Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 20 de 28
Filter
1.
Clin EEG Neurosci ; 54(1): 36-50, 2023 Jan.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35758261

ABSTRACT

Background. Alzheimer's cognitive-behavioral syndrome is the result of impaired connectivity between nerve cells, due to misfolded proteins, which accumulate and disrupt specific brain networks. Electroencephalography, because of its excellent temporal resolution, is an optimal approach for assessing the communication between functionally related brain regions. Objective. To detect and compare EEG resting-state networks (RSNs) in patients with amnesic mild cognitive impairment (aMCI), and healthy elderly (HE). Methods. We recruited 125 aMCI patients and 70 healthy elderly subjects. One hundred and twenty seconds of artifact-free EEG data were selected and compared between patients with aMCI and HE. We applied standard low-resolution brain electromagnetic tomography (sLORETA)-independent component analysis (ICA) to assess resting-state networks. Each network consisted of a set of images, one for each frequency (delta, theta, alpha1/2, beta1/2). Results. The functional ICA analysis revealed 17 networks common to groups. The statistical procedure demonstrated that aMCI used some networks differently than HE. The most relevant findings were as follows. Amnesic-MCI had: i) increased delta/beta activity in the superior frontal gyrus and decreased alpha1 activity in the paracentral lobule (ie, default mode network); ii) greater delta/theta/alpha/beta in the superior frontal gyrus (i.e, attention network); iii) lower alpha in the left superior parietal lobe, as well as a lower delta/theta and beta, respectively in post-central, and in superior frontal gyrus(ie, attention network). Conclusions. Our study confirms sLORETA-ICA method is effective in detecting functional resting-state networks, as well as between-groups connectivity differences. The findings provide support to the Alzheimer's network disconnection hypothesis.


Subject(s)
Alzheimer Disease , Cognitive Dysfunction , Humans , Aged , Electroencephalography , Brain , Neuroimaging , Magnetic Resonance Imaging/methods
2.
Neurol Sci ; 39(8): 1471-1473, 2018 Aug.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29756180

ABSTRACT

Swallowing difficulties are a common symptom of multiple sclerosis (MS). The early detection and treatment of dysphagia is critical to prevent complications, including poor nutrition, dehydration, and lung infections. Recently, transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) has been proven to be effective in ameliorating swallowing problems in stroke patients. In this pilot study, we aimed to assess safety and efficacy of transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) in the treatment of dysphagia in MS patients. We screened 30 patients by using the 10-item DYsphagia in MUltiple Sclerosis (DYMUS) questionnaire, and patients at risk for dysphagia underwent a clinical and fiberoptic endoscopic evaluation of swallowing (FEES). Six patients who presented with mild to moderate dysphagia underwent the experimental procedures. These consisted of 5 sessions of anodal tDCS applied in consecutive days over the right swallowing motor cortex. Patients were followed-up at 1 week, 1 month and 3 months after treatment, and changes in the Dysphagia Outcome and Severity Scale (DOSS) score between baseline and post-tDCS were assessed. Our results showed that in all patients, the tDCS treatment determined a mild but significant clinical benefit (one-point improvement in the DOSS score) lasting up to 1 month. In conclusion, our preliminary results show that anodal tDCS has therapeutic potential in the treatment of swallowing problems in patients suffering with MS. However, future double-blind, randomized, and sham-controlled studies are needed to confirm the present findings.


Subject(s)
Deglutition Disorders/etiology , Deglutition Disorders/therapy , Motor Cortex/physiology , Multiple Sclerosis/complications , Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation/methods , Adult , Electrodes , Electromyography , Evoked Potentials, Motor/physiology , Female , Follow-Up Studies , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Pilot Projects , Severity of Illness Index , Treatment Outcome
3.
J Neurol ; 252(9): 1074-81, 2005 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15822002

ABSTRACT

We studied the time-course of a levodopa oral bolus effects on the kinematics of patients affected by a mild akinetic-rigid form of idiopathic Parkinson's disease (PD). Eleven PD patients were evaluated: a) in OFF-state, that is before their first medication or after its withdrawal, b) in ON-state, that is at 1/2, 1, 2, 3, 4, 5, 6, 24, 30 and 48 hours after the administration of 250 mg of levodopa plus 25mg of carbidopa. The main kinematics (i. e.movement time, peak of velocity, peak of acceleration and peak of deceleration) of pointing movements to six target-stimuli placed on the horizontal plane of a table were recorded. Clinical conditions were assessed according to the Motor Examination section of the Unified Parkinson's Disease Rating Scale. The levopoda bolus had stable clinical effects only within the first six hours from its administration. The decline of the clinical response was marked by the changes of peak acceleration whereas other kinematics (i. e. movement time and the peak of velocity) changed also in the late observations (24, 30 and 48 hours after drug intake). The dissociation between the persistent improvement on movement time on peak velocity and the rapid deterioration of levodopa effects on early kinematics (i. e. peak acceleration) could be accounted for by a progressive decline in movement programming.


Subject(s)
Antiparkinson Agents/administration & dosage , Levodopa/administration & dosage , Parkinson Disease/drug therapy , Psychomotor Performance/drug effects , Administration, Oral , Adult , Aged , Biomechanical Phenomena , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Time Factors
4.
Neurology ; 60(9): 1539-41, 2003 May 13.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-12743250

ABSTRACT

The authors investigated whether repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) at 1 Hz (12 minutes; 90% of motor threshold) to the primary motor cortex (M1) leads to changes in diffusion-weighted imaging (DWI). After the rTMS train, there was a temporary small restriction in diffusion within the targeted left M1 that disappeared after 5 minutes. These findings provide a physiologic correlate to the reported behavioral consequences of off-line 1-Hz rTMS and reveal the transitory nature of the effects.


Subject(s)
Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Magnetics , Motor Cortex/metabolism , Adult , Diffusion , Humans , Male , Time Factors
5.
Neuroimage ; 18(3): 565-75, 2003 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-12667834

ABSTRACT

We explored the temporal dynamics of parietal and prefrontal cortex involvement in verbal working memory employing single-pulse transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS). In six healthy volunteers the left or right inferior parietal and prefrontal cortex was stimulated with the aid of a frameless stereotactic system. TMS was applied at 10 different time points 140-500 ms into the delay period of a two-back verbal working memory task. A choice reaction task was used as a control task. Interference with task accuracy was induced by TMS earlier in the parietal cortex than in the prefrontal cortex and earlier over the right than the left hemisphere. This suggests a propagation of information flow from posterior to anterior cortical sites converging in the left prefrontal cortex. Significant interference with reaction time was observed after 180 ms with left prefrontal cortex stimulation. These effects were not observed in the control task, underlining the task specificity of our results. We propose that the interference with right-sided prefrontal cortex stimulation leads to impaired performance due to disturbed input into the left prefrontal cortex, whereas left-sided TMS interferes directly with the final information processing. Left- and right-sided brain areas might be involved in parallel processing of semantic and object features of the stimuli, respectively.


Subject(s)
Dominance, Cerebral/physiology , Electromagnetic Fields , Frontal Lobe/physiology , Magnetic Resonance Imaging/instrumentation , Memory, Short-Term/physiology , Neuronavigation/instrumentation , Parietal Lobe/physiology , Reaction Time/physiology , Verbal Learning/physiology , Adult , Attention/physiology , Brain Mapping , Humans , Male , Nerve Net/physiology , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Prefrontal Cortex/physiology , Reading
6.
Cereb Cortex ; 12(4): 369-75, 2002 Apr.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11884352

ABSTRACT

The functional organization of working memory (WM) in the human prefrontal cortex remains unclear. Storage and processing functions might be segregated in ventral and dorsal areas of the prefrontal cortex, respectively. If so, storage functions might be spared, irrespective of informational domain, following damage or dysfunction in dorsolateral areas. Alternatively, WM and prefrontal function in general might be segregated according to informational domains (e.g. spatial versus object-based information). In the present study we used repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) to directly test these competing hypotheses. We applied rTMS to transiently and selectively disrupt the function of the dorsomedial, dorsolateral or ventral prefrontal cortex in normal human volunteers performing either a spatial or a face-recognition delayed-response task. Performance in the spatial task was impaired by rTMS of the dorsomedial prefrontal cortex. Performance in the face-recognition (non-spatial) task was impaired by rTMS of the ventral prefrontal cortex. Transient disruption of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex affected performance in both tasks. These findings provide evidence of domain-specific segregation of WM functions in widely separated areas of prefrontal cortex.


Subject(s)
Electromagnetic Fields , Memory, Short-Term/physiology , Prefrontal Cortex/physiology , Vision, Ocular/physiology , Adult , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Male , Reaction Time/physiology , Visual Perception/physiology
7.
Neuroreport ; 12(17): 3845-8, 2001 Dec 04.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11726806

ABSTRACT

Repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) allows the modulation of intra-cortical excitability and may therefore affect the descending control of spinal excitability. We applied rTMS at subthreshold intensity and 1 Hz frequency for 10 min to the left primary motor cortex representation of the flexor carpi radialis muscle (FCR) in 10 subjects and assessed the H and M responses to median nerve stimulation before and after the rTMS. Following rTMS, H wave thresholds significantly reduced by approximately 20%. Maximal H but not M wave amplitude significantly increased over the baseline, so that H/M amplitude ratio was increased by 41%. Sham stimulation did not induce any noticeable change in M or H waves. Slow rTMS might facilitate monosynaptic spinal cord reflexes by inhibiting the cortico-spinal projections modulating spinal excitability.


Subject(s)
Electric Stimulation Therapy/methods , Motor Cortex/physiology , Neurons/physiology , Pyramidal Tracts/physiology , Reflex, Monosynaptic/physiology , Spinal Cord/physiology , Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation , Adult , Electromyography , Humans , Male , Membrane Potentials/physiology , Movement Disorders/physiopathology , Movement Disorders/therapy , Muscle Contraction/physiology , Muscle Spasticity/physiopathology , Muscle Spasticity/therapy , Muscle, Skeletal/innervation , Muscle, Skeletal/physiology , Neural Conduction/physiology , Reaction Time/physiology
8.
J Neurophysiol ; 86(4): 1685-99, 2001 Oct.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11600632

ABSTRACT

Neurons involved in grasp preparation with hand and mouth were previously recorded in the premotor cortex of monkey. The aim of the present kinematic study was to determine whether a unique planning underlies the act of grasping with hand and mouth in humans as well. In a set of four experiments, healthy subjects reached and grasped with the hand an object of different size while opening the mouth (experiments 1 and 3), or extending the other forearm (experiment 4), or the fingers of the other hand (experiment 5). In a subsequent set of three experiments, subjects grasped an object of different size with the mouth, while opening the fingers of the right hand (experiments 6-8). The initial kinematics of mouth and finger opening, but not of forearm extension, was affected by the size of the grasped object congruently with the size effect on initial grasp kinematics. This effect was due neither to visual presentation of the object, without the successive grasp motor act (experiment 2) nor to synchronism between finger and mouth opening (experiments 3, 7, and 8). In experiment 9 subjects grasped with the right hand an object of different size while pronouncing a syllable printed on the target. Mouth opening and sound production were affected by the grasped object size. The results of the present study are discussed according to the notion that in an action each motor act is prepared before the beginning of the motor sequence. Double grasp preparation can be used for successive motor acts on the same object as, for example, grasping food with the hand and ingesting it after bringing it to the mouth. We speculate that the circuits involved in double grasp preparation might have been the neural substrate where hand motor patterns used as primitive communication signs were transferred to mouth articulation system. This is in accordance with the hypothesis that Broca's area derives phylogenetically from the monkey premotor area where hand movements are controlled.


Subject(s)
Fingers/physiology , Hand Strength/physiology , Motor Neurons/physiology , Mouth/physiology , Adult , Biomechanical Phenomena , Female , Fingers/innervation , Frontal Lobe/cytology , Frontal Lobe/physiology , Functional Laterality/physiology , Humans , Male , Motor Cortex/cytology , Motor Cortex/physiology , Mouth/innervation , Movement/physiology , Speech/physiology
9.
J Cogn Neurosci ; 13(6): 713-20, 2001 Aug 15.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11564316

ABSTRACT

Selective deficits in producing verbs relative to nouns in speech are well documented in neuropsychology and have been associated with left hemisphere frontal cortical lesions resulting from stroke and other neurological disorders. The basis for these impairments is unresolved: Do they arise because of differences in the way grammatical categories of words are organized in the brain, or because of differences in the neural representation of actions and objects? We used repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) to suppress the excitability of a portion of left prefrontal cortex and to assess its role in producing nouns and verbs. In one experiment subjects generated real words; in a second, they produced pseudowords as nouns or verbs. In both experiments, response latencies increased for verbs but were unaffected for nouns following rTMS. These results demonstrate that grammatical categories have a neuroanatomical basis and that the left prefrontal cortex is selectively engaged in processing verbs as grammatical objects.


Subject(s)
Frontal Lobe/physiology , Linguistics , Magnetics , Adolescent , Adult , Electric Stimulation , Female , Humans , Language Disorders/etiology , Language Disorders/physiopathology , Male
10.
Clin Neurophysiol ; 112(9): 1604-11, 2001 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11514242

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVE: To investigate the intracortical inhibitory and excitatory systems in the motor cortical representation of upper and lower facial muscles. METHODS: Paired-pulse transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) was applied to 7 healthy volunteers, with the interstimulus interval (ISI) between the conditioning stimulus (CS) and test stimulus, varied from 1 to 20 ms. CS was set at 90% of motor threshold. Muscle evoked potentials (MEPs) were recorded from first dorsal interosseus (FDI), orbicularis oculi (o. oculi) and mentalis muscles. RESULT: TMS evoked MEPs in o. oculi on both ipsi- and contralateral sides in all subjects. In the paired-pulse study, MEP amplitude in the mentalis decreased at short ISIs of 1-3 ms, followed by increases at 12-20 ms. These effects were similar to those in the FDI. O. oculi did not show a distinct inhibitory period at short ISIs and facilitation at long ISIs was detected but was significantly less than in FDI and mentalis. In o. oculi, there was no significant difference between the effects of ipsilateral and contralateral CS on the MEPs. CONCLUSION: The bi-hemispheric control of volitional movement and the modulation from brainstem projections appear to markedly influence intracortical inhibitory and excitatory systems in the motor cortical representation of o. oculi.


Subject(s)
Facial Muscles/physiology , Motor Cortex/physiology , Neural Inhibition/physiology , Adult , Electric Stimulation/methods , Evoked Potentials, Motor/physiology , Humans , Magnetics , Male , Reaction Time/physiology , Reference Values
11.
Neuroreport ; 12(7): 1489-92, 2001 May 25.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11388435

ABSTRACT

The effects of different phases of an observed movement on the modulation of cortical motor output were studied by means of transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS). A video-clip of a reaching-grasping action was shown and single TMS pulses were delivered during its passive observation. Times of cortical stimulation were related to the phases of the shown movement, locking them to the appearance of specific kinematic landmarks. The amplitude of the motor evoked potentials (MEPs) induced by TMS in the first dorsal interosseus (FDI) muscle was modulated by the amount of the observed finger aperture. The presence of such an effect is consistent with the notion of a mirror neuron system in premotor areas that couples action execution and action observation also in terms of temporal coding.


Subject(s)
Arm/physiology , Evoked Potentials, Motor/physiology , Hand Strength/physiology , Motor Cortex/physiology , Movement/physiology , Neurons/physiology , Acoustic Stimulation , Adult , Arm/innervation , Electric Stimulation , Humans , Magnetics , Neuropsychological Tests , Photic Stimulation , Psychomotor Performance/physiology
12.
Exp Brain Res ; 137(1): 36-44, 2001 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11310170

ABSTRACT

This kinematic study aimed to determine whether color is a stimulus property involved in the control of reaching-grasping movements. Subjects reached and grasped a target-object, located either on the right or on the left of the subject's midline. A distractor, placed along the subject's midline, could be randomly presented. The colors, i.e., both chromaticity (red and green stimuli were presented) and lightness, of the target and distractor were varied in experiment 1. Only stimulus lightness and only stimulus chromaticity were varied in experiments 2 and 3, respectively. In experiment 4 subjects matched with their thumb and index finger the size of the target-stimuli presented in experiment 1. Chromaticity (experiments 1 and 3) of the target and distractor influenced grasp, but not reach. Maximal finger aperture was larger during grasping the red than the green target. Data collected in the matching task (experiment 4) confirmed a trend to overestimate the red target and to underestimate the green one. During grasp, hand shaping was influenced by distractor chromaticity when it was different from target chromaticity. Distractor lightness affected reach, but not grasp (experiments 1 and 2). Reach was slower when the distractor was lighter and arm trajectory veered away from it. The results of the present study suggest that color, that is the ensemble of chromaticity and lightness, is a stimulus property involved in the control of reaching-grasping. The different effects of target color on reach and grasp support the notion that intrinsic object properties, such as color, affect grasp more than reach. In addition, the different effects of distractor chromaticity and lightness on reach and grasp confirm that target-objects are visually extracted from surrounding cues by means of different processes, according to the required motor response.


Subject(s)
Arm/physiology , Color Perception/physiology , Hand Strength/physiology , Movement/physiology , Orientation/physiology , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Space Perception/physiology , Adult , Arm/innervation , Biomechanical Phenomena , Female , Humans , Male , Neuropsychological Tests , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Photic Stimulation , Reaction Time/physiology
13.
Neuropsychologia ; 39(2): 132-9, 2001.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11163371

ABSTRACT

The aim of the present study was to determine whether children like adults (Gentilucci M, Chieffi S, Daprati E, Saetti MC, Toni I. Visual illusion and action. Neuropsychologia 1996;34:369-76; Gentilucci M, Daprati E, Gangitano M, Toni I. Eye position tunes the contribution of allocentric and egocentric information to target localisation in human goal directed arm movements. Neurosci Lett 1997;222:123-6) are influenced by visual illusions when they transform visual information in motor command. Children and adults pointed to a shaft extremity of the Müller-Lyer configurations, as well as to an extremity of a control configuration. Movements were executed in two experimental conditions. In the vision condition subjects saw both the stimulus and their hand before and during movement. In the no vision (memory) condition subjects saw the stimulus and their hand before, but not during movement. Movement started 5 s after vision was precluded. The Müller-Lyer illusion affected pointing kinematics of both children and adults. As found previously (Gentilucci M, Chieffi S, Daprati E, Saetti MC, Toni I. Visual illusion and action. Neuropsychologia 1996;34:369-76; Gentilucci M, Daprati E, Gangitano M, Toni I. Eye position tunes the contribution of allocentric and egocentric information to target localisation in human goal directed arm movements. Neurosci Lett 1997;222:123-6), subjects undershot and overshot the shaft extremity of the closed and of the open configuration, respectively. The illusion effect was greater in the no vision than in the vision condition. These results show that in children like in adults the system underlying visual perception in an object-centered frame of reference and that involved in motor control functionally interact with each other. Although the processes of target localisation were the same, the transformation of target position information in a sequence of motor patterns was different in children from that in adults. Even if both children and adults lengthened duration of the deceleration phase in the vision condition, only adults shortened duration of the acceleration phase in order to maintain constant movement time (Viviani P, Schneider R. A developmental study of the relationship between geometry and kinematics in drawing movements. J Exp Psychol 1991;17:198-218). This result suggests that children are yet unable to co-ordinate temporally acceleration with deceleration phase.


Subject(s)
Arm , Movement , Optical Illusions , Psychomotor Performance , Visual Perception , Acceleration , Adult , Analysis of Variance , Child , Female , Humans , Male , Psychophysics
14.
Exp Brain Res ; 133(4): 468-90, 2000 Aug.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10985682

ABSTRACT

We investigated the possible influence of automatic word reading on processes of visuo-motor transformation. Subjects reached and grasped an object on which the following Italian words were printed: "VICINO" (near) or "LONTAN" (far) on an object either near or far from the agent (experiments 1, 2); PICCOLO (small) or "GRANDE" (large) on either a small or a large object (experiment 4); and "ALTO" (high) or "BASSO" (low) on either a high or a low object (experiment 5). The kinematics of the initial phase of reaching-grasping was affected by the meaning of the printed words. Namely, subjects automatically associated the meaning of the word with the corresponding property of the object and activated a reach and/or a grasp motor program influenced by the word. No effect on initial reach kinematics was observed for words related to object properties not directly involved in reach control (experiment 3). Moreover, in all the experiments, the presented words poorly influenced perceptual judgement of object properties. In experiments 5-7, the effects of the Italian adjectives "ALTO" (high) and "BASSO" (low) on reaching-grasping control were compared with those of the Italian adverbs "SOPRA" (up) and "SOTTO" (down). Adjectives influenced visual analysis of target-object properties, whereas adverbs more directly influenced the control of the action. We suggest that these effects resemble the structure of a sentence, where adjectives are commonly referred to nouns, and adverbs to verbs. In other words, class of words and, in a broad sense, grammar influenced motor control. The results of the present study show that cognitive functions such as language can affect visuo-motor transformation. They are discussed according to the notion that a strict relation between language and motor control exists, and that the frontal cortex can be involved in interactions between automatic word reading and visuo-motor transformation.


Subject(s)
Hand Strength/physiology , Language , Movement/physiology , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Adolescent , Adult , Analysis of Variance , Biomechanical Phenomena , Female , Frontal Lobe/physiology , Humans , Male
15.
Neuropsychologia ; 38(10): 1398-404, 2000.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10869583

ABSTRACT

The kinematics of the action formed by reaching-grasping an object and placing it on a second target was studied in a patient who suffered from an acute vascular left brain lesion, which affected the Supplementary Motor Area proper (SMA-proper) (Matelli M, Luppino G. Thalamic input to mesial and superior area 6 in the macaque monkey. Journal of Comparative Neurology 1996;372:59-87, Matelli M, Luppino G, Fogassi L, Rizzolatti G. Thalamic input to inferior area 6 and area 4 in the macaque monkey. Journal of Comparative Neurology 1989;280:468-488), and in five healthy control subjects. The reach kinematics of the controls was affected by the positions of both the reaching-grasping and the placing targets (Gentilucci M, Negrotti A, Gangitano M. Planning an action. Experimental Brain Research 1997;115:116-28). In contrast, the reach kinematics of the patient was affected only by the position of the reaching-grasping target. By comparing these results with those previously found in Parkinson's disease patients executing the same action (Gentilucci M, Negrotti A. Planning and executing an action in Parkinson's disease patients. Movement Disorders 1999;1:69-79, Gentilucci M, Negrotti A. The control of an action in Parkinson's disease. Experimental Brain Research 1999;129:269-277), we suggest that the anatomical "motor" circuit formed by SMA-proper (see above), Basal Ganglia (BG) and Thalamus (Alexander GE, Crutcher MD. Functional architecture of basal ganglia circuits: neural substrates of parallel processing. Trends in the Neurosciences 1990;13:266-271, Hoover JE, Strick PL. Multiple output channels in the basal ganglia. Nature 1993;259:819-821) may be involved in the control of actions: SMA-proper assembles the sequence of the action, whereas BG updates its parameters and stores them.


Subject(s)
Ataxia/etiology , Frontal Lobe/physiopathology , Motor Cortex/physiopathology , Motor Skills , Stroke/complications , Acceleration , Adult , Arm , Female , Frontal Lobe/pathology , Functional Laterality , Hand Strength , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Male , Middle Aged , Motor Cortex/pathology , Reaction Time , Recovery of Function
16.
Brain Res Cogn Brain Res ; 9(2): 125-35, 2000 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10729696

ABSTRACT

The present study aimed to demonstrate that motor representations are used to recognise biological stimuli. In three experiments subjects were required to judge laterality of hands and forearms presented by pictures. The postures of the hands were those assumed when holding a small, medium and large sphere. In experiment 1, the sphere held in hand was presented, whereas in experiment 2 it was absent. In experiment 3, the same images, showing holding-a-sphere hands, as in experiment 1 were presented, but without forearm. In all experiments one finger of each hand could be absent. In experiment 1 recognition time was longer for those hand postures for which the corresponding grasping motor acts required more accuracy. This was confirmed by a control experiment (experiment 4), in which subjects actually grasped the spheres. Absence of fingers did not influence right-left hand recognition. However, the absence of target object in experiment 2, and of forearm in experiment 3 reduced the effects of the type of holding on hand laterality recognition. The results of the present study indicate that grasp representations are used to recognise hand laterality. In particular, the visual description of how hand and object interact in space (the opposition space [M.A. Arbib, Programs, schemas and neural networks for control of hand movement: beyond the RS frameworks, in: M. Jeannerod (Ed.), Attention and Performance XIII: Motor Representation and Control, Lawrence Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, 1990, 111-138; M.A. Arbib, T. Iberall, D. Lyons, Coordinated control programs for movements of the hand, in: A.W. Goodman, I. Darian-Smith (Eds.), Hand function and the neocortex, Springer, Berlin, 1985, pp. 135-170]) and the anchoring of the hand to the agent are the features of the grasp representations used in hand-recognition processes. The data are discussed according to the more general notion that motor representations are automatically extracted in the process of intuiting situations, or people's intentions. These motor representations, which are compared with those of other people, contain concrete information on the actions (the motor program) by which a situation is created and on the aim of the agents executing those actions.


Subject(s)
Form Perception/physiology , Functional Laterality/physiology , Hand Strength/physiology , Touch/physiology , Adult , Biomechanical Phenomena , Female , Fingers , Forearm , Humans , Male , Memory/physiology , Orientation/physiology , Photic Stimulation
17.
Exp Brain Res ; 122(4): 441-52, 1998 Oct.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9827863

ABSTRACT

In the present study we addressed the issue of how an object is visually isolated from surrounding cues when a reaching-grasping (prehension) movement towards it is planned. Subjects were required to reach and grasp an object presented either alone or with a distractor. In five experiments, different degrees of elaboration of the distractor were induced by varying: (1) the position of the distractor (central or peripheral); (2) the time when the distractor was suppressed (immediately or delayed, with respect to stimulus presentation); and (3) the type of distractor analysis (implicit or explicit). In addition, we tested whether the possible effects of the distractor on reaching-grasping were due to the use of an allocentric reference centered on it. This was obtained by comparing the effects of the distractor with those of a stimulus, the target of a placing movement successive to the reaching-grasping. The results of the five experiments can be summarized as follows. The necessary condition for an interference effect on both the reaching and the grasping components was the central presentation of the distractor. When the information on the distractor could be immediately suppressed, an interference effect was observed only on the grasp component. In the case of delayed suppression, an effect was found on the reaching component. Finally, when an overt analysis of the distractor was required, the interference effect disappeared. Two main conclusions have been drawn from the results of the present study. First, comparison between properties of the target and surrounding cues is performed by two independent processes for reaching and grasping an object. The process for the grasp relies more on allocentric cues than that for the reach. Second, when surrounding stimuli are automatically analyzed during visual search of the target, the process of visuo-motor transformation can incorporate their features into the target. In contrast, overt analysis of surrounding stimuli is performed separately from that of the target. Finally, the data of the present study are discussed in support of the premotor theory of attention.


Subject(s)
Arm/physiology , Hand Strength , Movement/physiology , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Visual Perception/physiology , Adult , Female , Humans , Male , Reaction Time/physiology , Time and Motion Studies
18.
Conscious Cogn ; 7(3): 478-93, 1998 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9787057

ABSTRACT

In the present study, we addressed the problem of whether hand representations, derived from the control of hand gesture, are used in handedness recognition. Pictures of hands and fingers, assuming either common or uncommon postures, were presented to right-handed subjects, who were required to judge their handedness. In agreement with previous results (Parsons, 1987, 1994; Gentilucci, Daprati, & Gangitano, 1998), subjects recognized handedness through mental movement of their own hand in order to match the posture of the presented hand. This was proved by a control experiment of physical matching. The new finding was that presentation of common finger postures affected responses differently from presentation of less common finger postures. These effects could be not attributed to mental matching movements nor related to richness in hand-finger cues useful for handedness recognition. The results of the present study are discussed in the context of the notion that implicit visual analysis of the presented hands is performed before mental movement of one's hand takes place (Parsons, 1987; Gentilucci et al., 1998). In this process, hand representation acquired by experience in the control and observation of one's and other people's hand gestures is used. We propose that such an immediate recognition mechanism belongs to the class of mental processes which are grouped under the name of intuition, that is, the processes by which situations or people's intentions are immediately understood, without conscious reasoning.


Subject(s)
Functional Laterality/physiology , Recognition, Psychology , Visual Perception , Adult , Cognition , Female , Gestures , Hand , Humans , Male
19.
Eur J Neurosci ; 10(2): 752-6, 1998 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9749737

ABSTRACT

We investigated the possible influence of automatic word reading on processes of visuo-motor transformation. Six subjects were required to reach and grasp a rod on whose visible face the word 'long' or 'short' was printed. Word reading was not explicitly required. In order to induce subjects to visually analyse the object trial by trial, object position and size were randomly varied during the experimental session. The kinematics of the reaching component was affected by word presentation. Peak acceleration, peak velocity, and peak deceleration of arm were higher for the word 'long' with respect to the word 'short'. That is, during the initial movement phase subjects automatically associated the meaning of the word with the distance to be covered and activated a motor program for a farther and/or nearer object position. During the final movement phase, subjects modified the braking forces (deceleration) in order to correct the initial error. No effect of the words on the grasp component was observed. These results suggest a possible influence of cognitive functions on motor control and seem to contrast with the notion that the analyses executed in the ventral and dorsal cortical visual streams are different and independent.


Subject(s)
Motor Skills/physiology , Reading , Acceleration , Adult , Female , Functional Laterality/physiology , Humans , Male
20.
Neuroreport ; 9(5): 887-91, 1998 Mar 30.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9579685

ABSTRACT

We used an interference paradigm in order to study integration between haptic and visual information in motor control and in perceptual analysis. Subjects either reached and grasped a visually presented sphere or matched its size with their left hand while manipulating with their right hand another sphere whose size could be smaller or greater. In four experiments haptic analysis of the manipulated sphere could be either automatically incorporated with or explicitly dissociated from visual analysis. In a fifth experiment reaching-grasping and matching were executed with the right hand, whereas manipulation was executed with the left hand. Manipulation with the right hand influenced finger shaping during grasping with the left hand when the sizes of the two objects were different. Interference was observed mainly in those experiments in which haptic analysis could be automatically integrated with visual analysis. In the matching task, no effect was observed. Finally, manipulation with the left hand did not produce any interference effect on reaching-grasping and matching executed by the right hand. The results of the present study suggest that somesthetic information is integrated with visual information only in sensorimotor transformations. In addition, they support the notion that the left hemisphere together with the right hemisphere is involved in the control of left hand reaching-grasping movements.


Subject(s)
Functional Laterality/physiology , Hand Strength/physiology , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Visual Perception/physiology , Adult , Female , Fingers/innervation , Fingers/physiology , Humans , Male , Photic Stimulation
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...