Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 20 de 20
Filter
Add more filters










Publication year range
1.
Sci Rep ; 11(1): 11240, 2021 05 27.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34045501

ABSTRACT

Eye movements toward sequentially presented face images with or without gaze cues were recorded to investigate whether those with ASD, in comparison to their typically developing (TD) peers, could prospectively perform the task according to gaze cues. Line-drawn face images were sequentially presented for one second each on a laptop PC display, and the face images shifted from side-to-side and up-and-down. In the gaze cue condition, the gaze of the face image was directed to the position where the next face would be presented. Although the participants with ASD looked less at the eye area of the face image than their TD peers, they could perform comparable smooth gaze shift to the gaze cue of the face image in the gaze cue condition. This appropriate gaze shift in the ASD group was more evident in the second half of trials in than in the first half, as revealed by the mean proportion of fixation time in the eye area to valid gaze data in the early phase (during face image presentation) and the time to first fixation on the eye area. These results suggest that individuals with ASD may benefit from the short-period trial experiment by enhancing the usage of gaze cue.


Subject(s)
Attention/physiology , Autism Spectrum Disorder/physiopathology , Eye Movements/physiology , Face , Adolescent , Cues , Female , Fixation, Ocular/physiology , Humans , Male , Young Adult
2.
Front Psychol ; 11: 587080, 2020.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33343460

ABSTRACT

In a study concerning visual body part recognition, a "self-advantage" effect, whereby self-related body stimuli are processed faster and more accurately than other-related body stimuli, was revealed, and the emergence of this effect is assumed to be tightly linked to implicit motor simulation, which is activated when performing a hand laterality judgment task in which hand ownership is not explicitly required. Here, we ran two visual hand recognition tasks, namely, a hand laterality judgment task and a self-other discrimination task, to investigate (i) whether the self-advantage emerged even if implicit motor imagery was assumed to be working less efficiently and (ii) how individual traits [such as autistic traits and the extent of positive self-body image, as assessed via the Autism Spectrum Quotient (AQ) and the Body Appreciation Scale-2 (BAS-2), respectively] modulate performance in these hand recognition tasks. Participants were presented with hand images in two orientations [i.e., upright (egocentric) and upside-down (allocentric)] and asked to judge whether it was a left or right hand (an implicit hand laterality judgment task). They were also asked to determine whether it was their own, or another person's hand (an explicit self-other discrimination task). Data collected from men and women were analyzed separately. The self-advantage effect in the hand laterality judgment task was not revealed, suggesting that only two orientation conditions are not enough to trigger this motor simulation. Furthermore, the men's group showed a significant positive correlation between AQ scores and reaction times (RTs) in the laterality judgment task, while the women's group showed a significant negative correlation between AQ scores and differences in RTs and a significant positive correlation between BAS-2 scores and dprime in the self-other discrimination task. These results suggest that men and women differentially adopt specific strategies and/or execution processes for implicit and explicit hand recognition tasks.

3.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33182290

ABSTRACT

Although the hand is an important organ in interpersonal interactions, focusing on this body part explicitly is less common in daily life compared with the face. We investigated (i) whether a person's recognition of their own hand is different from their recognition of another person's hand (i.e., self hand vs. other's hand) and (ii) whether a close social relationship affects hand recognition (i.e., a partner's hand vs. an unknown person's hand). For this aim, we ran an experiment in which participants took part in one of two discrimination tasks: (i) a self-others discrimination task or (ii) a partner/unknown opposite-sex person discrimination task. In these tasks, participants were presented with a hand image and asked to select one of two responses, self (partner) or other (unknown persons), as quickly and accurately as possible. We manipulated hand ownership (self (partner)/other(unknown person)), hand image laterality (right/left), and visual perspective of hand image (upright/upside-down). A main effect of hand ownership in both tasks (i.e., self vs. other and partner vs. unknown person) was found, indicating longer reaction times for self and partner images. The results suggest that close social relationships modulate hand recognition-namely, "self-expansion" to a romantic partner could occur at explicit visual hand recognition.


Subject(s)
Hand , Interpersonal Relations , Recognition, Psychology , Humans , Reaction Time , Sexual Partners
4.
Front Hum Neurosci ; 12: 430, 2018.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30405382

ABSTRACT

It is known that motor actions performed by individuals with autism spectrum disorders (ASD) are clumsy and a previous study revealed that children with ASD of around 8 years old showed less smooth movement and dysfunction of appropriate usage of online vision for grip aperture control. The present study investigates whether and how the kinematic properties of reach-to-grasp movements in older adolescents and adults with ASD [mean (±SD) age: 18.3 ± 2.1] differ from those in typically developing (TD) peers [mean (±SD) age: 19.1 ± 2.2]. Revealing the kinematic properties of reach-to-grasp movements in older adolescents and adults with ASD is indispensable in determining the developmental trajectory of this motor behavior in individuals with ASD. While wearing liquid crystal shutter goggles, participants reached for and grasped a cylinder with a diameter of either 4 or 6 cm. Two visual conditions were tested: a full vision (FV) condition (the goggles remained transparent during the movement) and a no vision (NV) condition (the goggles were closed immediately after the movement was initiated). These two visual conditions were either alternated with each trial in a single experimental session (alternated condition) or blocked within the session (blocked condition). We found that the reaching movement smoothness calculated as a normalized jerk score (i.e., index of skilled, coordinated human movements) of ASD participants did not differ significantly from that of TD peers although ASD participants showed smoother reaching in the alternated condition than in the blocked condition. The influence of online vision and its visual condition schedule on grip aperture during the in-flight phase was remarkably similar between the ASD and TD groups. Furthermore, we found that ASD group experienced a significant longer transition period from grasping end (i.e., stable holding when touching the surface of the object) to uplift initiation than the TD group. The results suggest that (1) deficits in movement smoothness and the use of online vision for motor control are rectified by the time individuals with ASD reach late adolescence and (2) older adolescents and adults with ASD still have difficulties chaining motor acts.

5.
Dev Sci ; 21(3): e12565, 2018 05.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28635049

ABSTRACT

Pointing is one of the communicative actions that infants acquire during their first year of life. Based on a hypothesis that early pointing is triggered by emergent reaching behavior toward objects placed at out-of-reach distances, we proposed a neural network model that acquires reaching without explicit representation of 'targets'. The proposed model controls a two-joint arm in a horizontal plane, and it learns a loop of internal forward and inverse transformations; the former predicts the visual feedback of hand position and the latter generates motor commands from the visual input through random generation of the motor commands. In the proposed model, the motor output and visual input were represented by broadly tuned neural units. Even though explicit 'targets' were not presented during learning, the simulation successfully generated reaching toward visually presented objects at within-reach and out-of-reach distances.


Subject(s)
Infant Behavior/physiology , Movement/physiology , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Feedback, Sensory , Hand/physiology , Humans , Infant , Learning , Neural Networks, Computer , Neurons/physiology
6.
J Neurol Sci ; 368: 344-8, 2016 Sep 15.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27538662

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVE: Acute multifocal embolic infarction (AMEI) is conventionally caused by etiologies such as cardioembolism due to atrial fibrillation (Af), but can also be caused by serious underlying diseases such as cancer. We characterized cancer-related AMEI and identified useful indicators for cancer-associated strokes. METHODS: A retrospective analysis was performed on 35 patients with Af-related AMEI and 35 patients with cancer-related AMEI selected from 1235 consecutive patients with acute infarcts. All patients received diffusion-weighted magnetic resonance (MR) imaging. Cerebral MR angiography, carotid and cardiac ultrasonography, electrocardiogram-monitoring and whole body computed tomography were also performed on these patients. D-dimer levels were evaluated on admission, and were measured during the sub-acute phase in 19 of the patients with Af and 27 of the patients with cancer. RESULTS: Acute phase D-dimer levels were significantly higher in patients with cancer than in patients with Af alone. The cut-off D-dimer value to identify cancer-associated infarcts was 2.0µg/mL. D-dimer levels during the sub-acute phase remained elevated in the cancer patients. CONCLUSIONS: We may differentiate cancer-associated AMEI from Af using a D-dimer level≥2.0µg/mL, which does not decrease during the sub-acute phase.


Subject(s)
Atrial Fibrillation/etiology , Neoplasms/complications , Stroke/complications , Aged , Atrial Fibrillation/diagnostic imaging , Female , Fibrin Fibrinogen Degradation Products/metabolism , Humans , Male , Neoplasms/classification , Neoplasms/diagnostic imaging , Neoplasms/metabolism , Neuroimaging , ROC Curve , Retrospective Studies , Stroke/diagnostic imaging
7.
Front Comput Neurosci ; 9: 143, 2015.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26696874

ABSTRACT

In human reach-to-grasp movement, visual occlusion of a target object leads to a larger peak grip aperture compared to conditions where online vision is available. However, no previous computational and neural network models for reach-to-grasp movement explain the mechanism of this effect. We simulated the effect of online vision on the reach-to-grasp movement by proposing a computational control model based on the hypothesis that the grip aperture is controlled to compensate for both motor variability and sensory uncertainty. In this model, the aperture is formed to achieve a target aperture size that is sufficiently large to accommodate the actual target; it also includes a margin to ensure proper grasping despite sensory and motor variability. To this end, the model considers: (i) the variability of the grip aperture, which is predicted by the Kalman filter, and (ii) the uncertainty of the object size, which is affected by visual noise. Using this model, we simulated experiments in which the effect of the duration of visual occlusion was investigated. The simulation replicated the experimental result wherein the peak grip aperture increased when the target object was occluded, especially in the early phase of the movement. Both predicted motor variability and sensory uncertainty play important roles in the online visuomotor process responsible for grip aperture control.

8.
J Neurol Sci ; 359(1-2): 236-40, 2015 Dec 15.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26671120

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: Although most patients with Parkinson's disease (PD) show decreased cardiac (123)I-metaiodobenzylguanidine (MIBG) uptake, some exhibit normal uptake. We evaluated the clinical characteristics of such patients. METHODS: We enrolled 154 non-demented patients showing parkinsonism with normal cardiac MIBG uptake and had been clinically followed up during 29.9 ± 27.6 months. We defined the patients who did not fit the exclusion criteria for PD and demonstrated ≥ 30% reduction in the Unified Parkinson's Disease Rating Scale (UPDRS) motor score after anti-Parkinson agent administration as probable PD. We compared clinical characteristics and the cardiac MIBG heart-to-mediastinum (H/M) ratio between the probable PD group (N=37) and other groups (N=117). RESULTS: The probable PD group showed significantly higher UPDRS motor scores and greater incidence of tremor/rigidity than those of other groups. In addition, they showed a significantly lower cardiac MIBG H/M ratio in the delayed phase (delayed, p<0.0001). Washout-rate (WR) was significantly higher in probable PD cases (p<0.0001). Among 16 probable PD patients undergoing serial cardiac MIBG scintigraphy, the delayed phase cardiac MIBG H/M ratio showed a significant decrease and WR significantly increased during follow-up periods. CONCLUSIONS: An increase in WR and lower delayed phase cardiac MIBG uptake were found to be characteristics of such patients.


Subject(s)
3-Iodobenzylguanidine/pharmacokinetics , Enzyme Inhibitors/pharmacokinetics , Heart/drug effects , Parkinsonian Disorders/diagnostic imaging , Adult , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Female , Heart/diagnostic imaging , Humans , Iodine Radioisotopes/pharmacokinetics , Male , Middle Aged , Parkinsonian Disorders/physiopathology , ROC Curve , Radionuclide Imaging , Retrospective Studies , Severity of Illness Index
9.
Exp Brain Res ; 233(8): 2257-68, 2015 Aug.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25929554

ABSTRACT

Our previous study (Hum Mov Sci 25:349-371, 2006) investigated whether and how online vision in the early phase of movement influences the control of reach-to-grasp movements (movement duration: approximately 1000 ms). We used liquid-crystal shutter goggles to manipulate the duration of available online vision during the movement and specified that online vision during the early phase influences grasping movements. The current study examined the effect of online early phase vision on the grip configuration according to the movement duration and compared it between two different movement durations (approximately 500 and 1000 ms). We found that non-availability of early phase online vision affected the grip configuration (i.e., inducing a larger peak grip aperture) even in the shorter movement duration. The influential period for online vision for grasping control shifts to an earlier time when movement time is shorter (i.e., from approximately 214 to 106 ms after movement onset), indicating a flexible mechanism for grip configuration according to the movement duration and the available online vision.


Subject(s)
Motor Activity/physiology , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Visual Perception/physiology , Adult , Female , Humans , Male , Time Factors , Young Adult
10.
Intern Med ; 54(2): 215-8, 2015.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25743015

ABSTRACT

A 74-year-old woman was hospitalized due to dysuria, weakness and dysesthesia of the lower extremities. She was in an immunosuppressed state following the administration of methylprednisolone therapy for idiopathic interstitial pneumonia. Cerebrospinal fluid and blood cultures were negative, and no infectious biomarkers were found. A gadolinium (Gd)-enhanced T1-weighted image of magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) revealed disseminated nodular lesions along the spinal cord. We suspected a diagnosis of seronegative deep mycosis and initiated anti-fungal therapy with voriconazole, which subsequently alleviated all of the patient's symptoms and MRI findings. Therefore, the presence of Gd-enhanced disseminated nodules on spinal MRI may be a good marker of deep meningeal mycosis.


Subject(s)
Meningitis, Fungal/diagnosis , Meningitis, Fungal/drug therapy , Spinal Cord/pathology , Voriconazole/therapeutic use , Aged , Female , Gadolinium DTPA , Humans , Immunocompromised Host , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Methylprednisolone/administration & dosage
11.
Toxicology ; 331: 112-8, 2015 May 04.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25758465

ABSTRACT

Clioquinol is considered to be a causative agent of subacute myelo-optico neuropathy (SMON), although the pathogenesis of SMON is yet to be elucidated. We have previously shown that clioquinol inhibits nerve growth factor (NGF)-induced Trk autophosphorylation in PC12 cells transformed with human Trk cDNA. To explore the further mechanism of neuronal damage by clioquinol, we evaluated the acetylation status of histones in PC12 cells. Clioquinol reduced the level of histone acetylation, and the histone deacetylase (HDAC) inhibitor Trichostatin A upregulated acetylated histones and prevented the neuronal cell damage caused by clioquinol. In addition, treatment with HDAC inhibitor decreased neurite retraction and restored the inhibition of NGF-induced Trk autophosphorylation by clioquinol. Thus, clioquinol induced neuronal cell death via deacetylation of histones, and HDAC inhibitor alleviates the neurotoxicity of clioquinol. Clioquinol is now used as a potential medicine for malignancies and neurodegenerative diseases. Therefore, HDAC inhibitors can be used as a candidate medicine for the prevention of its side effects on neuronal cells.


Subject(s)
Clioquinol/toxicity , Histone Deacetylase Inhibitors/pharmacology , Histones/metabolism , Hydroxamic Acids/toxicity , Neurons/drug effects , Neuroprotective Agents/pharmacology , Acetylation , Animals , Cell Death/drug effects , Cell Shape/drug effects , Cytoprotection , Humans , Neurons/enzymology , Neurons/pathology , PC12 Cells , Phosphorylation , Rats , Receptor, trkA/drug effects , Receptor, trkA/genetics , Receptor, trkA/metabolism , Signal Transduction/drug effects , Time Factors , Transfection
12.
Brain Res ; 1596: 13-21, 2015 Jan 30.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25454796

ABSTRACT

Activation of the high-affinity nerve growth factor (NGF) receptor Trk occurs through multiple processes consisted of translocation and clustering within the plasma membrane lipid rafts, dimerization and autophosphorylation. Here we found that a nonprotein extract of inflamed rabbit skin inoculated with vaccinia virus (Neurotropin(®)) enhanced efficiency of NGF signaling. In rat pheochromocytoma PC12 cells overexpressing Trk (PCtrk cells), Neurotropin augmented insufficient neurite outgrowth observed at suboptimal concentration of NGF (2ng/mL) in a manner depending on Trk kinase activity. Cellular exposure to Neurotropin resulted in an accumulation of Trk-GM1 complexes without affecting dimerization or phosphorylation states of Trk. Following NGF stimulation, Neurotropin significantly facilitated the time course of NGF-induced Trk autophosphorylation. These observations provide a unique mechanism controlling efficiency of NGF signaling, and raise the therapeutic potential of Neurotropin for various neurological conditions associated with neurotrophin dysfunction.


Subject(s)
Analgesics/pharmacology , G(M1) Ganglioside/metabolism , Nerve Growth Factor/metabolism , Polysaccharides/pharmacology , Receptor Protein-Tyrosine Kinases/metabolism , Signal Transduction/drug effects , Animals , Carbazoles/pharmacology , Cell Differentiation/drug effects , Dimerization , Dose-Response Relationship, Drug , Indole Alkaloids/pharmacology , Neurites/drug effects , PC12 Cells , Rats , Time Factors
13.
Brain Res ; 1583: 237-44, 2014 Oct 02.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25128605

ABSTRACT

Neuromyelitis optica (NMO) is an inflammatory demyelinating disease of the central nervous system and is considered to be caused by the binding of NMO-IgG to aquaporin 4 (AQP4) on astrocytes, which initiates complement-dependent cytotoxicity. AQP4 has two isoforms, i.e., M1 and M23. AQP4 is considered to form heterotetramers containing both isoforms in vivo. Most of the previous studies were performed using either one of the isoforms expressing cell lines. In this study, we generated a fluorescent epitope-tagged AQP4 M1 and M23 co-expressing astrocyte cell line and examined the subcellular targeting of AQP4. In this cell line, AQP4 was targeted mostly to membrane lipid rafts fraction evidenced by sucrose density gradient ultracentrifugation followed by Western blotting with anti-AQP4 antibody. Cholesterol depletion with methyl-ß-cyclodextrin or simvastatin resulted in the dislocation (relocation) of AQP4 from lipid rafts to non-rafts fraction of the membrane and AQP4 was not internalized intracellularly. This change in the localization of AQP4 on membrane significantly reduced complement-dependent cytotoxic effects of NMO-IgG obtained from patients with NMO without affecting AQP4 orthogonal arrays. Thus, these data strongly suggest that the targeting of AQP4 in the lipid rafts is closely related to the pathogenic effects of NMO-IgG.


Subject(s)
Aquaporin 4/metabolism , Membrane Microdomains/metabolism , Animals , Anticholesteremic Agents/pharmacology , Aquaporin 4/genetics , Astrocytes/drug effects , Astrocytes/metabolism , Blotting, Western , Cell Death/drug effects , Cell Death/physiology , Cell Line , Glial Fibrillary Acidic Protein , Humans , Immunoglobulin G/toxicity , Immunohistochemistry , Isomerism , Membrane Microdomains/drug effects , Mice , Microscopy, Fluorescence , Nerve Tissue Proteins/metabolism , Neuromyelitis Optica/immunology , Simvastatin/pharmacology , Subcellular Fractions/drug effects , Subcellular Fractions/metabolism , Transfection , beta-Cyclodextrins/pharmacology
14.
Front Behav Neurosci ; 8: 77, 2014.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24688460

ABSTRACT

When stepping onto a stopped escalator, we often perceive an "odd sensation" that is never felt when stepping onto stairs. The sight of an escalator provides a strong contextual cue that, in expectation of the backward acceleration when stepping on, triggers an anticipatory forward postural adjustment driven by a habitual and implicit motor process. Here we contrast two theories about why this postural change leads to an odd sensation. The first theory links the odd sensation to a lack of sensorimotor prediction from all low-level implicit motor processes. The second theory links the odd sensation to the high-level conflict between the conscious awareness that the escalator is stopped and the implicit perception that evokes an endogenous motor program specific to a moving escalator. We show very similar postural changes can also arise from reflexive responses to visual stimuli, such as contracting/expanding optic flow fields, and that these reflexive responses produce similar odd sensations to the stopped escalator. We conclude that the high-level conflict is not necessary for such sensations. In contrast, the implicitly driven behavioral change itself essentially leads to the odd sensation in motor perception since the unintentional change may be less attributable to self-generated action because of a lack of motor predictions.

15.
Neuropsychologia ; 55: 25-40, 2014 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24334110

ABSTRACT

Following the princeps investigations of Marc Jeannerod on action-perception, specifically, goal-directed movement, this review article addresses visual and non-visual processes involved in guiding the hand in reaching or grasping tasks. The contributions of different sources of correction of ongoing movements are considered; these include visual feedback of the hand, as well as the often-neglected but important spatial updating and sharpening of goal localization following gaze-saccade orientation. The existence of an automatic online process guiding limb trajectory toward its goal is highlighted by a series of princeps experiments of goal-directed pointing movements. We then review psychophysical, electrophysiological, neuroimaging and clinical studies that have explored the properties of these automatic corrective mechanisms and their neural bases, and established their generality. Finally, the functional significance of automatic corrective mechanisms-referred to as motor flexibility-and their potential use in rehabilitation are discussed.


Subject(s)
Arm/physiology , Hand/physiology , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Animals , Hand Strength/physiology , Humans , Visual Perception/physiology
16.
Hum Mov Sci ; 32(4): 580-95, 2013 Aug.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24054896

ABSTRACT

The aim of this study was to investigate the role of online vision of a target object and the participant's moving hand in the early phase of reach-to-grasp movements. We designed an experiment that separately manipulated the visibility of a moving hand and the target object by using two liquid crystal shutter plates placed in the same horizontal plane (25 cm above the experimental table). When target view was available immediately after movement onset, the effect of hand view in the early phase of movement was very limited. The effect of hand view appeared when target view in the early phase of movement was not available. This was even the case for the condition where the temporal range of non-availability of target view after movement initiation was 150 ms. Therefore, online vision (of target and hand) for controlling grasping was utilized in a flexible fashion that depended on the visual environment.


Subject(s)
Feedback, Sensory , Hand Strength , Motion Perception , Orientation , Proprioception , Psychomotor Performance , Reaction Time , Adolescent , Female , Humans , Male , Psychophysics , Sensory Deprivation , Size Perception , Young Adult
17.
Front Psychol ; 4: 44, 2013.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23404470

ABSTRACT

When performing the reach-to-grasp movement, fingers open wider than the size of a target object and then stop opening. The recorded peak grip aperture (PGA) is significantly larger when this action is performed without vision during the movement than with vision, presumably due to an error margin that is retained in order to avoid collision with the object. People can also pretend this action based on an internal target representation (i.e., pantomimed prehension), and previous studies have shown that kinematic differences exist between natural and pantomimed prehension. These differences are regarded as a reflection of variations in information processing in the brain through the dorsal and ventral streams. Pantomimed action is thought to be mediated by the ventral stream. This implies that visual information during the movement, which is essential to the dorsal stream, has little effect on the kinematic properties of pantomimed prehension. We investigated whether an online view of the external world affects pantomimed grasping, and more specifically, whether the dorsal stream is involved in its execution. Participants gazed at a target object and were then subjected to a 3-s visual occlusion, during which time the experimenter removed the object. The participants were then required to pretend to make a reach-to-grasp action toward the location where the object had been presented. Two visual conditions (full vision and no vision) were imposed during the pantomimed action by manipulating shutter goggles. The PGA showed significant differences between the two visual conditions, whereas no significant difference was noted for terminal grip aperture, which was recorded at the movement end. This suggests the involvement of the dorsal stream in pantomimed action and implies that pantomimed prehension is a good probe for revealing the mechanism of interaction between the ventral and dorsal streams, which is also linked to embodied cognition.

18.
PLoS One ; 7(5): e34985, 2012.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22563459

ABSTRACT

Previous studies demonstrated that human motor actions are not always monitored by perceptual awareness and that implicit motor control plays a key role in performing actions. In addition, appropriate evaluation of our own motor behavior is vital for human life. Here we combined a reaching task with a visual backward masking paradigm to induce an implicit motor response that is congruent or incongruent with the visual perception. We used this to investigate (i) how we evaluate such implicit motor response that could be inconsistent with perceptual awareness and (ii) the possible contributions of reaching error, external visual cues, and internal sensorimotor information to this evaluation. Participants were instructed, after each trial, to rate their own reaching performance on a 5-point scale (i.e., smooth-clumsy). They also needed to identify a color presented at a fixation point that could be changed just after the reaching start. The color was linked to the prime-mask congruency (i.e., congruent-green, incongruent-blue) in the practice phase, and then inconsistent pairs (congruent-blue or incongruent-green) were introduced in the test phase. We found early trajectory deviations induced by the invisible prime stimulus, and such implicit motor responses are significantly correlated with the action evaluation score. The results suggest the "conscious" action evaluation is properly monitoring online sensory outcomes derived by implicit motor control. Furthermore, statistical path analyses showed that internal sensorimotor information from the motor behavior modulated by the invisible prime was the predominant cue for the action evaluation, while the color-cue association learned in the practice phase in some cases biases the action evaluation in the test phase.


Subject(s)
Cues , Feedback, Sensory/physiology , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Visual Perception/physiology , Adult , Awareness/physiology , Female , Humans , Male , Photic Stimulation , Reaction Time/physiology , Young Adult
19.
PLoS One ; 4(6): e5782, 2009 Jun 03.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19492054

ABSTRACT

Our motor actions are sometimes not properly performed despite our having complete understanding of the environmental situation with a suitable action intention. In most cases, insufficient skill for motor control can explain the improper performance. A notable exception is the action of stepping onto a stopped escalator, which causes clumsy movements accompanied by an odd sensation. Previous studies have examined short-term sensorimotor adaptations to treadmills and moving sleds, but the relationship between the odd sensation and behavioral properties in a real stopped-escalator situation has never been examined. Understanding this unique action-perception linkage would help us to assess the brain function connecting automatic motor controls and the conscious awareness of action. Here we directly pose a question: Does the odd sensation emerge because of the unfamiliar motor behavior itself toward the irregular step-height of a stopped escalator or as a consequence of an automatic habitual motor program cued by the escalator itself. We compared the properties of motor behavior toward a stopped escalator (SE) with those toward moving escalator and toward a wooden stairs (WS) that mimicked the stopped escalator, and analyzed the subjective feeling of the odd sensation in the SE and WS conditions. The results show that moving escalator-specific motor actions emerged after participants had stepped onto the stopped escalator despite their full awareness that it was stopped, as if the motor behavior was guided by a "phantom" of a moving escalator. Additionally, statistical analysis reveals that postural forward sway that occurred after the stepping action is directly linked with the odd sensation. The results suggest a dissociation between conscious awareness and subconscious motor control: the former makes us perfectly aware of the current environmental situation, but the latter automatically emerges as a result of highly habituated visual input no matter how unsuitable the motor control is. This dissociation appears to yield an attribution conflict, resulting in the odd sensation.


Subject(s)
Motion Perception , Movement , Sensation , Adult , Awareness , Brain/physiology , Consciousness , Elevators and Escalators , Gait , Humans , Male , Motor Activity , Psychomotor Performance , Walking
20.
Hum Mov Sci ; 25(3): 349-71, 2006 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16707178

ABSTRACT

Two experiments were conducted to investigate (1) during which phase of the movement vision is most critical for control, and (2) how vision of the target object and the participant's moving limb affect the control of grasping during that movement phase. In Experiment 1, participants, wearing liquid crystal shutter goggles, reached for and grasped a cylinder with a diameter of 4 or 6 cm under a shutting paradigm (SP) and a re-opening paradigm (RP). In SP, the goggles closed (turned opaque) 0 ms, 150 ms, 350 ms, 500 ms, or 700 ms after movement onset, or remained open (transparent) during the prehension movements. In RP, the goggles closed immediately upon movement onset, and re-opened 0 ms (i.e., without initially shutting), 150 ms, 350 ms, 500 ms, or 700 ms after the initial shutting, or remained opaque throughout the prehension movements. The duration of the prehension movements was kept relatively constant across participants and trials at approximately 1100 ms, i.e., the duration of prehension movements typically observed in daily life. The location of the target object was constant during the entire experiment. The SP and RP paradigms were counter-balanced across participants, and the order of conditions within each session was randomized. The main findings were that peak grip aperture (PGA) in the 150 ms-shutting condition was significantly larger than in the 350 ms-shutting condition, and that PGA in the 350 ms-re-opening condition was significantly larger than in the 150 ms-re-opening condition. These results revealed that online vision between 150 ms and 350 ms was critical for grasp control on PGA in typical, daily-life-speeded prehension movements. Furthermore, the results obtained for the time after maximal deceleration (TAMD; movement duration-time to maximal deceleration) demonstrated that early-phase vision contributed to the temporal pattern of the later movement phases (i.e., TAMD). The results thus demonstrated that online vision in the early phase of movement is crucial for the control of grasping. In addition to the apparatus used in Experiment 1, two liquid shutter plates placed in the same horizontal plane (25 cm above the experimental table) were used in Experiment 2 to manipulate the visibility of the target and the participant's moving limb. The plate closest to the participant altered vision of the limb/hand, while the more distant plate controlled vision of the object. The conditions were as follows: (1) both plates were open during movement (full vision condition); (2) both plates were closed 0, 150, or 350 ms following onset of arm movement (front-rear condition: FR); or (3) only the near plate closed 0, 150, or 350 ms following the onset of the arm movement (front condition: F). The results showed that shutting at 0 and 150 ms in the FR condition caused a significantly larger PGA, while the timing of shutting in the F condition had little influence on the PGA. These findings indicated that online vision, especially of the target object, during the early phase of prehension movements is critical to the control of grasping.


Subject(s)
Arm , Hand Strength/physiology , Movement/physiology , Visual Perception , Humans , Motor Activity , Reaction Time , Wrist
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...