Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 4 de 4
Filter
Add more filters










Database
Language
Publication year range
1.
Vision Res ; 39(16): 2649-57, 1999 Aug.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10492827

ABSTRACT

In a previous study we found that blurred edges presented in peripheral vision look sharper than when they are looked at directly, a phenomenon we have called peripheral sharpness overconstancy (Galvin et al. (1997). Vision Research, 37, 2035-2039). In the current study we show that when visibility of the stimulus edges is compromised by very brief presentations, we can demonstrate sharpness overconstancy for static, foveal viewing. We also test whether the degree of sharpening is a function of the current visual context, but find no difference between the peripheral sharpness overconstancy (at 24 degrees eccentricity) of edges measured in a blurred context and that measured in a sharp context. We conclude that if the visual system does carry a template for sharp edges which contributes to edge appearance when visibility is poor, then that template is resistant to changes in context.


Subject(s)
Form Perception/physiology , Visual Fields , Adult , Contrast Sensitivity , Fovea Centralis/physiology , Humans , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Psychometrics , Sensory Thresholds/physiology , Time Factors
2.
Vision Res ; 37(15): 2035-9, 1997 Aug.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9327051

ABSTRACT

Although much has been learned about the spatial sampling and filtering properties of peripheral vision, little attention has been paid to the remarkably clear appearance of the peripheral visual field. To study the apparent sharpness of stimuli presented in the periphery, we presented Gaussian blurred horizontal edges at 8.3, 16.6, 24, 32, and 40 deg eccentricity. Observers adjusted the sharpness of a similar edge, viewed foveally, to match the appearance of the peripheral stimulus. All observers matched blurred peripheral stimuli with sharper foveal stimuli. We have called this effect "sharpness overconstancy". For field sizes of 4 deg, there was greater overconstancy at larger eccentricities. Scaling the field size of the peripheral stimuli by a cortical magnification factor produced sharpness overconstancy which was independent of eccentricity. In both cases, there was a slight sharpness underconstancy for peripherally presented edges blurred only slightly. We consider various explanations of peripheral sharpness overconstancy.


Subject(s)
Space Perception/physiology , Visual Fields , Adult , Female , Fovea Centralis/physiology , Humans , Male , Psychophysics , Visual Acuity
3.
Vision Res ; 36(15): 2283-95, 1996 Aug.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8776493

ABSTRACT

Motion reversal effects (the apparent reversal of the direction of motion of a high frequency sinusoidal grating) have been attributed to aliasing by the cone mosaic [Coletta et al. (1990). Vision Research, 30, 1631-1648] and postreceptoral layers [Anderson & Hess (1990). Vision Research, 30, 1507-1515] in human observers. We present data and a new model which suggest that at least two sampling arrays of different densities affect direction discrimination out to 30 degrees eccentricity. The first sampling layer matches anatomical estimates of the cone density. The second sampling layer is too dense to be the parasol cells alone; midget ganglion cells certainly contribute to this task. This is further evidence that motion perception is not mediated exclusively by the magnocellular stream.


Subject(s)
Motion Perception/physiology , Space Perception/physiology , Visual Fields , Discrimination, Psychological , Female , Fixation, Ocular , Humans , Mathematics , Models, Neurological , Psychometrics , Retinal Cone Photoreceptor Cells/physiology , Rotation
4.
Vision Res ; 32(12): 2251-9, 1992 Dec.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-1288002

ABSTRACT

Although spatial aliasing by the extrafoveal retina can occur under natural viewing conditions, it does not commonly disturb our vision. One possible explanation for this is that real scenes do not have sufficient power in the high frequencies to produce aliasing. We examined whether aliasing distorted the appearance of a high contrast edge, which is a common stimulus in the environment. Observers made a two-interval forced-choice discrimination between low-pass filtered and unfiltered edges at 0, 10, 20, and 40 deg eccentricity. This discrimination could be made only when frequency components were removed below both the cone and ganglion cell Nyquist frequencies at each eccentricity. Since supra-Nyquist frequency components could not be detected in edges, they are incapable of producing aliasing.


Subject(s)
Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Contrast Sensitivity/physiology , Discrimination, Psychological/physiology , Female , Humans , Male , Photoreceptor Cells/physiology , Retinal Ganglion Cells/physiology , Sensory Thresholds/physiology , Visual Acuity/physiology
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...