higher levels prior to reinforcement.


Subject(s)
Developmental Disabilities/psychology , Intellectual Disability/psychology , Internal-External Control , Motivation/physiology , Reinforcement, Psychology , Databases, Bibliographic/statistics & numerical data , Female , Humans , Male
14.
J Exp Anal Behav ; 106(1): 34-57, 2016 07.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27282331

ABSTRACT

Three experiments explored the impact of different reinforcer rates for alternative behavior (DRA) on the suppression and post-DRA relapse of target behavior, and the persistence of alternative behavior. All experiments arranged baseline, intervention with extinction of target behavior concurrently with DRA, and post-treatment tests of resurgence or reinstatement, in two- or three-component multiple schedules. Experiment 1, with pigeons, arranged high or low baseline reinforcer rates; both rich and lean DRA schedules reduced target behavior to low levels. When DRA was discontinued, the magnitude of relapse depended on both baseline reinforcer rate and the rate of DRA. Experiment 2, with children exhibiting problem behaviors, arranged an intermediate baseline reinforcer rate and rich or lean signaled DRA. During treatment, both rich and lean DRA rapidly reduced problem behavior to low levels, but post-treatment relapse was generally greater in the DRA-rich than the DRA-lean component. Experiment 3, with pigeons, repeated the low-baseline condition of Experiment 1 with signaled DRA as in Experiment 2. Target behavior decreased to intermediate levels in both DRA-rich and DRA-lean components. Relapse, when it occurred, was directly related to DRA reinforcer rate as in Experiment 2. The post-treatment persistence of alternative behavior was greater in the DRA-rich component in Experiment 1, whereas it was the same or greater in the signaled-DRA-lean component in Experiments 2 and 3. Thus, infrequent signaled DRA may be optimal for effective clinical treatment.


Subject(s)
Conditioning, Operant , Reinforcement Schedule , Animals , Child , Columbidae , Female , Humans , Male , Recurrence , Reinforcement, Psychology
15.
PLoS One ; 11(3): e0149646, 2016.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26933844

ABSTRACT

Copy number variants (CNVs) were detected and analyzed in 14 probands with autism and intellectual disability with self-injurious behavior (SIB) resulting in tissue damage. For each proband we obtained a clinical history and detailed behavioral descriptions. Genetic anomalies were observed in all probands, and likely clinical significance could be established in four cases. This included two cases having novel, de novo copy number variants and two cases having variants likely to have functional significance. These cases included segmental trisomy 14, segmental monosomy 21, and variants predicted to disrupt the function of ZEB2 (encoding a transcription factor) and HTR2C (encoding a serotonin receptor). Our results identify variants in regions previously implicated in intellectual disability and suggest candidate genes that could contribute to the etiology of SIB.


Subject(s)
DNA Copy Number Variations/genetics , Genetic Predisposition to Disease/genetics , Self-Injurious Behavior/genetics , Adolescent , Autistic Disorder/genetics , Child , Chromosomes, Human, Pair 14/genetics , Chromosomes, Human, Pair 21/genetics , Female , Homeodomain Proteins/genetics , Humans , Intellectual Disability/genetics , Male , Monosomy/genetics , Receptor, Serotonin, 5-HT2C/genetics , Repressor Proteins/genetics , Trisomy/genetics , Zinc Finger E-box Binding Homeobox 2
16.
Behav Interv ; 30(1): 1-35, 2015 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26236145

ABSTRACT

Severe problem behavior (e.g., self-injury and aggression) remains among the most serious challenges for the habilitation of persons with intellectual disabilities and is a significant obstacle to community integration. The current standard of behavior analytic treatment for problem behavior in this population consists of a functional assessment and treatment model. Within that model, the first step is to assess the behavior-environment relations that give rise to and maintain problem behavior, a functional behavioral assessment. Conventional methods of assessing behavioral function include indirect, descriptive, and experimental assessments of problem behavior. Clinical investigators have produced a rich literature demonstrating the relative effectiveness for each method, but in clinical practice, each can produce ambiguous or difficult-to-interpret outcomes that may impede treatment development. This paper outlines potential sources of variability in assessment outcomes and then reviews the evidence on strategies for avoiding ambiguous outcomes and/or clarifying initially ambiguous results. The end result for each assessment method is a set of best practice guidelines, given the available evidence, for conducting the initial assessment.

17.
J Appl Behav Anal ; 47(2): 293-313, 2014.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24782203

ABSTRACT

We assessed the efficacy of, and preference for, accumulated access to reinforcers, which allows uninterrupted engagement with the reinforcers but imposes an inherent delay required to first complete the task. Experiment 1 compared rates of task completion in 4 individuals who had been diagnosed with intellectual disabilities when reinforcement was distributed (i.e., 30-s access to the reinforcer delivered immediately after each response) and accumulated (i.e., 5-min access to the reinforcer after completion of multiple consecutive responses). Accumulated reinforcement produced response rates that equaled or exceeded rates during distributed reinforcement for 3 participants. Experiment 2 used a concurrent-chains schedule to examine preferences for each arrangement. All participants preferred delayed, accumulated access when the reinforcer was an activity. Three participants also preferred accumulated access to edible reinforcers. The collective results suggest that, despite the inherent delay, accumulated reinforcement is just as effective and is often preferred by learners over distributed reinforcement.


Subject(s)
Child Behavior Disorders/rehabilitation , Choice Behavior/physiology , Motivation , Reinforcement, Psychology , Activities of Daily Living , Adolescent , Child Behavior Disorders/psychology , Educational Status , Female , Humans , Male , Reinforcement Schedule , Token Economy , Young Adult
18.
J Appl Behav Anal ; 45(1): 143-8, 2012.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22403457

ABSTRACT

The delivery of food contingent on 10 s of consecutive toy engagement resulted in a decrease in engagement and a corresponding increase in other responses that had been previously reinforced with food. Similar effects were not observed when tokens exchangeable for the same food were delivered, suggesting that engagement was disrupted by the contingent provision of the food, which may have functioned as a discriminative stimulus that occasioned competing responses.


Subject(s)
Association Learning , Developmental Disabilities/rehabilitation , Discrimination Learning , Food , Probability , Reinforcement, Psychology , Association Learning/physiology , Child , Developmental Disabilities/etiology , Humans , Male
19.
J Appl Behav Anal ; 44(3): 543-58, 2011.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21941384

ABSTRACT

This study examined how the amount of effort required to produce a reinforcer influenced subsequent preference for, and strength of, that reinforcer in 7 individuals with intellectual disabilities. Preference assessments identified four moderately preferred stimuli for each participant, and progressive-ratio (PR) analyses indexed reinforcer strength. Stimuli were then assigned to one of four conditions for 4 weeks: fixed-ratio (FR) 1 schedule, escalating FR schedule, yoked noncontingent (NCR) delivery, and restricted access. Preference assessments and PR schedules were then repeated to examine changes in selection percentages and PR break points. Selection percentages decreased for all NCR stimuli but increased for most of the restricted stimuli. There were no systematic changes in selection percentages for either of the contingent stimuli. Break points increased, on average, for all conditions, but the increase was highest for the restricted stimuli and lowest for the NCR stimuli. These results are discussed in relation to recent basic research addressing the influence of effort on stimulus value.


Subject(s)
Behavior Therapy/methods , Developmental Disabilities/rehabilitation , Reinforcement Schedule , Adolescent , Adult , Child , Decision Making , Developmental Disabilities/psychology , Female , Humans , Male , Paired-Associate Learning , Young Adult
20.
Behav Anal Pract ; 2(2): 4-10, 2009.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22477702

ABSTRACT

Sidney W. Bijou's contributions to the practice of behavior analysis spanned nearly 7 decades, and they continue to influence what is considered "best practice" in our field today. By extending basic behavioral phenomena conducted with nonhuman animals to children in laboratory and naturalistic settings, Bijou's early research sparked a watershed of similar extensions. More importantly, Bijou's approach has led to a greater understanding of child development and behavior. This tribute will highlight what may be three of Bijou's most significant contributions to practice, namely his studies on (a) programmed instruction for children with developmental disabilities (Bijou, Birnbrauer, Kidder, & Tague, 1966), (b) children's responding under schedules of reinforcement and extinction (Bijou, 1957), and (c) the use of descriptive analysis to identify functional relations (Bijou, Peterson, & Ault, 1968).

SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...