Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 14 de 14
Filter
Add more filters










Publication year range
1.
Neuroimage Clin ; 18: 903-911, 2018.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29876275

ABSTRACT

Mechanisms underlying the self/other distinction have been mainly investigated focusing on visual, tactile or proprioceptive cues, whereas very little is known about the contribution of acoustical information. Here the ability to distinguish between self and others' voice is investigated by using a neuropsychological approach. Right (RBD) and left brain damaged (LBD) patients and healthy controls were submitted to a voice discrimination and a voice recognition task. Stimuli were paired words/pseudowords pronounced by the participant, by a familiar or unfamiliar person. In the voice discrimination task, participants had to judge whether two voices were same or different, whereas in the voice recognition task participants had to judge whether their own voice was or was not present. Crucially, differences between patient groups were found. In the discrimination task, only RBD patients were selectively impaired when their own voice was present. By contrast, in the recognition task, both RBD and LBD patients were impaired and showed two different biases: RBD patients misattributed the other's voice to themselves, while LBD patients denied the ownership of their own voice. Thus, two kinds of bias can affect self-voice recognition: we can refuse self-stimuli (voice disownership), or we can misidentify others' stimuli as our own (embodiment of others' voice). Overall, these findings reflect different impairments in self/other distinction both at behavioral and anatomical level, the right hemisphere being involved in voice discrimination and both hemispheres in the voice identity explicit recognition. The finding of selective brain networks dedicated to processing one's own voice demonstrates the relevance of self-related acoustic information in bodily self-representation.


Subject(s)
Brain Injuries/physiopathology , Brain/physiology , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Recognition, Psychology/physiology , Voice/physiology , Adult , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Brain/physiopathology , Female , Functional Laterality/physiology , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Visual Perception/physiology
2.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24110235

ABSTRACT

This paper presents an upper extremity exoskeleton with an original application in neuroscience. The novelty of this study is the investigation of the self-advantage phenomenon under various experimental conditions. Usually this kind of experiments lies only on human visual ability to explicitly and/or implicitly recognize their own arm movements. Using an exoskeleton to replay recorded trajectories allows to give another perspective to the previous studies in including the proprioceptive ability of humans. Twelve healthy subjects were involved in this study. The results show that the self advantage phenomenon is even more present in the implicit tasks.


Subject(s)
Arm/physiology , Movement , Adult , Humans , Proprioception , Recognition, Psychology , Robotics , Young Adult
3.
J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry ; 75(10): 1401-10, 2004 Oct.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15377685

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVES: The evolutionary pattern of spontaneous recovery from acute neglect was studied by assessing cognitive deficits and motor impairments. Detailed lesion reconstruction was also performed to correlate the presence of and recovery from neglect to neural substrates. METHODS: A consecutive series of right brain-damaged (RBD) patients with and without neglect underwent weekly tests in the acute phase of the illness. The battery assessed neglect deficits, neglect-related deficits, and motor impairment. Age-matched normal subjects were also investigated to ascertain the presence of non lateralised attentional deficits. Some neglect patients were also available for later investigation during the chronic phase of their illness. RESULTS: Partial recovery of neglect deficits was observed at the end of the acute period and during the chronic phase. Spatial attention was impaired in acute neglect patients, while non spatial attentional deficits were present in RBD patients with and without acute neglect. A strong association was found between acute neglect and fronto-parietal lesions. Similar lesions were associated with neglect persistence. In the chronic stage, neglect recovery was paralleled by improved motor control of the contralesional upper limb, thus emphasising that neglect is a negative prognostic factor in motor functional recovery. CONCLUSIONS: These findings show that spatial attention deficits partially improve during the acute phase of the disease in less than half the patients investigated. There was an improvement in left visuospatial neglect at a later, chronic stage of the disease, but this recovery was not complete.


Subject(s)
Brain Injuries/complications , Brain Injuries/psychology , Perceptual Disorders/etiology , Perceptual Disorders/pathology , Space Perception , Stroke/complications , Stroke/psychology , Acute Disease , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Attention , Brain Injuries/pathology , Case-Control Studies , Female , Functional Laterality , Humans , Longitudinal Studies , Male , Middle Aged , Motor Skills Disorders/etiology , Motor Skills Disorders/pathology , Neurologic Examination , Prognosis , Remission, Spontaneous
4.
Neurology ; 62(5): 749-56, 2004 Mar 09.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15007125

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVE: To assess the relative frequency of occurrence of motor, perceptual, peripersonal, and personal neglect subtypes, the association of neglect and other related deficits (e.g., deficient nonlateralized attention, anosognosia), and the neuroanatomic substrates of neglect in patients with right hemisphere stroke in rehabilitation settings. METHODS: The authors assessed 166 rehabilitation inpatients and outpatients with right hemisphere stroke with measures of neglect and neglect subtypes, attention, motor and sensory function, functional disability, and family burden. Detailed lesion analyses were also performed. RESULTS: Neglect was present in 48% of right hemisphere stroke patients. Patients with neglect had more motor impairment, sensory dysfunction, visual extinction, basic (nonlateralized) attention deficit, and anosognosia than did patients without neglect. Personal neglect occurred in 1% and peripersonal neglect in 27%, motor neglect in 17%, and perceptual neglect in 21%. Neglect severity predicted scores on the Functional Independence Measure and Family Burden Questionnaire more accurately than did number of lesioned regions. CONCLUSIONS: The neglect syndrome per se, rather than overall stroke severity, predicts poor outcome in right hemisphere stroke. Dissociations between tasks assessing neglect subtypes support the existence of these subtypes. Finally, neglect results from lesions at various loci within a distributed system mediating several aspects of attention and spatiomotor performance.


Subject(s)
Perceptual Disorders/etiology , Stroke/complications , Activities of Daily Living , Adult , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Brain/pathology , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Perceptual Disorders/classification , Stroke Rehabilitation
5.
Neuropsychologia ; 39(7): 725-33, 2001.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11311302

ABSTRACT

Recent studies have reported that left neglect can be ameliorated during active movements of a contralesional limb in the contralesional space. In contrast, a passive left hand movement does not seem to induce an amelioration of neglect, at least when it is associated to simultaneous active right movement (Robertson IH, North N, Neuropsychologia 31 (1993) 293-300). In the present study, we explored the possibility that a complex passive movement, such as abduction and adduction of the arm, is able to reduce neglect also when it is associated to simultaneous active right arm movements. To test this hypothesis neglect patients were required to perform an object cancellation test and a line bisection test by using the right hand, while the left arm was passively moved. Moreover, we verified the possibility that left arm stimulation activates the peripersonal more than the extrapersonal space, with the exception of the condition in which the far space can be reached by a tool that extends peripersonal space in the far space (Farnè A, Làdavas E, Neuroreport 11 (2000) 1645-1649). For this reason, patients were required to perform the tasks in near (70 cm) and in far (140 cm) space by means of a light pen (pointing task) and of a stick (reaching task). When the left arm was passively moved the results showed a significant reduction of neglect with respect to the baseline condition, and the improvement equally affected the near and the far space. A different effect for the near and far space was observed in relation to the task (pointing vs. reaching). In the pointing task, neglect was more severe in the far than in near space; however, this difference disappeared when the patients had to reach objects by means of a stick. In conclusion, the present study shows that the entity of improvement of visual neglect due to a left passive movement is related to the entity of proprioceptive signals specifying left hand position.


Subject(s)
Movement , Proprioception , Aged , Arm , Female , Functional Laterality , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Perceptual Disorders , Space Perception
6.
Curr Biol ; 10(22): 1475-7, 2000 Nov 16.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11102814

ABSTRACT

Brain areas exist that appear to be specialized for the coding of visual space surrounding the body (peripersonal space). In marked contrast to neurons in earlier visual areas, cells have been reported in parietal and frontal lobes that effectively respond only when visual stimuli are located in spatial proximity to a particular body part (for example, face, arm or hand) [1-4]. Despite several single-cell studies, the representation of near visual space has scarcely been investigated in humans. Here we focus on the neuropsychological phenomenon of visual extinction following unilateral brain damage. Patients with this disorder may respond well to a single stimulus in either visual field; however, when two stimuli are presented concurrently, the contralesional stimulus is disregarded or poorly identified. Extinction is commonly thought to reflect a pathological bias in selective vision favoring the ipsilesional side under competitive conditions, as a result of the unilateral brain lesion [5-7]. We examined a parietally damaged patient (D.P.) to determine whether visual extinction is modulated by the position of the hands in peripersonal space. We measured the severity of visual extinction in a task which held constant visual and spatial information about stimuli, while varying the distance between hands and stimuli. We found that selection in the affected visual field was remarkably more efficient when visual events were presented in the space near the contralesional finger than far from it. However, the amelioration of extinction dissolved when hands were covered from view, implying that the effect of hand position was not mediated purely through proprioception. These findings illustrate the importance of the spatial relationship between hand position and object location for the internal construction of visual peripersonal space in humans.


Subject(s)
Attention , Hand , Parietal Lobe/physiopathology , Vision, Ocular/physiology , Adult , Humans , Male
7.
J Cogn Neurosci ; 12(3): 415-20, 2000 May.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10931768

ABSTRACT

Far (extrapersonal) and near (peripersonal) spaces are behaviorally defined as the space outside the hand-reaching distance and the space within the hand-reaching distance. Animal and human studies have confirmed this distinction, showing that space is not homogeneously represented in the brain. In this paper we demonstrate that the coding of space as "far" and "near" is not only determined by the hand-reaching distance, but it is also dependent on how the brain represents the extension of the body space. We will show that when the cerebral representation of body space is extended to include objects or tools used by the subject, space previously mapped as far can be remapped as near. Patient P.P., after a right hemisphere stroke, showed a dissociation between near and far spaces in the manifestation of neglect. Indeed, in a line bisection task, neglect was apparent in near space, but not in far space when bisection in the far space was performed with a projection lightpen. However, when in the far space bisection was performed with a stick, used by the patient to reach the line, neglect appeared and was as severe as neglect in the near space. An artificial extension of the patient's body (the stick) caused a remapping of far space as near space.


Subject(s)
Agnosia/physiopathology , Space Perception/physiology , Stroke/physiopathology , Aged , Cerebral Cortex/physiology , Female , Fingers , Humans , Movement , Orientation/physiology
8.
Brain ; 122 ( Pt 2): 339-50, 1999 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10071061

ABSTRACT

We describe a patient (P.S.) who, following a right prestriate lesion, reported that objects in the left visual field appeared distorted and smaller than those on the right. Other aspects of visual processing were remarkably unaffected. We carried out a series of size comparison tests using simple or complex stimuli and requiring different types of behavioural responses. We found that P.S. significantly underestimated the size of stimuli presented in her left visual field. When comparison tasks involved stimuli placed along the vertical axis or in the right visual field, P.S. performed well. The vertical and horizontal components of size distortion were found to be differentially affected. We conclude that size processing may be dissociated from other aspects of visual processing, such as form or colour processing, and depends critically on part of the occipital, prestriate areas (Brodmann areas 18-19).


Subject(s)
Cerebrovascular Disorders/physiopathology , Functional Laterality/physiology , Perceptual Distortion/physiology , Size Perception/physiology , Visual Cortex/physiopathology , Aged , Cerebrovascular Disorders/diagnosis , Discrimination Learning/physiology , Female , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Photic Stimulation , Visual Fields/physiology
9.
Neuroreport ; 9(5): 835-9, 1998 Mar 30.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9579675

ABSTRACT

Studies of normal behaviour have shown that the process of selection takes a finite time, one measure of which is the attentional dwell time, that is the period of interference produced by one attended stimulus on a subsequent one. Here we investigated the time for selection in FB, a neurological patient suffering from a visuospatial disorder of attention (unilateral extinction). FB was asked to identify two letters displayed in rapid succession either to the left (damaged), or to the right (intact) visual hemifield. By varying the interval between stimuli, we measured how long the first letter continued to interfere with accuracy on the second- that is the first letter's attentional demand over time. The results showed that the process of selection has an abnormal duration in the affected visual field, being at least twice as long as in the intact field. We suggest that the slowed visual processing for the contralesional object may contribute to the competitive bias against that object which is the hallmark of unilateral extinction.


Subject(s)
Attention/physiology , Extinction, Psychological/physiology , Nervous System Diseases/physiopathology , Space Perception/physiology , Visual Perception/physiology , Aged , Cerebral Infarction/physiopathology , Cerebral Infarction/psychology , Humans , Male , Nervous System Diseases/psychology , Visual Fields/physiology
10.
Neuropsychologia ; 35(9): 1215-23, 1997 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9364492

ABSTRACT

Despite the fact that visual extinction is widely considered a space-based disturbance of selective attention, there has been little theoretical consensus about the nature of its pathogenic mechanism. A specific disruption in the ability to disengage attention from ipsilesional stimuli, or a loss of weight with which contralesional objects compete for visual selection, have been hypothesized to account for the disorder. We tested the merits of these two explanations in a right-hemisphere-lesioned patient, FB, who failed to recognize a contralesional target only when it was shown concurrently to an ipsilesional target (i.e. visual extinction). His task was to report two target letters presented in rapid succession to the left and right of the fixation point. The order of stimulus presentation (Left-First vs Right-First), and the intertarget interval (stimulus onset asynchrony) were varied systematically. We showed that contralesional extinction may occur for successively presented targets, not just for stimuli displayed at the same time. Of most importance, FB was seriously and equally impaired in dealing with a contralesional stimulus when this either preceded the ipsilesional stimulus or followed it by an interval less than about 600 msec. The data appear to contradict the disengagement hypothesis, which predicted a substantial reduction of extinction when a stimulus was displayed first into the lesioned side of space. We suggest that a competitive model of visual selective attention fits the data quite well.


Subject(s)
Functional Laterality , Photic Stimulation , Space Perception , Aged , Attention , Cerebral Infarction/complications , Cerebral Infarction/pathology , Fixation, Ocular , Humans , Male , Parietal Lobe/pathology , Perceptual Disorders/diagnosis , Perceptual Disorders/etiology
11.
Neuroreport ; 7(13): 2111-4, 1996 Sep 02.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8930969

ABSTRACT

We have studied the distortion of perceived time in a patient with left neglect. This patient consistently over-estimated the duration of stimuli in the neglected space. Overestimation was observed both with an interval comparison (300/700 ms) and with a time production (1 s) paradigm. We suggest that encoding duration in the hundreds of milliseconds range is a process based on an internal clock mechanism. The functioning of that clock varies as a function of the processing load.


Subject(s)
Biological Clocks , Brain Ischemia/physiopathology , Time Perception , Attention , Brain Ischemia/psychology , Functional Laterality , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Visual Perception
12.
Cortex ; 31(4): 767-77, 1995 Dec.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8750033

ABSTRACT

A persistent line of inquiry for the students of visuo-spatial neglect has involved the perceptual frame of reference respect to which the neglected region of space is defined. On standard testing conditions viewer-centered and object-centered systems of coordinates are confounded. In order to disambiguate these two reference frames FB, a patient with severe left visual neglect consequent upon a right parieto-temporal haemorrhage, was asked to identify chimeric figures presented at different orientations. FB continued to recognize poorly the left side of chimeric figures even when the display was rotated 90 degrees clockwise or anticlockwise so that the 'left' of the chimeric fell on the patient's egocentric up or down, respectively. The result suggests that, at least under the present testing conditions, unilateral neglect is tied to the principal (top-bottom) axis of the object. Object-centered vs. (viewer-centered) representational accounts of this finding are discussed.


Subject(s)
Orientation/physiology , Perceptual Disorders/psychology , Space Perception , Aged , Cerebral Arteries/diagnostic imaging , Cerebral Hemorrhage/diagnostic imaging , Cerebral Hemorrhage/psychology , Functional Laterality/physiology , Humans , Male , Motion Perception/physiology , Neuropsychological Tests , Perceptual Disorders/diagnostic imaging , Photic Stimulation , Tomography, X-Ray Computed
13.
Cortex ; 31(2): 331-43, 1995 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-7555010

ABSTRACT

Neglect patients' attention is usually pathologically captured by stimuli present on the side ipsilateral to the brain damage. In previous research it was shown that not only the mere presence but also the number of "relevant" stimuli on the right side influenced patient's performance. However, the influence of stimuli whose presence was completely irrelevant for the execution of the task was not previously studied. We asked neglect patients to bisect lines of 28 cm of length, which could be presented alone or with line-drawings of objects placed either only on the left, only on the right or bilaterally on the display. We found that the presence of irrelevant left-side stimuli improved the bisection performance in 3 out of 5 patients with respect to the baseline condition, in which, no stimuli were present on either side of the display. Right-side stimuli did not affect the performance, whereas bilateral stimuli tended to worsen the performance for those patients who presented the beneficial effect of left-side stimuli. These findings were discussed with reference to the hypotheses advanced to explain neglect.


Subject(s)
Attention , Brain Diseases/diagnosis , Functional Laterality , Neuropsychological Tests , Visual Fields , Adult , Aged , Form Perception , Humans , Middle Aged , Photic Stimulation , Task Performance and Analysis
14.
Cortex ; 30(2): 181-97, 1994 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-7924344

ABSTRACT

In this study we present evidence which supports the view that reading mechanisms, if implicit assessed, are available also in the presence of a severe deficit of spatial awareness. A Stroop-like task was performed by a right brain-damaged patient affected by severe extrapersonal neglect and neglect dyslexia. In reading words and color words, the patient showed the usual pattern of neglect errors; omission, substitution and addition errors. However, when asked to name the colors in which color words were written, naming time was found to be affected by the meaning of those words he was not able to read correctly. The pattern of results in MD and in a group of normal subjects, who performed a modified version of the Stroop test performed by MD, have been interpreted as evidence of MD's implicit reading of the left-hand letters of color words during the Stroop test. The theoretical implications of this finding are discussed. Moreover, the comparison between the performance of MD and the performance of another group of normal subjects suggested that implicit processing in MD was carried out at a lower level of efficiency than in normals.


Subject(s)
Attention/physiology , Awareness/physiology , Brain Damage, Chronic/physiopathology , Dominance, Cerebral/physiology , Dyslexia, Acquired/physiopathology , Reading , Adult , Brain Damage, Chronic/diagnosis , Brain Damage, Chronic/psychology , Cerebral Cortex/physiopathology , Color Perception/physiology , Discrimination Learning/physiology , Dyslexia, Acquired/diagnosis , Dyslexia, Acquired/psychology , Hemiplegia/diagnosis , Hemiplegia/physiopathology , Hemiplegia/psychology , Humans , Male , Mental Recall/physiology , Neuropsychological Tests , Semantics
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...