Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 4 de 4
Filter
Add more filters











Database
Language
Publication year range
1.
PLoS One ; 9(11): e111700, 2014.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25390034

ABSTRACT

INTRODUCTION: Electronic devices in the bedroom are broadly linked with poor sleep in adolescents. This study investigated whether there is a dose-response relationship between use of electronic devices (computers, cellphones, televisions and radios) in bed prior to sleep and adolescent sleep patterns. METHODS: Adolescents aged 11-17 yrs (n = 1,184; 67.6% female) completed an Australia-wide internet survey that examined sleep patterns, sleepiness, sleep disorders, the presence of electronic devices in the bedroom and frequency of use in bed at night. RESULTS: Over 70% of adolescents reported 2 or more electronic devices in their bedroom at night. Use of devices in bed a few nights per week or more was 46.8% cellphone, 38.5% computer, 23.2% TV, and 15.8% radio. Device use had dose-dependent associations with later sleep onset on weekdays (highest-dose computer adjOR  = 3.75: 99% CI  = 2.17-6.46; cellphone 2.29: 1.22-4.30) and weekends (computer 3.68: 2.14-6.32; cellphone 3.24: 1.70-6.19; TV 2.32: 1.30-4.14), and later waking on weekdays (computer 2.08: 1.25-3.44; TV 2.31: 1.33-4.02) and weekends (computer 1.99: 1.21-3.26; cellphone 2.33: 1.33-4.08; TV 2.04: 1.18-3.55). Only 'almost every night' computer use (: 2.43: 1.45-4.08) was associated with short weekday sleep duration, and only 'almost every night' cellphone use (2.23: 1.26-3.94) was associated with wake lag (waking later on weekends). CONCLUSIONS: Use of computers, cell-phones and televisions at higher doses was associated with delayed sleep/wake schedules and wake lag, potentially impairing health and educational outcomes.


Subject(s)
Adolescent Behavior , Computers , Sleep/physiology , Adolescent , Australia , Cell Phone , Child , Female , Humans , Internet , Male , Radio , Sleep Wake Disorders/epidemiology , Surveys and Questionnaires , Technology , Television
2.
J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry ; 41(1): 39-44, 2010 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19781689

ABSTRACT

This study investigated the time-course of attentional bias in socially phobic (SP) and non-phobic (NP) adults. Participants viewed angry and happy faces paired with neutral faces (i.e., face-face pairs) and angry, happy and neutral faces paired with household objects (i.e., face-object pairs) for 5000ms. Eye movement (EM) was measured throughout to assess biases in early and sustained attention. Attentional bias occurred only for face-face pairs. SP adults were vigilant for angry faces relative to neutral faces in the first 500ms of the 5000ms exposure, relative to NP adults. SP adults were also vigilant for happy faces over 500ms, although there were no group-based differences in attention to happy-neutral face pairs. There were no group differences in attention to faces throughout the remainder of the exposure. Results suggest that social phobia is characterised by early vigilance for social cues with no bias in subsequent processing.


Subject(s)
Attention/physiology , Bias , Emotions/physiology , Facial Expression , Phobic Disorders/physiopathology , Phobic Disorders/psychology , Adolescent , Adult , Eye Movements/physiology , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Neuropsychological Tests , Photic Stimulation , Reaction Time/physiology , Time Factors , Young Adult
3.
Child Youth Care Forum ; 38(6): 327-340, 2009 Dec 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19960111

ABSTRACT

Although sleep problems often comprise core features of psychiatric disorders, inadequate attention has been paid to the complex, reciprocal relationships involved in the early regulation of sleep, emotion, and behavior. In this paper, we review the pediatric literature examining sleep in children with primary psychiatric disorders as well as evidence for the role of early sleep problems as a risk factor for the development of psychopathology. Based on these cumulative data, possible mechanisms and implications of early sleep disruption are considered. Finally, assessment recommendations for mental health clinicians working with children and adolescents are provided toward reducing the risk of and improving treatments for sleep disorders and psychopathology in children and adolescents.

4.
J Anxiety Disord ; 23(7): 841-7, 2009 Oct.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19447004

ABSTRACT

This study examined the time-course of attentional bias in anxious and non-anxious children and adolescents aged 7-17 years using eye movement as an index of selective attention. Participants completed two eye-tracking tasks in which they viewed happy-neutral and negative-neutral face pairs for 3000 and 500 ms, respectively. When face pairs were presented for 3000 ms eye movement data showed no evidence of an attentional bias at any stage of attentional processing. When face pairs were presented for 500 ms a bias in initial orienting occurred; anxious adolescents directed their first fixation away from negative faces and anxious children directed their first fixation away from happy faces. Results suggest that childhood anxiety is characterized by a bias in initial orienting, with no bias in sustained attention, although only for briefly presented faces.


Subject(s)
Anxiety Disorders/psychology , Attention , Emotions , Facial Expression , Pattern Recognition, Visual , Adolescent , Anxiety Disorders/diagnosis , Child , Eye Movements , Female , Fixation, Ocular , Humans , Male , Reaction Time , Reference Values
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL